Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8bljj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T01:59:34.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Oversimplification in the Study of Emotional Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2013

Kelly A. Bennion
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts
Jaclyn H. Ford
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts
Brendan D. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts
Elizabeth A. Kensinger*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Elizabeth A. Kensinger, McGuinn Hall, Room 300, 140 Commonwealth Avenue, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467. E-mail: elizabeth.kensinger@bc.edu

Abstract

This Short Review critically evaluates three hypotheses about the effects of emotion on memory: First, emotion usually enhances memory. Second, when emotion does not enhance memory, this can be understood by the magnitude of physiological arousal elicited, with arousal benefiting memory to a point but then having a detrimental influence. Third, when emotion facilitates the processing of information, this also facilitates the retention of that same information. For each of these hypotheses, we summarize the evidence consistent with it, present counter-evidence suggesting boundary conditions for the effect, and discuss the implications for future research. (JINS, 2013, 19, 1–9)

Type
Short Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abercrombie, H.C., Kalin, N.H., Thurow, M.E., Rosenkranz, M.A., Davidson, R.J. (2003). Cortisol variation in humans affects memory for emotionally laden and neutral information. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 505516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumler, G., Lienert, G.A. (1993). Reevaluation of the Yerkes-Dodson law by nonparametric-tests of trend. Studia Psychologica, 35, 431436.Google Scholar
Bradley, M.M., Greenwald, M.K., Petry, M.C., Lang, P.J. (1992). Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 379390.Google ScholarPubMed
Brainerd, C.J., Stein, L.M., Silveira, R.A., Rohenkohl, G., Reyna, V.F. (2008). How does negative emotion cause false memories? Psychological Science, 19, 919925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R., Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, T. (2007). Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 761779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, T.W., Etzel, J.A., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. (2006). The influence of autonomic arousal and semantic relatedness on memory for emotional words. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 2633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, T.W., Lovallo, W.R. (2001). Enhanced memory for emotional material following stress-level cortisol treatment in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 307317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L., Gorski, L., Le, K. (2003). Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learning & Memory, 10, 270274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L., McGaugh, J.L. (1995). A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 410421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, H.Y., Kensinger, E.A., Rajaram, S. (2013). Emotional content enhances true but not false memory for categorized stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 41, 403415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-A. (1984). The relationship between induced emotional arousal and amnesia. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 25, 147160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christianson, S.-A. (1989). Flashbulb memories: Special, but not so special. Memory and Cognition, 17, 435443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-A. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 284309.Google ScholarPubMed
Chun, M.M., Turk-Browne, N.B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, M.A., Anderson, S.J., Larsen, S.F., Donnely, C.M., McDaniel, M.A., McClelland, A.G.R., Logie, R.H. (1994). The formation of flashbulb memories. Memory & Cognition, 22, 326343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Argembeau, A., Comblain, C., Van der Linden, M. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for positive, negative, and neutral events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 281294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolcos, F., Diaz-Granados, P., Wang, L., McCarthy, G. (2008). Opposing influences of emotional and non-emotional distracters upon sustained prefrontal cortex activity during a delayed-response working memory task. Neuropsychologia, 46, 326335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolcos, F., McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 20722079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Rimmele, U., Reichwald, U., Hautzinger, M. (2004). Acute stress impairs recognition for positive words – Association with stress-induced cortisol secretion. Stress, 7, 173181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougal, S., Rotello, C.M. (2007). “Remembering” emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 423429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easterbrook, J.A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Rey, J., Redondo, J. (2007). Recognition memory for pictorial stimuli: Biasing effects of stimulus emotionality. Psicothema, 19, 375380.Google ScholarPubMed
Gold, P.E., Van Buskirk, R.B. (1975). Facilitation of time-dependent memory processes with posttrial epinephrine injections. Behavioral Biology, 13, 145153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanoch, Y., Vitouch, O. (2004). When less is more: Information, emotional arousal, and the ecological reframing of the Yerkes-Dodson law. Theory & Psychology, 14, 427452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heuer, F., Reisberg, D. (1990). Vivid memories of emotional events: The accuracy of remembered minutiae. Memory & Cognition, 18, 496506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Johansson, M., Mecklinger, A., Treese, A.C. (2004). Recognition memory for emotional and neutral faces: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 18401853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kebeck, G., Lohaus, A. (1986). Effect of emotional arousal on free recall of complex material. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 63, 461462.Google Scholar
Kensinger, E.A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1, 99113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion, 3, 378393.Google ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 33103315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., Garoff-Eaton, R.J., Schacter, D.L. (2007). Effects of emotion on memory specificity: Memory trade-offs elicited by negative visually arousing stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 575591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kityama, S. (1990). Interaction between affect and cognition in word perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinsmith, L.J., Kaplan, S. (1963). Paired associates learning as a function of arousal and interpolated interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 190193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBar, K.S., Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 5464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libkuman, T.M., Nichols-Whitehead, P., Griffith, J., Thomas, R. (1999). Source of arousal and memory for detail. Memory & Cognition, 27, 166190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, R.B. (1967). Reinforcement. In G.C. Quarton, T. McInechuck, and F.O. Schmitt (Eds.), The neurosciences: A study program (pp. 568576). New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R., Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 5562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupien, S.J., Gaudreau, S., Tchiteya, B.M., Maheu, F., Sharma, S., Nair, N.P.V., Meaney, M.J. (1997). Stress-induced declarative memory impairment in healthy elderly subjects: Relationship to cortisol reactivity. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 82, 20702075.Google ScholarPubMed
Maratos, E.J., Allan, K., Rugg, M.D. (2000). Recognition memory for emotionally negative and neutral words: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 38, 14521465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratos, E.J., Rugg, M.D. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates of the retrieval of emotional and non-emotional context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 877891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., Mitchell, K.J., Raye, C.L., Novak, D.L., Green, E.J., Johnson, M.K. (2006). Emotional arousal can impair feature binding in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 614625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., Sutherland, M.R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 114133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M., Wible, C.G., Cohen, N.J. (1988). Is there a special flashbulb-memory mechanism? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 171181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mickley Steinmetz, K.R., Addis, D.R., Kensinger, E.A. (2010). The effect of arousal on the emotional memory network depends on valence. Neuroimage, 53, 318324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mickley Steinmetz, K.R., Kensinger, E.A. (2013). The emotion-induced memory trade-off: More than an effect of overt attention? Memory and Cognition, 41, 6981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, B.D., Kensinger, E.A. (2012). The effects of emotion and encoding strategy on associative memory. Memory and Cognition, 40, 10561069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, B.D., Kensinger, E.A. (in press). Age-related changes in associative memory for emotional and non-emotional integrative representations. Psychology and Aging.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, C.M., Solomon, L.Z., Florer, F., Thompson, T. (2004). Flashbulb memories of personal events of 9/11 and the day after for a sample of New York City residents. Psychological Reports, 95, 304310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, J.D., Jackson, E.D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W.J., Nadel, L. (2007). Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Learning and Memory, 14, 861868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E.A., Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisberg, D., Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion. Oxford: University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riggs, L., McQuiggan, D.A., Farb, N., Anderson, A., Ryan, J.D. (2011). The role of overt attention in emotion-modulated memory. Emotion, 11, 776785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimmele, U., Domes, G., Mathiak, K., Hautzinger, M. (2003). Cortisol has different effects on human memory for emotional and neutral stimuli. Neuroreport, 14, 24852488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchey, M., Dolcos, F., Cabeza, R. (2008). Role of amygdala connectivity in the persistence of emotional memories over time: An event-related fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 24942504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roozendaal, B., Williams, C.L., McGaugh, J.L. (1999). Glucocorticoid receptor activation in the rat nucleus of the solitary tract facilitates memory consolidation: Involvement of the basolateral amygdala. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 13171323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D.C., Kozin, M. (1984). Vivid memories. Cognition, 16, 6380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakaki, M., Nga, L., Mather, M. (2013). Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal cortex at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 12061224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, S.R. (2012a). Extraordinary memories for exceptional events. New York: Psychological Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S.R. (2012b). Memory for emotional words in sentences: The importance of emotional contrast. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 10151035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L., Bohringer, A., Chatterjee, M., Schachinger, H. (2008). Effects of pre-learning stress on memory for neutral, positive, and negative words: Different roles of cortisol and autonomic arousal. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90, 4453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L., Wolf, O.T. (2010). Learning under stress impairs memory formation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 93, 183188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharot, T., Phelps, E.A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 294306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, T., Giesbrecht, T., Jelicic, M., Merckelbach, H. (2007). Context-dependent enhancement of declarative memory performance following acute psychosocial stress. Biological Psychology, 76, 116123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, T., Wolf, O.T., Giesbrecht, T., Sijstermans, K., Telgen, S., Joëls, M. (2009). Stress selectively and lastingly promotes learning of context-related high arousing information. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 11521161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, M.R., Mather, M. (2012). Negative arousal amplifies the effects of saliency in short-term memory. Emotion, 12, 13671372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talarico, J.M., Rubin, D.C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talarico, J.M., Rubin, D.C. (2007). Flashbulb memories are special after all; In phenomenology, not accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 557578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmi, D., Anderson, A.K., Riggs, L., Caplan, J.B., Moscovitch, M. (2008). Immediate memory consequences of the effect of emotion on attention to pictures. Learning and Memory, 15, 172182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talmi, D., Luk, B.T.C., McGarry, L.M., Moscovitch, M. (2007). The contribution of relatedness and distinctiveness to emotionally-enhanced memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 555574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmi, D., Schimmack, U., Paterson, T., Moscovitch, M. (2007). The role of attention and relatedness in emotionally enhanced memory. Emotion, 7, 89102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vo, M.L.-H., Jacobs, A.R., Kuchinke, L., Hogmann, M., Conrad, M., Schacht, A., Hutzler, F. (2008). The coupling of emotion and cognition in the eye. Psychophysiology, 45, 130140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J.L., Driver, J., Dolan, R.J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M.P., Armony, J.L., Driver, J., Dolan, R.J. (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 12711278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waring, J.D., Kensinger, E.A. (2009). Emotional memory in Alzheimer's disease. In M.-K. Sun (Ed.), Research progress in Alzheimer's disease and dementia (Vol. 4, pp. 936). Hauppage, NY: Nova Publishers.Google Scholar
WeaverC.A., III C.A., III. (1993). Do you need a “flash” to form a flashbulb memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 3946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windmann, S., Kruger, T. (1998). Subconscious detection of threat as reflected by an enhanced response bias. Consciousness & Cognition, 7, 603633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windmann, S., Kutas, M. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates of emotion-induced recognition bias. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 577592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerkes, R.M., Dodson, J.D. (1908). The relation of strength stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, C.A., Kelley, C.M. (2010). “I'll remember this!” Effects of emotionality on memory predictions versus memory performance. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 240253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar