Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T06:34:00.274Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive Functions and Attention in Childhood Epilepsies: A Neuropsychological Hallmark of Dysfunction?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 November 2020

Elisa Cainelli*
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Jacopo Favaro
Affiliation:
Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Pietro De Carli
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Concetta Luisi
Affiliation:
Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy Department of Neuroscience, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Alessandra Simonelli
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Marilena Vecchi
Affiliation:
Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Stefano Sartori
Affiliation:
Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
Clementina Boniver
Affiliation:
Paediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padova, 35100Padova, Italy
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Elisa Cainelli, Ph.D., Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35133Padova, Italy. E-mail: elisa.cainelli@unipd.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Patients with epilepsy are at risk for several lifetime problems, in which neuropsychological impairments may represent an impacting factor. We evaluated the neuropsychological functions in children suffering from three main epilepsy categories. Further, we analyzed the longitudinal evolution of the neuropsychological profile over time.

Methods:

Patients undergoing neuropsychological evaluation at our Department from 2012 to 2018 were identified retrospectively. We selected patients aged 6–16 years and with at least two evaluations. Three epilepsy categories were considered: focal/structural, focal self-limited, and idiopathic generalized. Each evaluation included the same structured assessment of main neuropsychological domains. The effect of the epilepsy category, illness duration, seizure status, and medication was computed in multilevel models.

Results:

We identified 103 patients (focal self-limited = 27; focal/structural = 51; and idiopathic generalized = 25), for 233 evaluations. The majority of deficits were reported in attention and executive functions (>30% of patients); the results were dichotomized to obtain global indexes. Multilevel models showed a trend toward statistical significance of category of epilepsy on the global executive index and of illness duration on global attention index. Illness duration predicted the scores of executive and attention tasks, while category and medication predicted executive task performance. Focal/structural epilepsies mostly affected the executive domain, with deficits persisting over time. By contrast, an ameliorative effect of illness duration for attention was documented in all epilepsies.

Conclusions:

This study offers lacking information about the evolution of deficits in time, the role of epilepsy category, and possible psychological implications for high-order cognitive skills, central in several social and academic problems.

Type
Regular Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2020

INTRODUCTION

Patients with epilepsy are at risk for several problems, which can affect their life quality and social competences more than the seizures themselves.

Among these issues, cognitive and neuropsychological impairments represent a very impacting factor, particularly in those patients who show structural etiology (Rudzinski & Meador, Reference Rudzinski and Meador2013; Witt & Helmstaedter, Reference Witt and Helmstaedter2012). For example, the reported incidence of cognitive dysfunctions in children with focal cortical dysplasia ranges from 50% to 80% (Korman et al., Reference Korman, Krsek, Duchowny, Maton, Pacheco-Jacome and Rey2013; Krsek et al., Reference Krsek, Maton, Korman, Pacheco-Jacome, Jayakar, Dunoyer and Duchowny2008). Furthermore, other structural epilepsies, such as temporal lobe epilepsy, associated with mesiotemporal sclerosis, have been associated with high rates of cognitive impairments (Allone et al., Reference Allone, Lo Buono, Corallo, Pisani, Pollicino, Bramanti and Marino2017). Furthermore, recent findings have shown in pediatric temporal lobe epilepsy an association of executive functions with depression (Schraegle, Nussbaum, & Titus, Reference Schraegle, Nussbaum and Titus2018). However, impairment of overall cognitive function or isolated neuropsychological difficulties, such as visual perception, attention, and memory, have been reported even in self-limited focal epilepsies in childhood, with no MRI evidence of brain lesions, easily controlled seizures, and good prognosis (Deonna et al., Reference Deonna, Zesiger, Davidoff, Maeder, Mayor and Roulet2000; Pal et al., Reference Pal, Ferrie, Addis, Akiyama, Capovilla, Caraballo and Koutroumanidis2016). The risk of both pervasive and specific neuropsychological impairments is also reported in idiopathic generalized epilepsy (Henkin et al., Reference Henkin, Sadeh, Kivity, Shabtai, Kishon-Rabin and Gadoth2005; Loughman, Bowden, & D’Souza, Reference Loughman, Bowden and D’Souza2014). In fact, both self-limited focal epilepsies and idiopathic generalized epilepsies have a complex and polygenic genetic background that could be in a relationship and in common with cognitive impairment (Lesca et al., Reference Lesca, Rudolf, Labalme, Hirsch, Arzimanoglou, Genton and Szepetowski2012; Ratcliffe et al., Reference Ratcliffe, Wandschneider, Baxendale, Thompson, Koepp and Caciagli2020). Finally, the most severe condition is represented by the developmental and epileptic encephalopathies with neonatal to childhood-onset that are associated with drug-resistant epilepsy and significant cognitive impairment.

Among the possible neuropsychological disturbances, executive and attention problems are highly reported and they are a common feature among the different epilepsies types (Conant, Wilfong, Inglese, & Schwarte, Reference Conant, Wilfong, Inglese and Schwarte2010; Culhane-Shelburne, Chapieski, Hiscock, & Glaze, Reference Culhane-Shelburne, Chapieski, Hiscock and Glaze2002; D’Agati, Cerminara, Casarelli, Pitzianti, & Curatolo, Reference D’Agati, Cerminara, Casarelli, Pitzianti and Curatolo2012) (Witt & Helmstaedter, Reference Witt and Helmstaedter2012; for a review, see MacAllister, Vasserman, Rosenthal, & Sherman, Reference MacAllister, Vasserman, Rosenthal and Sherman2014). However, the cause of this association deserves further investigation. Executive functions represent a constellation of cognitive skills that drive goal-oriented behavior and are critical to the ability to adapt to an ever-changing world. Up to 50% of children with epilepsy demonstrate executive dysfunctions (Campiglia et al., Reference Campiglia, Seegmuller, Le Gall, Fournet, Roulin and Roy2014; Høie et al., Reference Høie, Sommerfelt, Waaler, Alsaker, Skeidsvoll and Mykletun2008; Parrish et al., Reference Parrish, Geary, Jones, Seth, Hermann and Seidenberg2007; Slick, Lautzenhiser, Sherman, & Eyrl, Reference Slick, Lautzenhiser, Sherman and Eyrl2006). Deficits can persist or worsen over time (Bailet & Turk, Reference Bailet and Turk2000; Masur et al., Reference Masur, Shinnar, Cnaan, Shinnar, Clark, Wang and Glauser2013; Piccinelli et al., Reference Piccinelli, Beghi, Borgatti, Ferri, Giordano, Romeo and Balottin2010) with impact on academic (Fastenau et al., Reference Fastenau, Shen, Dunn, Perkins, Hermann and Austin2004; Høie et al., Reference Høie, Sommerfelt, Waaler, Alsaker, Skeidsvoll and Mykletun2008) and social functioning (Nassau & Drotar, Reference Nassau and Drotar1997). In the same way, attention deficits have been found to have significant implications on children’s global functioning in the presence of epilepsy. Interestingly, it has shown that inattention is specifically implicated in academic underachievement (Seidenberg et al., Reference Seidenberg, Beck, Geisser, O’Leary, Giordani and Berent1988) and that attention appears to be the only variable that predicts academic performance in children with epilepsy among memory abilities, self-esteem, and socioeconomic status and after controlling for intelligence (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Phillips, Griebel, Sharp, Lange, Edgar and Simpson2001). ADHD is also overrepresented among children with epilepsy in comparison to the general population (Dunn & Kronenberger, Reference Dunn and Kronenberger2005); furthermore, epileptic patients with ADHD meet the usual criteria for inattentive-type ADHD (Dunn & Kronenberger, Reference Dunn and Kronenberger2005; Gascoigne et al., Reference Gascoigne, Smith, Barton, Webster, Gill and Lah2017).

With the aim to provide an overview of neuropsychological impairments in childhood epilepsies, with a particular focus on attention and executive functions, we evaluated the performances of children suffering from the three main categories of epilepsy, such as focal/structural epilepsies, focal self-limited epilepsies, and generalized epilepsies. Further, we analyzed the longitudinal evolution of the neuropsychological profile in these three groups of patients.

METHODS

Participant and Eligibility Criteria

We retrospectively identified all patients undergoing neuropsychological evaluation at the Pediatric Neurology and Neurophysiology Unit of the Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University Hospital of Padua (Italy) between September 2012 and February 2018. Among all the identified patients, we considered only those with complete and standardized evaluations. Subsequently, we selected those patients with at least two subsequent neuropsychological assessment. In order to increase the comparability between neuropsychological tests, we selected the patients aged 6 to 16 years.

Children were grouped based on the three following categories: focal/structural epilepsies, so-called focal self-limited, and idiopathic generalized epilepsies (Scheffer et al., Reference Scheffer, Berkovic, Capovilla, Connolly, French, Guilhoto and Zuberi2017). Patients with monogenic or genetic syndromes or with not well-defined etiology were excluded.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Clinical and Neurological Characteristics

We collected the following demographic data of the sample: gender, handedness, age at diagnosis, and comorbidities.

Patients underwent diagnostic procedures to define the type of epilepsy, etiology, and, when possible, epileptic syndrome, according to the 2017 Position Paper of ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology (Scheffer et al., Reference Scheffer, Berkovic, Capovilla, Connolly, French, Guilhoto and Zuberi2017).

The diagnostic process included clinical-anamnestic and neurological evaluations, Video-EEG during wakefulness and sleep, long-term EEG monitoring (in cases where routine EEG was not sufficient for classification), high-resolution cerebral MRI, and, when indicated, 18 F-FDG PET-MRI.

Focal epilepsies were defined as structural when clinical or electro-clinical semiology of the seizures was attributable to the structural alterations which were shown on MRI. In structural epilepsies, we also included cases in which, although there was no precise etiological diagnosis (negative MRI or aspecific findings), the electro-clinical data, the monomorphism of the seizures during evolution, the pharmacoresistance/pharmaco-dependence and, if available, PET-MRI data, oriented toward a structural etiology.

The definition of a specific syndrome is possible only considering the presence of a cluster of features incorporating seizure types, EEG data, neuroimaging, and comorbidities.

Data on seizure status and medication at each evaluation were collected and considered in the analysis. Seizure frequency was evaluated in the 3 months preceding neuropsychological assessment. It was defined as: daily (at least one seizure/day), weekly (at least one seizure/week), monthly (more than one seizure/month), sporadic (one seizure/month or less), as well as seizure-free patients. Medication has scored as follows: no medication, monotherapy, and politherapy. The presence of comorbidities has been evaluated and summarized as follows: no comorbidities, psychopathological comorbidities, and medical comorbidities.

Neuropsychological Assessment

For each evaluation, we collected the age of the child and the time elapsed from the diagnosis of epilepsy (expressed in months and defined as “illness duration”). The clinical setting in which neuropsychological assessments were performed is described in the following paragraph.

Children with a new diagnosis of epilepsy are sent to the neuropsychological service for an assessment. This evaluation is not always performed very close to the onset of epilepsy because ours is a tertiary referral hospital, and the diagnosis is often made in other centers. Furthermore, we want to avoid possible interfering effects associated with the diagnostic process. Subsequent evaluations are performed at least a year apart to avoid practice effects. Usually, children with a new onset of epilepsy underwent an initial evaluation and a follow-up; however, if there is a clinical indication, patients may perform multiple evaluations. The clinical indication may refer to trouble not only on cognitive function or academic problems but also on medical or factors associated with epilepsy without apparently cognitive counterpart. A structured neuropsychological evaluation characterizes each assessment carried out by a trained child neuropsychologist (E.C.). The following cognitive domains were assessed: abstract reasoning, using the Raven Colored Matrices (Raven, Raven, & Court, Reference Raven, Raven and Court1998); language, using the naming test and the semantic verbal fluency test (Bisiacchi, Cendron, Gugliotta, Tressoldi, & Vio, Reference Bisiacchi, Cendron, Gugliotta, Tressoldi and Vio2005); memory, using the digit span test and the Corsi block-tapping test, which evaluate short-term verbal and visual-spatial memory, the words list and list recall, which evaluate learning and long-term verbal memory (Bisiacchi et al., Reference Bisiacchi, Cendron, Gugliotta, Tressoldi and Vio2005), and the backward digit span test, which evaluates working memory (Bisiacchi et al., Reference Bisiacchi, Cendron, Gugliotta, Tressoldi and Vio2005); attention, using the Bells test (Stoppa & Biancardi, Reference Stoppa and Biancardi1997), which evaluates selective and sustained attention, and the Trial Making Test A (TMT A) (Scarpa et al., Reference Scarpa, Piazzini, Pesenti, Brovedani, Toraldo, Turner and Bottini2006), which evaluates scan and search speed; executive functions, using the phonemic verbal fluency test, which evaluates the ability to access the lexicon through a phonemic cue by setting up an adequate verbal search strategy (Bisiacchi et al., Reference Bisiacchi, Cendron, Gugliotta, Tressoldi and Vio2005); the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) (Scarpa et al., Reference Scarpa, Piazzini, Pesenti, Brovedani, Toraldo, Turner and Bottini2006), which evaluates frontal lobes functions; the Trial Making Test B (TMT B), which evaluates attention shifting (Scarpa et al., Reference Scarpa, Piazzini, Pesenti, Brovedani, Toraldo, Turner and Bottini2006); and visual-motor abilities, using the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Caffarra, Vezzadini, Dieci, Zonato, & Venneri, Reference Caffarra, Vezzadini, Dieci, Zonato and Venneri2002), which evaluates praxis and planning abilities.

Description, procedure, and references for all neuropsychological tasks used are reported in the Supplement Table A.

Statistical Analysis

Scores for the neuropsychological instruments were age-corrected and converted into z-scores, equivalent or standard scores, as appropriate, using published normative data. The z-scores indicate the deviation from the mean population score, which is set to 0, standard deviation 1. A z score of −2 (or less) comprises 2.5 % of the normal distribution and is considered to be significantly lower than average. Equivalent scores are a 5-point scale standardized after adjustment for age and education. An equivalent score of 0 is considered to be significantly lower than average, one a borderline score, and 2–5 average scores. Standard scores indicate the deviation from the mean population score, which is set to 10, standard deviation 3. A standard score of 4 (or less) is considered to be significantly lower than average.

We calculated a global index of dysfunctions for executive functions and attention from impairments of single functions: we classified this as a dysfunction if a patient obtained an impaired score on at least two tasks. This methodology provided dichotomous values, and it is useful in order to quantify a range of impairments in a unitary measure.

In order to test the linear effect of illness duration, the child’s age at each assessment was computed and used as a predictor (centered and considered as a fixed and random effect) in different multilevel models, in addition to the other target predictor (i.e., epilepsy category) and the control variable (i.e., the medication). Multilevel linear models were implied for phonemic fluency, TMT B, FAB, Bells accuracy, and rapidity. Multilevel logistic models were computed for attention and executive functioning global indexes. Since we were also interested in the moderation of the epilepsy category on the effect of illness duration, we tested two models for each dependent variable: one with the main effects and one with the main effects as well as the interaction effect of illness duration × diagnosis. Analyses were performed with R-software (R Core Team, 2020), using the lme4 package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) for multilevel modeling, the Car package (Fox & Weisberg, Reference Fox and Weisberg2011) to obtain Type II Wald chi-square tests. R 2 calculation is based on Nakagawa, Johnson, & Schielzeth (Reference Nakagawa, Johnson and Schielzeth2017). In order to interpret the significant interaction effect of diagnosis and illness duration, the library effects (Fox, Reference Fox2003) were used to represent the results visually, and simple slope analysis was performed utilizing the reghelper package (Hughes, Reference Hughes2017). Finally, we repeated the main analyses controlling the models also for the potentially confounding effect of the seizure status, and results are reported in the supplementary materials.

RESULTS

The flow chart with the procedure of selection of the final sample is reported in Figure 1.

Fig. 1. Flow chart with the procedure of selection of the final sample.

According to the above-mentioned eligibility criteria, we identified 103 patients (focal self-limited N = 27; focal/structural N = 51; generalized N = 25) and 233 neuropsychological evaluations. Of the self-limited epilepsy group, 19 patients (70.4%) had two assessments, 6 (22.2%) three assessments, and 2 (7.4%) four assessments. Of the focal/structural epilepsy group, 40 patients (78.4%) had two assessments, 10 (19.6%) three assessments, and 1 (2%) had four assessments. Of the generalized epilepsy group, 21 patients (84%) had two assessments, 4 (16%) three assessments, and none had four assessments.

The percentage of medication used, alone or combined in a polytherapy, respectively, at first and at last assessment, is carbamazepine (21.9%, 18.2%), valproic acid (35.4%, 40.9%), oxcarbazepine (8.3%, 8%), levetiracetam (15.6%, 14.8%), ethosuximide (9.4%, 6.8%), methylphenidate (0%, 1.1%), lacosamide (1%, 2.3%), topiramate (1%, 0%), lamotrigine (4.2%, 5.7%), clobazam (1%, 2.3%), clonazepam (1%, 0%), and dintoin (1%, 0%).

Table 1 reports the demographic data, comorbidities, temporal characteristics of the evaluations, seizure status, and medication at first and last evaluation considered for the three groups.

Table 1. For each examined epilepsy category, gender, handedness, comorbidities, age at onset, the time elapsed (in months) from the diagnosis to the first and last evaluation, the number of evaluations performed, and the age (in months) at the diagnosis, seizure status, and medication are reported

Within the focal self-limited epilepsy group, we were able to identify an epileptic syndrome in all the cases. Fourteen patients (51.8%) were classified as affected by childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes, 9 (33.3%) by Panayiotopoulos syndrome, 3 (11.1%) by childhood occipital epilepsy and only one patient (3.7%) was classified as affected by Landau–Kleffner syndrome.

Within the generalized epilepsy group, we were able to define an epileptic syndrome in 21 cases (84%). Eight patients (32%) were classified as affected by childhood absence epilepsy, 5 (20%) by Epilepsy with myoclonic, atonic seizures, and 4 (16%) by epilepsy with eyelid myoclonias. Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy, juvenile absence epilepsy, epilepsy with generalized tonic-clonic seizures alone and myoclonic epilepsy in infancy were represented by one patient each (4%)

In order to further characterize the group of patients affected by focal epilepsy of structural etiology, we specify in Table 2 the different types of lesions that we have identified. However, in 15 cases (29.4%), the MRI was normal, and in 6 cases (11.7%), the neuroimaging data revealed only aspecific structural abnormalities (asymmetry of temporo-mesial structures, or in the organization of sulci and gyri). These patients, as explained in the methods, were classified as focal/structural epilepsies according to a comprehensive evaluation of electro-clinical phenotype, and PET data if available. Among the 30 patients with a defined structural lesion, in 20 cases (67%), it was unilateral (55% of lesions localized in the left hemisphere, 45% of lesions in the right hemisphere). In 10 patients (33%), the lesion involved both the cerebral hemispheres. In 17 patients (57%), the extension of the structural abnormality was limited to one lobe; frontal lobes were involved in 30.3% of cases, temporal lobes in 39.3% of cases, parietal lobes in 24.2% of cases and occipital lobes in 6% of cases.

Table 2. Different types of lesions of focal/structural epilepsies

Within the focal/structural epilepsy group, seven patients (13.7%) were surgically treated.

The percentage of deficits and mean z-scores reported in neuropsychological tasks at the first and last assessment for each group of patients are reported in Tables 3 and 4.

Table 3. Percentage of deficits (scores < 2 SD) obtained by the three group of patients at first and last assessment in neuropsychological tasks

Table 4. Mean z-scores (± standard deviations) obtained by the three groups of patients at first and last assessment in neuropsychological tasks

Results of the multilevel models aimed at testing the effects of illness duration, epilepsy category, and medication on executive tasks are presented in Table 5.

Table 5. Results of the regression analyses on the global executive index and the relative subscales

p = .062; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

The multilevel logistic regression found no change in time for the global executive index as well as no effect of the medication. Still, a trend toward statistical significance (p = .06) has been shown for the effect of the epilepsy category. No interaction between illness duration and epilepsy category was found. On the contrary, as shown in Figure 2, Phonemic fluency increases over time and its score significantly decreases when the medication increases. Also, the epilepsy categories differ in terms of Phonemic fluency, suggesting that focal self-limited epilepsies are associated with lower fluency, while focal/structural epilepsies obtain higher scores but lower when compared with generalized epilepsies. No significant interaction between illness duration and epilepsy category was found.

Fig. 2. Main effects of illness duration, epilepsy category, and medication on phonemic fluency.

Neither main effects nor interaction effects were found for TMT B, while a significant diagnosis × time interaction has been found for FAB. Figure 3 shows the effects, and subsequent simple slope analyses show that time has a positive non-significant effect on the epilepsy category = focal self-limited (b = .41, SE = .30, t = 1.38, p > .05, Cohen’s d = .75), a non-significant positive effect on epilepsy category = focal/structural (b = .04, SE = .15, t = .30, p > .05, Cohen’s d = .17), and a significant negative effect on epilepsy category = generalized (b = −.58, SE = .23, t = −2.40, p < .05, Cohen’s d = −1.02). In other words, only the patients with generalized epilepsy show a change of FAB over time, independently of the medication used.

Fig. 3. Interaction effect of illness duration and epilepsy category on FAB.

The results of the multilevel models aimed at testing the effects of illness duration, epilepsy category, and medication on attention are presented in Table 6.

Table 6. Results of the regression analyses on the global attention index and the relative subscales

p = .070; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Regarding the global attention index, the multilevel logistic model shows no significant effect of medication and epilepsy category. Still, a trend toward statistical significance (p = .07) suggests that attention tends to increase over time. No interaction between illness duration and epilepsy category was found. Bells accuracy and rapidity are significantly increased over time, but neither main effects of medication of type of epilepsy nor interaction effects were found.

The main analyses were repeated, controlling for the potentially confounding effect of seizure status. Results are presented in Supplement tables (Tables B and C) and show that the associations remain unchanged in terms of effect sizes and statistical significance.

DISCUSSION

In the present study, we found a high percentage of executive and attention deficits in all three categories of examined patients, namely focal/structural epilepsies, so-called focal self-limited, and idiopathic generalized epilepsies.

Interestingly, by considering executive dysfunctions, patients with focal/structural epilepsies were the most affected independently of localization of the epileptogenic zone (the group contains several different localizations) and the deficit appeared to be persistent over time. By contrast, we did not find significant differences in frequency of attention impairments between the three groups of patients and an ameliorative effect of illness duration was documented in all the patients: at the last evaluation, attention abilities appeared to get better, independently of the epilepsy category.

Existing Literature

The finding of a high percentage of executive deficits in our cohort is consistent with the previous literature (Kavanaugh, Scarborough, & Salorio, Reference Kavanaugh, Scarborough and Salorio2015; Schraegle & Titus, Reference Schraegle and Titus2016). Impairments have been reported among a broad variety of seizure types and etiologies (Parrish et al., Reference Parrish, Geary, Jones, Seth, Hermann and Seidenberg2007; Hermann, Seidenberg, & Jones, Reference Hermann, Seidenberg and Jones2008) and have been documented even at the onset of epilepsy (Filippini et al., Reference Filippini, Ardu, Stefanelli, Boni, Gobbi and Benso2016; Parrish et al., Reference Parrish, Geary, Jones, Seth, Hermann and Seidenberg2007). However, empirical studies that directly compare cognitive functioning in children with different epileptic syndromes are very scarce. The majority of studies focused on risk factors, such as medication or seizure frequency, and often based on a retrospective evaluation of nationwide registries. However, some remarkable works have been conducted cross-syndrome comparisons. Fastenau et al. (Reference Fastenau, Johnson, Perkins, Byars, DeGrauw, Austin and Dunn2009) compared several types of epilepsy, both generalized and “localization-related,” with controls sibling. They found that children with symptomatic/cryptogenic etiology performed worse than those with idiopathic etiology on verbal memory and learning. For attention/executive/construction, both localization-related cryptogenic and generalized idiopathic absence groups scored lower than siblings. Another remarkable study is that of Hermann et al. (Reference Hermann, Seidenberg and Jones2008), who searched for cognitive phenotypes in children with new-onset focal and generalized idiopathic epilepsies. Confirmatory factor analysis identified five cognitive factors (verbal, perceptual, speed, attention, and executive), and latent class analysis identified three clusters of participants with epilepsy: (1) average and similar to controls, (2) mild impairment across multiple cognitive domains, and (3) impairment across all domains with severe attentional impairment. A subsequent effort of the same group investigated the neuropsychological profile of children who have new-/recent-onset idiopathic generalized epilepsy and idiopathic localization-related (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Dabbs, Walker, Jones, Hsu, Stafstrom and Hermann2013). They found considerable cognitive abnormality at baseline, including patterns of shared abnormalities across syndromes (e.g., psychomotor slowing) as well as unique syndrome-specific cognitive effects (e.g., executive function in idiopathic generalized epilepsy and language/verbal memory in idiopathic localization-related epilepsy). Academic difficulties are evident in approximately 50% of the children with epilepsy, affecting all syndrome groups to an equal degree. Lopes and colleagues examined the IQ (Lopes et al., Reference Lopes, Simões, Monteiro, Fonseca, Martins, Ventosa and Robalo2013) and memory abilities (Lopes, Monteiro, Fonseca, Robalo, & Simões, Reference Lopes, Monteiro, Fonseca, Robalo and Simões2014) in three common epilepsy syndromes (frontal lobe epilepsy, childhood absence epilepsy, and benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes). They showed that frontal lobe epilepsy has lower intelligence and memory scores. Also, they showed that type of epilepsy and duration of epilepsy were the best indicators of intellectual functioning and memory problems. Literature reported other interesting examples of syndromes comparisons (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yan, Gao, Xu, Zhou and Chen2017; Culhane-Shelburne et al., Reference Culhane-Shelburne, Chapieski, Hiscock and Glaze2002; Law, Smith, & Widjaja, Reference Law, Smith and Widjaja2018; Nolan et al., Reference Nolan, Redoblado, Lah, Sabaz, Lawson, Cunningham and Bye2004; Riccio, Pliego, Cohen, & Park, Reference Riccio, Pliego, Cohen and Park2015).

Discussion of Our Results on Executive Functions

Although executive problems were overrepresented in each group, our study showed that executive functions were strongly and pervasively affected in focal/structural epilepsies. This result is particularly interesting if we consider that this group comprises patients with different structural abnormalities and localizations of the seizure focus. Executive dysfunctions were originally described on the basis of patterns of deficits observed in patients with frontal lobe lesions (Stuss & Benson, Reference Stuss and Benson1984; Stuss, Reference Stuss2011; Rabinovici, Stephens, & Possin, Reference Rabinovici, Stephens and Possin2015). Our observations are coherent with recent acquisitions, according to whose executive function depends on distributed neural networks that not only involve the prefrontal cortex prominently but also include parietal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum, and white matter (Cainelli, Mioni, Boniver, Bisiacchi, & Vecchi, Reference Cainelli, Mioni, Boniver, Bisiacchi and Vecchi2019; Cainelli, Arrigoni, & Vedovelli, Reference Cainelli, Arrigoni and Vedovelli2020; Collette et al., Reference Collette, Van Der Linden, Laureys, Delfiore, Degueldre, Luxen and Salmon2005; Monchi et al., Reference Monchi, Petrides, Strafella, Worsley and Doyon2006; Rabinovici et al., Reference Rabinovici, Stephens and Possin2015). It has also been shown in other pathological conditions (Cainelli, Nosadini, Sartori, & Suppiej, Reference Cainelli, Nosadini, Sartori and Suppiej2019). Even if specific effects on executive function may be more likely with seizures originating from the frontal lobe, focal seizures arising from other lobes may result in secondary impairments (Guimarães et al., Reference Guimarães, Li, Rzezak, Fuentes, Franzon, Augusta Montenegro and Guerreiro2007; Rzezak et al., Reference Rzezak, Fuentes, Guimarães, Thome-Souza, Kuczynski, Li and Valente2007), determining interferences in the distributed neural networks underlying executive functions (Pereira et al., Reference Pereira, Alessio, Sercheli, Pedro, Bilevicius, Rondina and Cendes2010; Tracy et al., Reference Tracy, Osipowicz, Spechler, Sharan, Skidmore, Doucet and Sperling2014; Vlooswijk et al., Reference Vlooswijk, Vaessen, Jansen, De Krom, Majoie, Hofman and Backes2011). Interestingly, numerous functional imaging studies in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy have demonstrated altered connectivity in the default mode networks as a probable consequence of disrupted networks in the frontal lobe (Liao et al., Reference Liao, Zhang, Pan, Mantini, Ding, Duan and Chen2010; Lin, Riley, Juranek, & Cramer, Reference Lin, Riley, Juranek and Cramer2008; Riederer et al., Reference Riederer, Lanzenberger, Kaya, Prayer, Serles and Baumgartner2008; Vlooswijk et al., Reference Vlooswijk, Vaessen, Jansen, De Krom, Majoie, Hofman and Backes2011; Waites et al., Reference Waites, Briellmann, Saling, Abbott and Jackson2006).

In our patients with focal/structural epilepsies, executive function impairments were more frequent than in patients affected by focal and generalized epilepsies without structural damage. This suggests that the presence of a structural alteration may determine a more pervasive and persistent disruption of diffuse cerebral networks compared to other types of non-lesional epilepsies, including the so-called system epilepsies (Avanzini et al., Reference Avanzini, Manganotti, Meletti, Moshé, Panzica, Wolf and Capovilla2012). Interestingly, Rzezak and colleagues already showed that symptomatic epilepsy might determine worse executive performances compared to the so-called cryptogenic epilepsy (Rzezak et al., Reference Rzezak, Fuentes, Guimarães, Thome-Souza, Kuczynski, Li and Valente2007).

Discussion of Our Results on Attention

Attention involves several different components and partially overlap with executive functions; previous studies showed that children with epilepsy exhibit a wide range of attention difficulties (D’Alessandro et al., Reference D’Alessandro, Piccirilli, Tiacci, Ibba, Maiotti, Sciarma and Testa1990; Gascoigne et al., Reference Gascoigne, Smith, Barton, Webster, Gill and Lah2017; Semrud-Clikeman & Wical, Reference Semrud-Clikeman and Wical1999) and a high percentage of inattentive-type ADHD (Dunn & Kronenberger, Reference Dunn and Kronenberger2005; Gascoigne et al., Reference Gascoigne, Smith, Barton, Webster, Gill and Lah2017). However, a cross-sectional design (e.g., Cnaan et al., Reference Cnaan, Shinnar, Arya, Adamson, Clark, Dlugos, Hirtz, Masur and Glauser2017; D’Alessandro et al., Reference D’Alessandro, Piccirilli, Tiacci, Ibba, Maiotti, Sciarma and Testa1990; Deonna et al., Reference Deonna, Zesiger, Davidoff, Maeder, Mayor and Roulet2000; Fonseca Wald et al., Reference Fonseca Wald, Klinkenberg, Voncken, Ebus, Aldenkamp, Vles and Debeij-Van Hall2019; Masur et al., Reference Masur, Shinnar, Cnaan, Shinnar, Clark, Wang and Glauser2013; Shinnar et al., Reference Shinnar, Shinnar, Cnaan, Clark, Dlugos, Hirtz and Glauser2017), short follow-up periods and a lack of proper age bias correction (Cnaan et al., Reference Cnaan, Shinnar, Arya, Adamson, Clark, Dlugos, Hirtz, Masur and Glauser2017; Glauser et al., Reference Glauser, Cnaan, Shinnar, Hirtz, Dlugos, Masur and Adamson2010; Masur et al., Reference Masur, Shinnar, Cnaan, Shinnar, Clark, Wang and Glauser2013; Shinnar et al., Reference Shinnar, Shinnar, Cnaan, Clark, Dlugos, Hirtz and Glauser2017) are significant limitations of these studies, making further confirmation necessary. In this regard, our paper based on well-defined eligibility criteria, comparable protocols, and an extended follow-up - confirmed this observation, showing a high percentage of attention deficits without significant differences in frequency between the three groups of examined patients in our cohort. Moreover, an ameliorative effect of time was documented in all the patients independent of the epilepsy category after a consistent longitudinal follow-up, similar to what was previously observed by Fonseca Wald et al. (Reference Fonseca Wald, Klinkenberg, Voncken, Ebus, Aldenkamp, Vles and Debeij-Van Hall2019) in children with absent epilepsy after a follow-up time of more than 12 months, in both children with and without seizure freedom.

The link between attention function and epilepsy is complex; performance in attention skills may be affected by different factors, mainly by neurologic factors, such as ictal and interictal epileptiform activity (Marston, Besag, Binnie, & Fowler, Reference Marston, Besag, Binnie and Fowler1993) and antiepileptic drugs (Loring & Meador, Reference Loring and Meador2004; Schmitz, Reference Schmitz2006). Interestingly, the favorable course of attention displayed in our work is mainly due to the fact that the trend has resulted in being independent of the antiepileptic treatment. Conversely, the potential link between other clinical, neurophysiologic, environmental, and psychological variables has not been explored in our work due to the size of the sample, representing a limitation in our study.

The evolution in time of the attention performance is intriguing and not easy to interpret. The phenomenon is a cross-disease characteristic and did not appear to associate with other clinical variables, such as medication and seizure status. Therefore, we are tempted to interpret it as a psychological factor. Attention is strongly associated with psychological functioning and, in particular to anxiety and depression. It is reasonable to suppose that psychological factors might worsen the attention performance close to diagnosis time. This influence could wane over time, contributing at least in part to the improvement course of attention disturbances (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007; Grillon, Robinson, Mathur, & Ernst, Reference Grillon, Robinson, Mathur and Ernst2016). At a confirmation, it has shown that in children with new/recent-onset idiopathic generalized and with localization-related epilepsies, behavioral problems are present near the time of diagnosis and tend to abate over time (Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Rathouz, Jones, Jackson, Hsu, Stafstrom and Hermann2015). Clinicians working with psychiatric patients (in particular with anxiety disorders and depression) refer to the concept of “cognitive bias,” a wide range of patterns of dysfunctions – such as attentional bias –due to psychological disturbances. The first evaluation in our Department is not usually performed very close to the onset of epilepsy in order to avoid the effects of the distress due to the new diagnosis. The stigma that, unfortunately, still characterizes this condition may cause patients and their families a long period of distress with a negative impact on the initial neuropsychological presentation. In particular, the parental depression and anxiety associated with these levels of stress could initiate a dysfunctional cycle where dysfunctional parental involvement worsens the already challenged developmental trajectory of the child (Cottrell & Khan, Reference Cottrell and Khan2005; De Carli, Riem Madelon, & Parolin, Reference De Carli, Riem Madelon and Parolin2017; Sacchi et al., Reference Sacchi, De Carli, Vieno, Piallini, Zoia and Simonelli2018).

Conclusion

In conclusion, our results confirmed the high percentage of deficits in executive abilities and attention in epileptic populations found in previous studies. Patients with focal/structural epilepsies were mostly affected in the executive domain, independent of localization of the seizure focus, and the deficits persisted over time. By contrast, we did not find significant differences between the three groups of patients in the attention domain and an ameliorative effect of illness duration was documented in all patients. Even if similar problems have already been found in previous literature, few studies in the literature compare different epilepsy syndromes; therefore, despite the several limitations of this study, we add new information about the role of etiology and the evolution of deficits in time. Furthermore, we suggested a possible influence of psychological factors, as shown in other social and academic problems. Executive functions and attention have been shown to play a pivotal role in supporting other neuropsychological functions that may be impaired among patients with pediatric epilepsy (Black et al., Reference Black, Shih, Sepeta, Facella-Ervolini, Isquith and Berl2019). They are strong predictors of adaptive functioning within this population (Culhane-Shelburne et al., Reference Culhane-Shelburne, Chapieski, Hiscock and Glaze2002). Weak executive skills and attention also predict psychiatric comorbidity and behavioral problems among children with epilepsy (Alfstad et al., Reference Alfstad, Torgersen, Van Roy, Hessen, Hansen, Henning and Lossius2016; Baum et al., Reference Baum, Byars, deGrauw, Dunn, Bates, Howe and Austin2010) and have consistently been associated with decreased quality of life among pediatric patients (Love et al., Reference Love, Webbe, Kim, Lee, Westerveld and Salinas2016; Schraegle & Titus, Reference Schraegle and Titus2016; Sherman, Slick, & Eyrl, Reference Sherman, Slick and Eyrl2006). Interestingly, it has shown that training based on executive functions may improve the quality of life of children with epilepsy (Schraegle & Titus, Reference Schraegle and Titus2016). Thus, they remain an essential target for evaluation and the implementation of prompt rehabilitative intervention. Future research might explore the efficacy of an integrative intervention program, which takes care of both the cognitive and the psychological aspects, and their synergistic action.

Acknowledgments

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have nothing to disclose.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720001125

References

Alfstad, K., Torgersen, H., Van Roy, B., Hessen, E., Hansen, B.H., Henning, O., … Lossius, M.I. (2016). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and youth with epilepsy: An association with executive dysfunction? Epilepsy and Behavior, 56, 8894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.01.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allone, C., Lo Buono, V., Corallo, F., Pisani, L.R., Pollicino, P., Bramanti, P., & Marino, S. (2017). Neuroimaging and cognitive functions in temporal lobe epilepsy: A review of the literature. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 381, 715). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2017.08.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avanzini, G., Manganotti, P., Meletti, S., Moshé, S.L., Panzica, F., Wolf, P., & Capovilla, G. (2012). The system epilepsies: A pathophysiological hypothesis. Epilepsia, 53(5), 771778. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528–1167.2012.03462.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailet, L.L. & Turk, W.R. (2000). The impact of childhood epilepsy on neurocognitive and behavioral performance: A prospective longitudinal study. Epilepsia, 41(4), 426431. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528–1157.2000.tb00184.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B.M., & Walker, S.C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, K.T., Byars, A.W., deGrauw, T.J., Dunn, D.W., Bates, J.E., Howe, S.R., … & Austin, J.K. (2010). The effect of temperament and neuropsychological functioning on behavior problems in children with new-onset seizures. Epilepsy and Behavior, 17(4), 467473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bisiacchi, P., Cendron, M., Gugliotta, M., Tressoldi, P., & Vio, C. (2005). BVN. Batteria di Valutazione Neuropsicologica per l’Età Evolutiva. Erickson.Google Scholar
Black, C.L., Shih, S.W., Sepeta, L.N., Facella-Ervolini, J.M., Isquith, P.K., & Berl, M.M. (2019). Everyday executive function in focal onset pediatric epilepsy on the parent-report BRIEF2. Child Neuropsychology, 25(1), 2243. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2018.1424326 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A. (2002). Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: Normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurological Sciences, 22(6), 443447. https://doi.org/10.1007/s100720200003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cainelli, E., Arrigoni, F., & Vedovelli, L. (2020). White matter injury and neurodevelopmental disabilities: A cross-disease (dis)connection. Progress in Neurobiology, 193(June), 101845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101845 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cainelli, E., Mioni, G., Boniver, C., Bisiacchi, P.S., & Vecchi, M. (2019). Time perception in childhood absence epilepsy: Findings from a pilot study. Epilepsy and Behavior, 99, 106460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.106460 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cainelli, E., Nosadini, M., Sartori, S., & Suppiej, A. (2019). Neuropsychological and psychopathological profile of anti-NMDAR encephalitis: A possible pathophysiological model for pediatric neuropsychiatric disorders. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 34(8), 13091319. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acy088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campiglia, M., Seegmuller, C., Le Gall, D., Fournet, N., Roulin, J.L., & Roy, A. (2014). Assessment of everyday executive functioning in children with frontal or temporal epilepsies. Epilepsy and Behavior, 39, 1220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.07.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, D., Yan, X., Gao, Z., Xu, K., Zhou, X., & Chen, Q. (2017). Common and distinctive patterns of cognitive dysfunction in children with benign epilepsy syndromes. Pediatric Neurology, 72, 3641.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.12.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cnaan, A., Shinnar, S., Arya, R., Adamson, P.C., Clark, P.O., Dlugos, D., Hirtz, D.G., Masur, D., & Glauser, T.A. (2017). Second monotherapy in childhood absence epilepsy. Neurology, 88(2), 182190. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003480 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collette, F., Van Der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 409423. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20118 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conant, L.L., Wilfong, A., Inglese, C., & Schwarte, A. (2010). Dysfunction of executive and related processes in childhood absence epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 18(4), 414423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.05.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cottrell, L. & Khan, A. (2005). Impact of childhood epilepsy on maternal sleep and socioemotional functioning. Clinical Pediatrics, 44(7), 613616. https://doi.org/10.1177/000992280504400709 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culhane-Shelburne, K., Chapieski, L., Hiscock, M., & Glaze, D. (2002). Executive functions in children with frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(5), 623632. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702801308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Agati, E., Cerminara, C., Casarelli, L., Pitzianti, M., & Curatolo, P. (2012). Attention and executive functions profile in childhood absence epilepsy. Brain and Development, 34(10), 812817. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2012.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Alessandro, P., Piccirilli, M., Tiacci, C., Ibba, A., Maiotti, M., Sciarma, T., & Testa, A. (1990). Neuropsychological features of benign partial epilepsy in children. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 11(3), 265269. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2117599 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Carli, P., Riem Madelon, M.E., & Parolin, L. (2017). Approach-avoidance responses to infant facial expressions in nulliparous women: Associations with early experience and mood induction. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 104113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.08.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deonna, T., Zesiger, P., Davidoff, V., Maeder, M., Mayor, C., & Roulet, E. (2000). Benign partial epilepsy of childhood: A longitudinal neuropsychological and EEG study of cognitive function. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42(9), 595603. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162200001122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, D.W. & Kronenberger, W.G. (2005). Childhood epilepsy, attention problems, and ADHD: Review and practical considerations. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 12(4), 222228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spen.2005.12.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M.W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M.G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528–3542.7.2.336 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fastenau, P.S., Johnson, C.S., Perkins, S.M., Byars, A.W., DeGrauw, T.J., Austin, J.K., & Dunn, D.W. (2009). Neuropsychological status at seizure onset in children: Risk factors for early cognitive deficits. Neurology, 73(7), 526534. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b23551 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fastenau, P.S., Shen, J., Dunn, D.W., Perkins, S.M., Hermann, B.P., & Austin, J.K. (2004). Neuropsychological predictors of academic underachievement in pediatric epilepsy: Moderating roles of demographic, seizure, and psychosocial variables. Epilepsia, 45(10), 12611272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0013–9580.2004.15204.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filippini, M., Ardu, E., Stefanelli, S., Boni, A., Gobbi, G., & Benso, F. (2016). Neuropsychological profile in new-onset benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS): Focusing on executive functions. Epilepsy and Behavior, 54, 7179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.11.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonseca Wald, E.L.A., Klinkenberg, S., Voncken, T.P.C., Ebus, S.C.M., Aldenkamp, A.P., Vles, J.S.H., … Debeij-Van Hall, M.H.J.A. (2019). Cognitive development in absence epilepsy during long-term follow-up. Child Neuropsychology, 25(8), 10031021. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2019.1614156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 127. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v008.i15 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, J. & Weisberg, S. (2011). Package ‘car’. Companion to Applied Regression, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage (Issue September 2012). Sage Publication. http://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion Google Scholar
Gascoigne, M.B., Smith, M.L., Barton, B., Webster, R., Gill, D., & Lah, S. (2017). Attention deficits in children with epilepsy: Preliminary findings. Epilepsy and Behavior, 67, 712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.11.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glauser, T.A., Cnaan, A., Shinnar, S., Hirtz, D.G., Dlugos, D., Masur, D., … Adamson, P.C. (2010). Ethosuximide, valproic acid, and lamotrigine in childhood absence epilepsy. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(9), 790799. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0902014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grillon, C., Robinson, O.J., Mathur, A., & Ernst, M. (2016). Effect of attention control on sustained attention during induced anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 700712. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1024614 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guimarães, C.A., Li, L.M., Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Franzon, R.C., Augusta Montenegro, M., … Guerreiro, M.M. (2007). Temporal lobe epilepsy in childhood: Comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. Journal of Child Neurology, 22(7), 836840. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073807304701 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henkin, Y., Sadeh, M., Kivity, S., Shabtai, E., Kishon-Rabin, L., & Gadoth, N. (2005). Cognitive function in idiopathic generalized epilepsy of childhood. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47(2), 126132. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162205000228 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., & Jones, J. (2008). The neurobehavioural comorbidities of epilepsy: can a natural history be developed? The Lancet Neurology, 7(2), 151160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474–4422(08)70018–8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Høie, B., Sommerfelt, K., Waaler, P.E., Alsaker, F.D., Skeidsvoll, H., & Mykletun, A. (2008). The combined burden of cognitive, executive function, and psychosocial problems in children with epilepsy: A population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50(7), 530536. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469–8749.2008.03015.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, J. (2017). reghelper: Helper Functions for Regression Analysis (p. 59). https://cran.r-project.org/package=reghelper Google Scholar
Jackson, D.C., Dabbs, K., Walker, N.M., Jones, J.E., Hsu, D.A., Stafstrom, C.E., … Hermann, B.P. (2013). The neuropsychological and academic substrate of new/recent-onset epilepsies. Journal of Pediatrics, 162(5). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.10.046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanaugh, B.C., Scarborough, V.R., & Salorio, C.F. (2015). Parent-rated emotional-behavioral and executive functioning in childhood epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 42, 2228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.11.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korman, B., Krsek, P., Duchowny, M., Maton, B., Pacheco-Jacome, E., & Rey, G. (2013). Early seizure onset and dysplastic lesion extent independently disrupt cognitive networks. Neurology, 81(8), 745751. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a1aa2a CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krsek, P., Maton, B., Korman, B., Pacheco-Jacome, E., Jayakar, P., Dunoyer, C., … Duchowny, M. (2008). Different features of histopathological subtypes of pediatric focal cortical dysplasia. Annals of Neurology, 63(6), 758769. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21398 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, N., Smith, M.L., & Widjaja, E. (2018). Thalamocortical connections and executive function in pediatric temporal and frontal lobe epilepsy. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 39(8), 15301535. https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A5691 Google ScholarPubMed
Lesca, G., Rudolf, G., Labalme, A., Hirsch, E., Arzimanoglou, A., Genton, P., … Szepetowski, P. (2012). Epileptic encephalopathies of the Landau-Kleffner and continuous spike and waves during slow-wave sleep types: Genomic dissection makes the link with autism. Epilepsia, 53(9), 15261538. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528–1167.2012.03559.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, W., Zhang, Z., Pan, Z., Mantini, D., Ding, J., Duan, X., … Chen, H. (2010). Altered functional connectivity and small-world in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. PLoS ONE, 5(1), e8525. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008525 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, J.J., Riley, J.D., Juranek, J., & Cramer, S.C. (2008). Vulnerability of the frontal-temporal connections in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 82(2–3), 162170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2008.07.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, A, Monteiro, J., Fonseca, M., Robalo, C., & Simões, M. (2014). Memory functioning in children with epilepsy: frontal lobe epilepsy, childhood absence epilepsy, and benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Behavioural Neurology, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/218637 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, AF, Simões, M., Monteiro, J., Fonseca, M., Martins, C., Ventosa, L., … Robalo, C. (2013). Intellectual functioning in children with epilepsy: Frontal lobe epilepsy, childhood absence epilepsy and benign epilepsy with centro-temporal spikes. Seizure, 22(10), 886892. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2013.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loring, D.W. & Meador, K.J. (2004). Cognitive side effects of antiepileptic drugs in children. Neurology, 62(6), 872877. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000115653.82763.07 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughman, A., Bowden, S.C., & D’Souza, W. (2014). Cognitive functioning in idiopathic generalised epilepsies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 2034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Love, C.E., Webbe, F., Kim, G., Lee, K.H., Westerveld, M., & Salinas, C.M. (2016). The role of executive functioning in quality of life in pediatric intractable epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 64, 3743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.08.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacAllister, W.S., Vasserman, M., Rosenthal, J., & Sherman, E. (2014). Attention and executive functions in children with epilepsy: What, why, and what to do. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3(3), 215225. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.839605 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marston, D., Besag, F., Binnie, C.D., & Fowler, M. (1993). Effects of transitory cognitive impairment on psychosocial functioning of children with epilepsy: a therapeutic trial. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35(7), 574581. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469–8749.1993.tb11694.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, D., Shinnar, S., Cnaan, A., Shinnar, R.C., Clark, P., Wang, J., … Glauser, T.A. (2013). Pretreatment cognitive deficits and treatment effects on attention in childhood absence epilepsy. Neurology, 81(18), 15721580. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a9f3ca CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Strafella, A.P., Worsley, K.J., & Doyon, J. (2006). Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. Annals of Neurology, 59(2), 257264. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20742 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, S., Johnson, P.C.D., & Schielzeth, H. (2017). The coefficient of determination R2 and intra-class correlation coefficient from generalized linear mixed-effects models revisited and expanded. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 14(134), 20170213. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nassau, J.H. & Drotar, D. (1997). Social competence among children with central nervous system-related chronic health conditions: A review. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22(6), 771793. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/22.6.771 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, M.A., Redoblado, M.A., Lah, S., Sabaz, M., Lawson, J.A., Cunningham, A.M., … Bye, A.M.E. (2004). Memory function in childhood epilepsy syndromes. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 40(1–2), 2027. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440–1754.2004.00284.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pal, D.K., Ferrie, C., Addis, L., Akiyama, T., Capovilla, G., Caraballo, R., … Koutroumanidis, M. (2016). Idiopathic focal epilepsies: The lost tribe. Epileptic Disorders, 18(3), 252288. https://doi.org/10.1684/epd.2016.0839 Google ScholarPubMed
Parrish, J., Geary, E., Jones, J., Seth, R., Hermann, B., & Seidenberg, M. (2007). Executive functioning in childhood epilepsy: Parent-report and cognitive assessment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(6), 412416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469–8749.2007.00412.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, F.R.S., Alessio, A., Sercheli, M.S., Pedro, T., Bilevicius, E., Rondina, J.M., … & Cendes, F. (2010). Asymmetrical hippocampal connectivity in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: Evidence from resting state fMRI. BMC Neuroscience, 11(1), 66. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471–2202–11–66 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccinelli, P., Beghi, E., Borgatti, R., Ferri, M., Giordano, L., Romeo, A., … & Balottin, U. (2010). Neuropsychological and behavioural aspects in children and adolescents with idiopathic epilepsy at diagnosis and after 12 months of treatment. Seizure, 19(9), 540546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2010.07.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2020). The R Project for Statistical Computing (R. D. C. Team (ed.)). R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rabinovici, G.D., Stephens, M.L., & Possin, K.L. (2015). Executive dysfunction. CONTINUUM Lifelong Learning in Neurology, 21(3), 646659. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.CON.0000466658.05156.54 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratcliffe, C., Wandschneider, B., Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., Koepp, M.J., & Caciagli, L. (2020). Cognitive function in genetic generalized epilepsies: Insights from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. In Frontiers in Neurology (Vol. 11). Frontiers Media S.A. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2020.00144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J., Raven, J., & Court, J. (1998). Manual for Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (1998 edition). Oxford Psychologists Press. ++++=++’+++++===https://vpngate4.hrz.tu-chemnitz.de/+CSCO+1075676763663A2F2F66707562796E652E7462627479722E7172++/scholar?q=Manual+for+Raven’s+Advanced+Progressive+Matrices+%281998+edition%29&btnG=&hl=de&as_sdt=0%2C5 Google Scholar
Riccio, C.A., Pliego, J.A., Cohen, M.J., & Park, Y. (2015). Executive function performance for children with epilepsy localized to the frontal or temporal lobes. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 4(4), 277284. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2014.923774 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riederer, F., Lanzenberger, R., Kaya, M., Prayer, D., Serles, W., & Baumgartner, C. (2008). Network atrophy in temporal lobe epilepsy: A voxel-based morphometry study. Neurology, 71(6), 419425. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000324264.96100.e0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudzinski, L.A. & Meador, K.J. (2013). Epilepsy and neuropsychological comorbidities. CONTINUUM Lifelong Learning in Neurology, 19(3), 682696). https://doi.org/10.1212/01.CON.0000431382.06438.cd CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Guimarães, C.A., Thome-Souza, S., Kuczynski, E., Li, L.M., … Valente, K.D. (2007). Frontal lobe dysfunction in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Pediatric Neurology, 37(3), 176185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.05.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sacchi, C., De Carli, P., Vieno, A., Piallini, G., Zoia, S., & Simonelli, A. (2018). Does infant negative emotionality moderate the effect of maternal depression on motor development? Early Human Development, 119, 5661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.03.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarpa, P., Piazzini, A., Pesenti, G., Brovedani, P., Toraldo, A., Turner, K., … Bottini, G. (2006). Italian neuropsychological instruments to assess memory, attention and frontal functions for developmental age. Neurological Sciences, 27(6), 381396. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072–006–0717–5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheffer, I.E., Berkovic, S., Capovilla, G., Connolly, M.B., French, J., Guilhoto, L., … Zuberi, S.M. (2017). ILAE classification of the epilepsies: Position paper of the ILAE commission for classification and terminology. Epilepsia, 58(4), 512521. https://doi.org/10.1111/epi.13709 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitz, B. (2006). Effects of antiepileptic drugs on mood and behavior. Epilepsia, 47(Suppl. 2), 2833. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528–1167.2006.00684.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schraegle, W.A., Nussbaum, N.L., & Titus, J.B. (2018). Executive dysfunction and depression in pediatric temporal lobe epilepsy: The contribution of hippocampal sclerosis and psychosocial factors. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24(6), 606616. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000140 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schraegle, W.A. & Titus, J.B. (2016). Executive function and health-related quality of life in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 62, 2026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.06.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M., Beck, N., Geisser, M., O’Leary, D., Giordani, B., & Berent, S. (1988). Neuropsychological correlates of academic achievement of children with epilepsy. J Epilepsy, 1, 2329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semrud-Clikeman, M. & Wical, B. (1999). Components of attention in children with complex partial seizures with and without ADHD. Epilepsia, 40(2), 211215. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9952269 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherman, E.M.S., Slick, D.J., & Eyrl, K.L. (2006). Executive dysfunction is a significant predictor of poor quality of life in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 47(11), 19361942. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528–1167.2006.00816.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shinnar, R.C., Shinnar, S., Cnaan, A., Clark, P., Dlugos, D., Hirtz, D.G., … Glauser, T.A. (2017). Pretreatment behavior and subsequent medication effects in childhood absence epilepsy. Neurology, 89(16), 16981706. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000004514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slick, D., Lautzenhiser, A., Sherman, E., & Eyrl, K. (2006). Frequency of scale elevations and factor structure of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in children and adolescents with intractable epilepsy. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3), 181189. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600611320 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoppa, E. & Biancardi, A. (1997). Il test delle Campanelle modificato: una proposta per lo studio dell’attenzione in età evolutiva. Psichiatria Dell’Infanzia e Dell’Adolescenza, 64, 7384.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T. (2011). Functions of the frontal lobes: Relation to executive functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(5), 759765. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000695 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D.T. & Benson, D.F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95(1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.95.1.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J.I., Osipowicz, K., Spechler, P., Sharan, A., Skidmore, C., Doucet, G., & Sperling, M.R. (2014). Functional connectivity evidence of cortico-cortico inhibition in temporal lobe epilepsy. Human Brain Mapping, 35(1), 353366. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22181 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlooswijk, M.C.G., Vaessen, M.J., Jansen, J.F.A., De Krom, M.C.F.T. M., Majoie, H.J.M., Hofman, P.A.M., … Backes, W.H. (2011). Loss of network efficiency associated with cognitive decline in chronic epilepsy. Neurology, 77(10), 938944. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31822cfc2f CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waites, A.B., Briellmann, R.S., Saling, M.M., Abbott, D.F., & Jackson, G.D. (2006). Functional connectivity networks are disrupted in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of Neurology, 59(2), 335343. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20733 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J., Phillips, T., Griebel, M.L., Sharp, G.B., Lange, B., Edgar, T., & Simpson, P. (2001). Factors Associated with Academic Achievement in Children with Controlled Epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2(3), 217223. https://doi.org/10.1006/ebeh.2001.0166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witt, J.A. & Helmstaedter, C. (2012). Should cognition be screened in new-onset epilepsies? A study in 247 untreated patients. Journal of Neurology, 259(8), 17271731. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415–012–6526–2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Q., Rathouz, P.J., Jones, J.E., Jackson, D.C., Hsu, D.A., Stafstrom, C.E., … Hermann, B.P. (2015). Longitudinal trajectories of behavior problems and social competence in children with new onset epilepsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(1), 3744. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12549 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow chart with the procedure of selection of the final sample.

Figure 1

Table 1. For each examined epilepsy category, gender, handedness, comorbidities, age at onset, the time elapsed (in months) from the diagnosis to the first and last evaluation, the number of evaluations performed, and the age (in months) at the diagnosis, seizure status, and medication are reported

Figure 2

Table 2. Different types of lesions of focal/structural epilepsies

Figure 3

Table 3. Percentage of deficits (scores < 2 SD) obtained by the three group of patients at first and last assessment in neuropsychological tasks

Figure 4

Table 4. Mean z-scores (± standard deviations) obtained by the three groups of patients at first and last assessment in neuropsychological tasks

Figure 5

Table 5. Results of the regression analyses on the global executive index and the relative subscales

Figure 6

Fig. 2. Main effects of illness duration, epilepsy category, and medication on phonemic fluency.

Figure 7

Fig. 3. Interaction effect of illness duration and epilepsy category on FAB.

Figure 8

Table 6. Results of the regression analyses on the global attention index and the relative subscales

Supplementary material: File

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials 1

Download Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 23.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials 2

Download Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 21 KB
Supplementary material: File

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials

Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials 3

Download Cainelli et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 19.4 KB