Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T12:20:07.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing Metacognition during a Cognitive Task: Impact of “On-line” Metacognitive Questions on Neuropsychological Performances in a Non-clinical Sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2014

C. Quiles
Affiliation:
Université de Bordeaux, F-33000 Bordeaux, France INSERM, U657, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
H. Verdoux*
Affiliation:
Université de Bordeaux, F-33000 Bordeaux, France INSERM, U657, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
A. Prouteau
Affiliation:
Université de Bordeaux, F-33000 Bordeaux, France Laboratoire de Psychologie Santé et Qualité de la vie EA 4139, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Hélène Verdoux, Hôpital Charles Perrens, 121 rue de la Béchade, 33076 Bordeaux Cedex, France. E-mail: helene.verdoux@u-bordeaux2.fr

Abstract

Whereas metacognition is of great interest for neuropsychological practice, little is known about the impact of metacognitive questions during a neuropsychological assessment. This study explored the impact of measuring “on-line” metacognitive processes on neuropsychological performances in a non-clinical population. Participants were randomly assigned to a “standard” or a “metacognitive” neuropsychological test procedure. The “standard” procedure assessed executive functions (Modified Card Sorting Test), episodic memory (“Rappel libre Rappel indicé16), working memory (digit span test Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III) and social cognition (Faces Test). In the “metacognitive” procedure, two questions were added after each item of these tests to evaluate “on-line” metacognitive monitoring and control. Working memory performances were better and episodic memory performances lower in the “metacognitive” versus the “standard” procedure. No significant difference was found concerning executive functioning or social cognition. The assessment of “on-line” metacognition might improve working memory performances by enhancing concentration, and might impair episodic memory performances by acting as a distractor. These findings may have implications for the development of cognitive remediation programs. (JINS, 2014, 20, 547–554)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Awh, E., Vogel, E.K., & Oh, S.H. (2006). Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience, 139, 201208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacon, E., Danion, J.M., Kauffmann-Muller, F., & Bruant, A. (2001). Consciousness in schizophrenia: A metacognitive approach to semantic memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 473484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacon, E., & Izaute, M. (2009). Metacognition in schizophrenia: Processes underlying patients’ reflections on their own episodic memory. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 10311037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action (Vol. 45). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A.T., Baruch, E., Balter, J.M., Steer, R.A., & Warman, D.M. (2004). A new instrument for measuring insight: The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophrenia Research, 68, 319329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begg, I.M., Martin, L.A., & Needham, D.R. (1992). Memory monitoring: How useful is self-knowledge about memory? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 195218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buschke, H. (1984). Cued recall in amnesia. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 433440.Google Scholar
Butler, D.L., & Winne, P.H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65, 245281.Google Scholar
Chiou, K.S., Carlson, R.A., Arnett, P.A., Cosentino, S.A., & Hillary, F.G. (2011). Metacognitive monitoring in moderate and severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 720731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowley, J.S., Wesensten, N., Kamimori, G., Devine, J., Iwanyk, E., & Balkin, T. (1992). Effect of high terrestrial altitude and supplemental oxygen on human performance and mood. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 63, 696701.Google Scholar
Danion, J.M., Gokalsing, E., Robert, P., Massin-Krauss, M., & Bacon, E. (2001). Defective relationship between subjective experience and behavior in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 20642066.Google Scholar
Dimaggio, G., Vanheule, S., Lysaker, P.H., Carcione, A., & Nicolo, G. (2009). Impaired self-reflection in psychiatric disorders among adults: A proposal for the existence of a network of semi independent functions. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 653664.Google Scholar
Flavell, J.H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In, The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A.C. (2012). Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 129135.Google Scholar
Godefroy, O., Groupe de Réflexion et d’Evaluation des Fonctions Executives. (2008). Fonctions executives et pathologies neurologiques et psychiatriques évaluation en pratique clinique. Marseille: Solal.Google Scholar
Grober, E., Buschke, H., Crystal, H., Bang, S., & Dresner, R. (1988). Screening for dementia by memory testing. Neurology, 38, 900903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamm, J.A., Renard, S.B., Fogley, R.L., Leonhardt, B.L., Dimaggio, G., Buck, K.D., & Lysaker, P.H. (2012). Metacognition and social cognition in schizophrenia: Stability and relationship to concurrent and prospective symptom assessments. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 13031312.Google Scholar
Hoffman, B., & Spatariu, A. (2008). The influence of self-efficacy and metacognitive prompting on math problem-solving efficiency. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 875893.Google Scholar
Kauffman, D.F. (2004). Self-regulated learning in web-based environments: Instructional tools designed to facilitate cognitive strategy use, metacognitive processing and motivational beliefs. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 30, 139161.Google Scholar
Koren, D., Poyurovsky, M., Seidman, L.J., Goldsmith, M., Wenger, S., & Klein, E.M. (2005). The neuropsychological basis of competence to consent in first-episode schizophrenia: A pilot metacognitive study. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 609616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren, D., Seidman, L.J., Goldsmith, M., & Harvey, P.D. (2006). Real-world cognitive–and metacognitive–dysfunction in schizophrenia: A new approach for measuring (and remediating) more “right stuff”. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 310326.Google Scholar
Koren, D., Seidman, L.J., Poyurovsky, M., Goldsmith, M., Viksman, P., Zichel, S., & Klein, E. (2004). The neuropsychological basis of insight in first-episode schizophrenia: A pilot metacognitive study. Schizophrenia Research, 70, 195202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A. (2007). Metacognition and consciousness. In P.D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 289325). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103, 490517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 7582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J.W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339354.Google Scholar
Lysaker, P.H., Shea, A.M., Buck, K.D., Dimaggio, G., Nicolo, G., Procacci, M., … Rand, K.L. (2010). Metacognition as a mediator of the effects of impairments in neurocognition on social function in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122, 405413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merceron, K., & Prouteau, A. (2013). Evaluation de la cognition sociale en langue française chez l’adulte: outils disponibles et recommandations de bonne pratique clinique. L’évolution psychiatrique, 78, 5370.Google Scholar
Nelson, H.E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T.O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, T.O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition?. In J. Metcalfe & A.P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. Cambridge: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Prouteau, A., Verdoux, H., Briand, C., Lesage, A., Lalonde, P., Nicole, L., … Stip, E. (2004). Self-assessed cognitive dysfunction and objective performance in outpatients with schizophrenia participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophrenia Research, 69, 8591.Google Scholar
Quiles, C., Prouteau, A., & Verdoux, H. (2013). Characteristics and impact of metacognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Encephale, 39, 123129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, G., Frith, C.D., & Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science, 278, 16161619.Google Scholar
Schoo, L.A., van Zandvoort, M.J., Biessels, G.J., Kappelle, L.J., & Postma, A. (2013). Insight in cognition: Self-awareness of performance across cognitive domains. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 20, 95102.Google Scholar
Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Falcone, M., Nicolò, G., Procacci, M., & Alleva, G. (2003). How to evaluate metacognitive functioning in psychotherapy? The metacognition assessment scale and its applications. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10, 238261.Google Scholar
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G.C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 2233; 34–57.Google Scholar
STATA, C. (2009). Stata Statistical Software: Release 11. College Station TX: StataCorp.Google Scholar
Tas, C., Brown, E.C., Esen-Danaci, A., Lysaker, P.H., & Brune, M. (2012). Intrinsic motivation and metacognition as predictors of learning potential in patients with remitted schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46, 10861092.Google Scholar
Thiede, K.W., Anderson, M.C.M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of Metacognitive Monitoring Affects Learning of Texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 6673.Google Scholar
Tuboly, G., Rudas, L., Csillik, A., Nagy, A., Benedek, K., Benedek, G., & Braunitzer, G. (2012). Haemodynamic parameters and cognitive function during modeled acute volume loss. Acta Physiologica Hungarica, 99, 118125.Google Scholar
Van der Linden, M., Groupe de Réflexion sur l’Evaluation de la Mémoire (2004). L’évaluation des troubles de la mémoire: Présentation de quatre tests de mémoire épisodique (avec leur étalonage). Marseille: Solal.Google Scholar
Veenman, M., Prins, F., & Elshout, J. (2002). Initial inductive learning in a complex computer simulated environment: The role of metacognitive skills and intellectual ability. Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 327341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D.A. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd ed. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B.J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 329339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar