Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:46:54.879Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Accuracy of estimates of serving size using digitally displayed food photographs among Japanese adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2022

Nana Shinozaki
Affiliation:
Department of Social and Preventive Epidemiology, School of Public Health, The University of Tokyo, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan
Kentaro Murakami*
Affiliation:
Department of Social and Preventive Epidemiology, School of Public Health, The University of Tokyo, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan
*
*Corresponding author: Kentaro Murakami, fax +81-3-5841-7873, email kenmrkm@m.u-tokyo.ac.jp

Abstract

We evaluated the accuracy of the estimated serving size using digital photographs in a newly developed food atlas. From 209 food items in the food atlas, we selected 14 items with various appearances for evaluation. At the study site, fifty-four participants aged 18–33 years served fourteen foods in the amount they usually ate. After they left, each food item was weighed by a researcher. The following day, the participants estimated the quantity of each food they served based on food photographs using a web-based questionnaire. We compared the weights of the foods the participants served (true serving sizes) and those determined based on the photographs (estimated serving sizes). For ten of the fourteen food items, significant differences were observed between the estimated and true serving sizes, ranging from a 29⋅8 % underestimation (curry sauce) to a 34⋅0 % overestimation (margarine). On average, the relative difference was 8⋅8 %. Overall, 51⋅6 % of the participants were within ±25 % of the true serving size, 81⋅9 % were within ±50 % and 93⋅4 % were within ±75 %. Bland–Altman plots showed wide limits of agreement and increased variances with larger serving sizes for most food items. Overall, no association was found between estimation errors and participant characteristics. The food atlas has shown potential for assessment of portion size estimation. Further development, refinement and testing are needed to improve the usefulness of the digital food photographic atlas as a portion size estimation aid.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

One of the major sources of error in dietary assessment is the estimation of the food portion size(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1,Reference Almiron-Roig, Navas-Carretero and Emery2) . Although weighing foods is considered accurate in quantifying the amount of food consumed, it is time-consuming, requires a high level of cooperation from respondents and may alter respondents’ eating behaviour(Reference Rutishauser3). Therefore, various portion size estimation aids have been developed as alternatives for assessing food portions, such as household measures, food models and food photographs(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1). Food photographs are less burdensome for respondents, easy to use for interviewees, portable and inexpensive, and cover a broad range of foods(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1,Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4Reference Sharma and Chadha7) . During data collection using food photographs, participants are asked to indicate the quantity of food they consumed by selecting a single picture or reporting a fraction, multiple or percentage of the amount shown in one photograph(Reference Naska, Valanou and Peppa8). Today, food photographs with multiple-portion images are widely used in various dietary assessment methods, including automated self-administered 24-h recalls(Reference Jacques, Lemieux and Lamarche9Reference Simpson, Bradley and Poliakov11).

Food photographs for portion size estimation should be designed for each country based on food availability, preferences and dietary intake data(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5) . In addition, the characteristics of food photographs (e.g. the method of food presentation) may affect the perception, conceptualisation and memory of respondents, ultimately influencing the validity of the photographs(Reference Nelson, Atkinson and Darbyshire12,Reference Nelson, Atkinson and Darbyshire13) . Furthermore, it has been reported that the characteristics of respondents, such as age, affect the accuracy of portion size estimation(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1,Reference Almiron-Roig, Navas-Carretero and Emery2) . Therefore, it is necessary to validate food photographs before using them as a portion size estimation aid in dietary surveys. To date, food photographs have been validated in various countries(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5,Reference Naska, Valanou and Peppa8,Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10,Reference Foster, Matthews and Lloyd14Reference Harris-Fry, Paudel and Karn45) . Although there are food photographs for estimationg portion size in Japan(Reference Saeki, Otaki and Kitagawa46), these were developed and validated food photographs in an elderly population (≥60 years) consisting of mostly women (90 %) living in one prefecture in Japan. Moreover, the portion sizes of the images were based on general recipes rather than dietary data. Thus, the comprehensiveness and representativeness of the food photographs are questionable. Given that Japanese cuisines consist of various amorphous foods, portion size estimation aids using digital photographs of foods consumed in Japan should be carefully developed and validated(Reference Thoradeniya, de Silva and Arambepola43,Reference Almiron-Roig, Aitken and Galloway47) .

We recently developed a digital food photographic atlas to help estimate portion size in dietary surveys in Japan(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura48). Briefly, it was developed based on dietary record (DR) data among 644 Japanese adults for 4–16 d (5512 d in total), including weekdays (working days) and weekend days (non-working days). From the 1962 food and dish items identified from the DR, we selected approximately 300 top items in terms of the frequency of consumption, the sum of the consumed amount and energy contribution in the entire population. After eliminating food and dish items that did not require a photograph for portion size estimation, 209 commonly consumed food and dish items were included in the food atlas. Of these, 105 items are presented as a series of three to seven photographs showing gradually increasing portion sizes, while 104 items are shown as guide photographs representing a range of portion sizes and food varieties in one photograph. Portion sizes were determined based on market research and the distribution of food consumption in the DR. Moreover, the food atlas includes photographs of thirty-four household measurement items, such as cups and glasses.

Although the food atlas can be used as a portion size estimation tool in a self-administered 24-h dietary recall in the future, its validity has not been evaluated. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate the validity of images from the newly developed food atlas as a portion size estimation aid for Japanese adults. We evaluated the accuracy of the estimated serving size (the amount of food as served) rather than portion size (the amount of food as consumed). This was due to difficulties in asking study participants to eat a test meal during the infection control of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). In addition, we explored the association between estimation errors and participant characteristics, such as sex and age.

Methods

Selection of food items

Since it was infeasible to assess the validity of all food items in the food atlas(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura48) with limited research resources, this study evaluated representative food items. Based on the number of food items (n 9–13) assessed in previous validation studies on food photographs(Reference Subar, Crafts and Zimmerman29,Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34,Reference Foster, O'Keeffe and Matthews49,Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50) , we decided to evaluate approximately ten foods which could include at least one item from different food categories: amorphous/soft foods, liquid, spread, single-unit foods and small pieces(Reference Subar, Crafts and Zimmerman29). A set of representative food items was selected considering various appearances, consistencies and textures(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference de Vlieger, Weltert and Molenaar6,Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10) , mainly containing ingredients from different food groups(Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34). We selected curry and rice, salad and white rice as amorphous/soft foods; dressing, coffee and miso soup as a liquid; margarine as spread; grilled mackerel and bananas as single-unit foods; and cookies, Japanese fried chicken and simmered squash as foods in small-piece shapes. The description and portion size of these twelve food items are listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Description of foods and food photographs selected for this study

* A series of photographs show gradually increasing portion sizes.

A guide photograph shows the range of portion sizes and food varieties in one photograph.

Study participants

The study participants included only students and staff at the University of Tokyo due to the entrance restriction to a study site on campus during the COVID-19 pandemic. Recruitment was conducted through e-mail and social media (LINE and Twitter), which included a web link to a study website. Potential participants were encouraged to read the details of the study on the website, with the purpose of the study concealed(Reference Nelson and Haraldsdóttir51). The inclusion criteria were healthy students or faculty members of the University of Tokyo aged 18–65 years who could complete a web-based questionnaire using a computer. The exclusion criteria were individuals who had majored in nutritional science or cooking (e.g. dietitians and chefs), pregnant or lactating women, and those with severe visual or neurological deficits or severe food allergies. In addition, individuals who did not usually eat any of the three test foods (white rice, bread or bananas) were excluded. This was because, while this study asked the participants to serve test foods, if they had never or rarely eaten a certain test food, they were asked to imagine a similar food while serving the food, which was considered difficult for the three foods listed above due to their unique shape and characteristics. To check eligibility, a questionnaire was administered on the website, and those who met all these criteria could be applied for the study using an online reservation form. The numbers of male and female participants were controlled to be almost equal by controlling the available slots by sex. We aimed to include fifty participants based on the guideline of validation studies of food photographs(Reference Nelson and Haraldsdóttir51). Assuming a 20 % dropout rate(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4), sixty-three adults (thirty-three males and thirty females) were invited to participate in this study.

The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Tokyo (2021121NI-(1); approved 15 September 2021). Online informed consent was obtained from each participant by clicking the ‘I agree’ box on the online booking form. In addition, written informed consent was obtained from all the participants during their visit to the study site. Each participant received an Amazon gift card worth 2000 Japanese yen (approximately £12⋅5) when they completed the study.

Study design

Data were collected in November 2021. On the first day of the study, participants served test foods on plates at the study site (serving session). The next day, participants were asked about the type and amount of food they served using food photographs through a web-based questionnaire (estimation session). Thus, the study consisted of two separate sessions over 2 d for each participant (Fig. 1), which can replicate a situation similar to self-administered 24-h recalls. Prior to data collection, a pilot study was conducted with five students and staff in our department to improve the study protocol.

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the study.

Preparation of test foods and the study site for the serving session

Prior to the serving session, the test foods were purchased from local supermarkets and convenience stores. We bought several products of different shapes and sizes for bananas (n 5), cookies (n 6) and Japanese fried chicken (n 3). For other foods, one product was purchased for each item. The products purchased were identical to those shown in the photographs for several items (curry, white rice, dressing, margarine, simmered squash and one of the three types of Japanese fried chicken). In contrast, for other food items, the products purchased were different from those shown in the photographs.

Each food was prepared on an appropriate cooking utensil, such as rice cookers, pots and plates. The amount of food prepared was at least 1⋅5 times greater than the seventh portion size in the food atlas(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura48), except for some cookies with a large bulk per unit weight, which were prepared in quantities appropriate for the plate. All foods were arranged on tables at a study site, which was prepared to be similar to the situation of serving food at home (Supplementary Fig. S1(a)). The twelve test foods were categorised into lunch, snack, dinner and other menus, considering the standard number and combinations of dishes at meals in the Japanese diet(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura52). The lunch menu consisted of curry and rice, salad, dressing and bananas; the snack menu comprised cookies and coffee; the dinner menu comprised a bowl of white rice, miso soup, grilled mackerel (or Japanese fried chicken) and simmered squash; and the other menu was margarine, accompanied by a slice of bread. Each menu was separately placed on the table (Supplementary Fig. S1(b)–(e)). A total of sixty-four types of various tableware (plates, cups, rice bowls, soup bowls, chopsticks, spoons, knives and forks), including twenty used in the photos of the test foods, were also prepared on a separate table (Supplementary Fig. S1(f)). Each tableware was assigned an identification (ID), weighed on a calibrated cooking scale (KW-320, Tanita, Tokyo, Japan) and measured up to 300 g in 0⋅1 g, 300–1500 g in 0⋅5 g and 1500–3000 g in 1 g.

Serving session

Each serving session was conducted by one person at a time (seven participants per day). Before each session, the author (K. M.) weighed all test foods using a calibrated cooking scale. The participants received verbal and written explanations of the study protocol from the author (N. S.) in a space separated from the study site. We told the participants in advance that they would not eat the food to prevent the expectation of eating the food from altering their behaviours(Reference Vereecken, Dohogne and Covents18). The participants were requested to answer a paper questionnaire asking, ‘How hungry are you right now?’. Potential responses were provided on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘extremely hungry’ to ‘not hungry’. The questionnaire also asked whether the participants ate each test food. If there was a food that had never or rarely been eaten, the participants were instructed to serve the food imagining similar shapes of the food (e.g. peanut butter instead of margarine)(Reference Vereecken, Dohogne and Covents18) to prevent missing information about the serving size of the food. This procedure also imitates an actual self-administered 24-h dietary recall method in which some food items are estimated using portion images that closely resemble those foods(Reference Bradley, Simpson and Poliakov53).

Participants were then invited to the study site. First, the participants were told, ‘Assuming this menu is your lunch, please serve each food on a plate in the amount you usually eat’. They were asked to use tableware (e.g. plates, rice bowls and cups) similar to those they usually used at home. In addition, they were asked to select the appropriate cutlery (i.e. chopsticks, spoons, knives and forks) for the meal. After serving the food, the participants were asked to sit in front of the table and arrange each plate and cutlery on a tray; thus, their eye level would be closer to the real eating situation. This process was repeated in the order of snack, dinner and margarine. For bananas, participants were asked to choose one of the available variations. For cookies and Japanese fried chicken, the participants were first asked to choose one of the variations available and then serve it in the amount they would eat. For the dinner menu, participants were asked to serve white rice, miso soup, simmered squash and grilled mackerel as one menu. They were then asked to serve Japanese fried chicken instead of grilled mackerel, with the other items remaining.

After each participant left the room, the author (K. M.) weighed the plate or cup in which each food item was served. The author (N. S.) recorded the total weight of the plate and food and the IDs of the plates and cutlery on measurement record sheets. The time taken to complete the process of serving food was also recorded. The food served was returned to each cooking utensil, if possible. Moreover, the food was refilled as needed.

Estimation session

On the morning of the next day, we sent each participant an e-mail containing a link to a web-based questionnaire asking him/her to estimate the amount of each food served on the previous day based on the food photographs. The participants were asked to answer the questionnaire within 12 h after the reception of the e-mail to replicate the situation of 24-h recalls.

The serving size of each food was determined using either a guide photograph (Fig. 2(a)) or a series of photographs (Fig. 2(b)). Both types of photographs contained one or two standard reference objects (e.g. knife and fork) to help estimate the quantity of food and dishes and the size of the plate(Reference Al Marzooqi, Burke and Al Ghazali54,Reference Nelson and Haraldsdóttir55) . The guide photographs were used for bananas, cookies and Japanese fried chicken, with one photograph representing various sizes and types of food (Supplementary Fig. S2(i), (j), and (k), respectively). The number of variations shown in the photographs was three for bananas, nineteen for cookies and five for Japanese fried chicken (Table 1). For these items, the participants were first asked to select one that looked similar to the food they had served on the previous day (Fig. 2(a)). For cookies and Japanese fried chicken, the participants further answered the number of pieces they served.

Fig. 2. Example screens from the web-based questionnaire, with English translation shown in the boxes. (a) Screenshot of the question to ask the type of cookies using a guide photograph and (b) screenshot of the question to ask the serving size of curry and rice using a series of photographs.

A series of photographs, which show gradually increasing portion sizes (Table 1), were used for the other items (examples of photographs shown in Supplementary Fig. S2 (a) for curry and rice, (b) for salad, (c) for white rice, (d) for dressing, (e) for coffee, (f) for miso soup, (g) for margarine, (h) for grilled mackerel and (l) for simmered squash). Since curry and rice are usually served on one plate and eaten together in Japan, the photos also showed their portions on a single plate, from which the participants were asked to choose one (Fig. 2(b)). The participants were asked to choose the image closest to the amount of food they had served on the previous day (Fig. 2(b)). There were seven portion sizes, except for white rice and miso soup on small and medium bowls (3–6 portions). Moreover, the participants could report the serving size other than the specific photograph in a free-text field, such as less than the smallest, more than the largest or between pictures. The serving sizes of white rice, miso soup and coffee were asked through a two-step question. First, the participants were shown photographs of different rice bowls (n 3), soup bowls (n 3) and coffee cups (n 6), and were asked to choose one that was similar to the tableware they used in the serving session (Supplementary Fig. S2(c), (f), and (e), respectively). Next, the participants were shown images of portions for each tableware, from which they were asked to choose one.

The questionnaire also included questions on sex, age (years), body height (cm), weight (kg), occupation (undergraduate students, graduate students or faculty members) and cooking frequency per week (less than once, two or three, four or five or more than six). To prevent any missing responses, the questionnaire was designed such that the participants could not move to the next page before answering all questions.

To unify the layout of the answer screen as consistently as possible among participants, the participants were required to use a computer to answer the questionnaire and not use a smartphone or tablet. The participants answered the questionnaire at any location. The web-based questionnaire was developed using the online survey platform Questant (Macromill, Inc., Tokyo, Japan).

Calculation of the true and estimated serving sizes

During the serving session, the amount of food served (true serving size) was calculated for each food item by subtracting the pre-weighed weight of each plate or cup from the total weight of the plate and food.

For the estimation session, the estimated serving sizes using the food photographs (estimated serving size) were computed based on the weight of the food in the selected photograph in the food atlas database(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura48). The estimated serving sizes of the Japanese fried chicken and cookies were calculated as the weight of a single piece selected multiplied by the number of pieces reported. If the serving size was estimated as a multiple of a specific photograph (e.g. 0⋅9 times of photo A) in the free-text field, this value was used to calculate the serving size. If no specific value was given, the serving size was calculated by multiplying the food weight of the selected photograph by 0⋅8 for the answer ‘a little less’ or 1⋅2 for the answer ‘a little more’. These values were determined based on the average weight ratio between the food weights for each photo in the series of photographs (0⋅7 for decreasing portions and 1⋅4 for increasing portions). When the number between photos was reported, the mean value of the two food weights was used.

Statistical analysis

For the analysis, curry and rice were analysed separately for the whole curry and rice, curry sauce and white rice, resulting in a total of fourteen items for evaluation. We assessed the difference between the true and estimated serving size as follows. First, the absolute difference (g) between the true and estimated serving sizes was evaluated using the paired t-test and Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test results were presented because the two tests yielded similar results. Second, the relative difference (%) was calculated by subtracting the true serving size from the estimated serving size, divided by the true serving size and multiplied by 100. Moreover, the percentage of participants selecting serving sizes within ±10, ±25, ±50 and ±75 % of the true serving size was calculated. Furthermore, Bland–Altman plots(Reference Martin Bland and Altman56) were used to assess the degree of agreement between the true and estimated serving sizes. We also used linear regression analysis to examine the proportional bias between true and estimated serving sizes(Reference Montenij, Buhre and Jansen57).

The basic characteristics of the participants were presented as the mean and standard deviation (sd) or the number of participants (%). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as body weight (kg) divided by the square of body height (m2). The cooking frequencies of ‘four or five’ and ‘more than six’ were combined because each category included a few participants. Similarly, the hunger level was grouped into three categories: ‘hungry’, ‘neither’ and ‘not hungry’.

We assessed the association between participant characteristics (sex, age, BMI, occupation, cooking frequency and hunger level) and errors in serving size estimation. Age and BMI were converted into categorical variables using the median, generating younger (18–23 years, n 31) or older groups (24–33 years, n 23), and lower BMI (16⋅8–20⋅6 kg/m2, n 27) or higher BMI groups (20⋅8–30⋅8 kg/m2, n 27), respectively. The occupation categories ‘graduate student’ and ‘faculty member’ were combined because the latter included a limited number of participants. To compare the mean relative difference among categories, the Wilcoxon rank-sum test and the Kruskal–Wallis test were used as appropriate.

All statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Analysis System (SAS) version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Two-sided P-values < 0⋅05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Participant characteristics

Of the sixty-three participants enrolled in the study, eight did not attend, and one did not answer the web-based questionnaire (Fig. 1). Consequently, fifty-four participants (twenty-seven men and twenty-seven women) aged 18–33 years were included in the analysis. Table 2 shows the participant characteristics. The mean age was 23⋅6 years (sd: 3⋅2), and the mean BMI was 20⋅8 kg/m2 (sd: 2⋅5). Approximately half (52 %) of the participants were undergraduate students. Most participants (69 %) reported habitual cooking more than twice a week. The mean duration of the experiment for serving food was 11⋅5 min (sd: 2⋅2). The median ratio of the participants who had never or rarely eaten the test food for each food item was 2⋅8 % (interquartile range: 0–8⋅3).

Table 2. Participant characteristics (n 54)

sd, standard deviation.

Differences between the true and the estimated serving sizes

Table 3 shows the differences between the true and estimated serving sizes for each food item. In ten of the fourteen food items examined, a significant difference was observed between estimated and true serving sizes. The mean estimated serving sizes of four items (curry and rice, curry sauce, cookies and a bowl of rice) were smaller than the true serving size, whereas those of six items (salad, banana, coffee, miso soup, Japanese fried chicken and margarine) were larger than the true serving size. The mean relative difference between the estimated and true serving sizes ranged from a 29⋅8 % underestimation for curry sauce to a 34⋅0 % overestimation for margarine. On average, the relative difference was 8⋅8 %.

Table 3. Difference between the true and the estimated serving sizes of food and drink items (n 54)

sd, standard deviation.

* Absolute difference (g) = estimated serving size − true serving size. Thus, positive and negative values indicate overestimation and underestimation of the serving size, respectively.

Relative difference (%) = ([estimated serving size − true serving size]/true serving size) × 100.

Differences between the amounts served and estimated were tested using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

The lowest percentage of participants whose estimate was within ±10 % of the true serving size was observed for Japanese fried chicken (7⋅4 %), while the highest percentage was observed for simmered squash (37⋅0 %). On average, 51⋅6 % of the participants were within ±25 % of the true serving size, 81⋅9 % were within ±50 % and 93⋅4 % were within ±75 %.

Agreement between the true and the estimated serving sizes

The Bland–Altman plots are shown in Fig. 3 for four food items served for dinner (a bowl of rice, miso soup, grilled mackerel and simmered squash) and Supplementary Fig. S3 for other items. Because of the limited number of photographs in the portion size selection, most plots showed several diagonal rows of data points. The limits of agreement were wide for most food items, mainly because of increased dispersion with larger serving sizes. Linear regression analysis demonstrated that the slope of the mean bias for each food item was significantly different from 0 for five items (salad, dressing, coffee, Japanese fried chicken and margarine), indicating a proportional bias that differences between the true and estimated serving size increased as the mean food weight increased.

Fig. 3. Bland–Altman plots assessing the agreement of the served and estimated food amounts in fifty-four Japanese adults: (a) a bowl of rice, (b) miso soup, (c) grilled mackerel, (d) simmered squash and (e) Japanese fried chicken. The solid line represents the mean difference, and the dotted line represents lower and upper limits of agreement with a solid regression line added.

Association between participant characteristics and estimation error

Overall, there were no relative differences in most food items among the categories of participant characteristics, including sex, age, BMI, occupation, cooking frequency and hunger level. However, the relative difference in the volume of coffee was larger in women than in men (mean relative difference: 33⋅3 % v. 20⋅8 %, P = 0⋅04). Moreover, compared with the younger group, the older group had a larger relative difference for miso soup (mean relative difference: 15⋅6 % v. 41⋅8 %, P = 0⋅02) and simmered pumpkin (mean relative difference: −6⋅4 % v. 14⋅2 %, P = 0⋅004). Furthermore, the estimated serving size of the simmered pumpkin was smaller than the true serving size among undergraduate students, whereas it was larger than that in the group of graduate students and university staff (mean relative difference: −5⋅0 % v. 10⋅3 %, P = 0⋅01).

Discussion

Main findings

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to assess the validity of comprehensive digital food photographs as a portion size estimation aid in Japan. The results showed significant differences between the estimated and true serving sizes for most food items, with wide limits of agreement. However, on average, the relative difference was small, and more than half of estimates were within ±25 % of the true serving size. These findings indicate that the food atlas can be used in portion size estimation. However, further development, refinement and testing are needed to improve the usefulness of the digital food photographic atlas as a portion size estimation aid.

Comparison with previous studies

The acceptable accuracy of portion size measurement has not yet been established(Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22,Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50) . However, a previous study considered that the relative error within 25 % was acceptable(Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5), and the present study showed a mean relative difference within this range (8⋅8 %). Similarly, previous studies, which asked adult participants to recall the consumed amount of food of the previous day, also showed a mean relative difference within the range: 3⋅6 % in the USA(Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10), −4⋅1 % in Lebanon(Reference Tueni, Mounayar and Birlouez-Aragon35) and 4⋅5 % in Nepal(Reference Harris-Fry, Paudel and Karn45).

In the present study, the relative difference between the true and estimated serving sizes was large among food items, indicating that the estimation accuracy varied across foods. Moreover, we observed both underestimation and overestimation of food serving sizes. These results are consistent with those of previous studies. For instance, the percentage errors across foods in studies using recall methods for adults ranged from −29 to +38 % in the UK(Reference Robson and Livingstone17), −24 to 17 % in the USA(Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10), −14 to +117 % in Belgium(Reference de Keyzer, Huybrechts and de Maeyer19), −7 to +9 % in Bolivia(Reference Lazarte, Encinas and Alegre38), −25 to +6 % in Malawi(Reference Flax, Thakwalakwa and Schnefke36), −8 to 6 % in Burkina Faso(Reference Huybregts, Roberfroid and Lachat39), −12 to +15 % in Lebanon(Reference Tueni, Mounayar and Birlouez-Aragon35) and −8 to +82 % in the United Arab Emirates(Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34). On average, in the present study, 51⋅6 % of the participants had estimates within ±25 % of the true serving size, which is higher than that of adults in the USA (38 %)(Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10) and the Netherlands (35 %)(Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50). Nevertheless, a direct comparison of the results is difficult because of the large differences in the study design, such as the target population, selection of food items and timing of estimation(Reference Vereecken, Dohogne and Covents18).

The accuracy of portion size estimation is affected by the type, shape, texture and size of food(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1,Reference Sharma and Chadha7) . Portion size estimation is particularly difficult for foods with certain characteristics, such as amorphous foods(Reference Amoutzopoulos, Page and Roberts1,Reference de Vlieger, Weltert and Molenaar6,Reference Subar, Crafts and Zimmerman29,Reference Amougou, Cohen and Mbala40) . In contrast, foods consumed in a defined unit can be easily estimated. For instance, the proportion of reported portion sizes within 10 and 25 % of the true serving size was the largest for single-unit foods in previous studies(Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10,Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50) . However, we did not find a clear association between estimation errors and food characteristics. For example, although both grilled mackerel and bananas are single-unit foods, a non-significant difference between the true and estimated serving sizes was observed only for the grilled mackerel. Furthermore, the percentage of participants whose estimate was within ±10 % of the true serving size was lowest for Japanese fried chicken and highest for simmered squash, although these foods had small pieces in common. These results suggest that the accuracy of estimates may vary even among foods with similar characteristics, as previously reported(Reference Huybregts, Roberfroid and Lachat39). Meanwhile, all foods depicted in the guide photographs (i.e. bananas, cookies and Japanese fried chicken) showed a significant difference between the estimated and true values, suggesting that the type of photograph may affect the accuracy of the estimates. However, a formal analysis comparing the estimation accuracy between food characteristics or types of photographs was hindered because of the small number of foods within each category.

The conditions of the study may also affect the estimation accuracy. The fact that participants had never or rarely eaten the test foods may have affected the accuracy of the estimates, whereas the percentage of such individuals was low for each food in this study. In addition, the percentages of subjects who estimated within 10 and 25 % of the true serving size were similar between foods for which the items prepared in the serving session were identical to those in the photographs (curry and rice, curry sauce, white rice, dressing, a bowl of rice, margarine and simmered squash) and those for the other foods (data not shown).

The limits of agreement were wide for most food items, indicating a large individual variation in the estimation ability. In general, estimation accuracy decreases as the portion size increases(Reference Vereecken, Dohogne and Covents18,Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22) . Indeed, we observed an increase in variance with increasing amounts for most foods, similar to previous studies(Reference Turconi, Guarcello and Berzolari21,Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34,Reference Huybregts, Roberfroid and Lachat39) . Moreover, previous studies have indicated the presence of a ‘flat slope syndrome’, in which small portions are overestimated and large portions are underestimated(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5,Reference Naska, Valanou and Peppa8,Reference Flax, Thakwalakwa and Schnefke36,Reference Amougou, Cohen and Mbala40,Reference Lafrenière, Lamarche and Laramée58) . For instance, a previous study reported that portions of ≥100 g were, on average, underestimated by 2⋅4 %, whereas small portions (<100 g) were overestimated by 17⋅1 %(Reference Lafrenière, Lamarche and Laramée58). However, we observed that both the food items with the means of true serving sizes of ≥100 g (nine items) and <100 g (five items) were overestimated (mean relative difference, 3⋅9 and 17⋅6 %, respectively). Moreover, the proportional bias observed for several items (dressing, salad, coffee, Japanese fried chicken and margarine) indicated that the portion size is more likely to be overestimated as it increases. A previous study explained that one of the reasons for flat slope syndrome was that portion size options were limited to below the smallest portion and above the largest portion(Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5). This could not have happened in this study since the participants were allowed to report a serving size smaller than the smallest food quantity and that larger than the largest one; nevertheless, no such answers were reported. As the number of participants and test foods was limited in this study, further studies on the association between portion size and estimation accuracy are required.

Previous studies have found inconsistent associations between errors in portion size estimation using food photographs and BMI(Reference Nelson, Atkinson and Darbyshire12,Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22,Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34) , sex(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference Robson and Livingstone17,Reference de Keyzer, Huybrechts and de Maeyer19,Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22,Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34,Reference Amougou, Cohen and Mbala40) and age(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4,Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22,Reference Ali, Platat and El Mesmoudi34,Reference Thoradeniya, de Silva and Arambepola43,Reference Nelson, Atkinson and Darbyshire59) . Overall, the present study showed no difference in the errors in serving size estimation among the categories of participant characteristics. Therefore, the general error pattern associated with a particular food may be due to its photographic presentation rather than to specific participant characteristics(Reference Robson and Livingstone17). In the present study, the serving size of coffee was overestimated in women than in men, which is inconsistent with a previous study(Reference de Keyzer, Huybrechts and de Maeyer19). Since there was a large variation in the study design in previous studies, further studies are needed to determine the effect of participant characteristics on the accuracy of portion size estimation.

Strengths and limitations

The strength of the present study is its design, which imitates the situation of a 24-h dietary recall. It allowed evaluation of the accuracy of estimates concerning the conceptualisation of foods and memory while recalling the diet(Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50). However, the present study had several limitations. First, the convenience sample consisted of young and highly educated individuals, which reduced the generalisability of the results. Moreover, participants may have been motivated to report the serving size accurately because they received a reward. Additionally, owing to limited research resources, the present study included the minimum number of participants recommended for validation studies of food photographs(Reference Nelson and Haraldsdóttir51). Consequently, our sample size may have resulted in non-significant results. A post hoc power analysis revealed that the four items that did not significantly differ between true and estimated serving sizes had inadequate statistical power, ranging from 11 % (simmered squash) to 42 % (dressing). Therefore, these findings should be interpreted with caution. The validity of food photographs should be evaluated in a larger population with diverse backgrounds, including children and older adults. Second, food photographs may have varied in resolution and size, depending on the devices used to answer the web questionnaire, possibly causing between-person variations in estimation accuracy. Previous studies have reported that the use of tablets showed less accuracy than the use of computer screen(Reference Nichelle, Almeida and Camey4), whereas the size of the images of food portions did not affect the accuracy of estimation in both children(Reference Baranowski, Baranowski and Watson30,Reference Thoradeniya, de Silva and Arambepola43) and adults(Reference Subar, Crafts and Zimmerman29). Accordingly, we asked the participants to answer the web questionnaire using any computer available, and almost all participants (98⋅2 %) responded to the web questionnaire through the computer. Third, the food photographs were tested in a highly controlled setting. Although participants were not informed of the purpose of the study, they may have paid more attention to the amount of food. Moreover, they were asked to serve a limited number of foods that had almost the same appearance in the photographs, whereas people consumed a much wider variety of foods in the real world. The tableware variation in the serving session was also limited, including some of the same tableware as those in the photographs, which is unlikely to occur in a real-life situation. Therefore, our results should be considered a best-case scenario. Fourth, the number of food items validated in this study accounted for only 6 % of foods included in the food atlas(Reference Shinozaki, Murakami and Asakura48). However, because the test food items were selected to represent various food categories, appearances and food groups, the food atlas may be generally able to estimate portion sizes. Further validation studies with other photographs are needed to confirm the validity of the entire food atlas. Lastly, we could not evaluate the validity of the food photographs in estimating the portion size (the amount of food consumed) because the foods were not consumed for hygiene reasons. Previous studies in adults have suggested that food weights can be both underestimated and overestimated regardless of whether study participants ate(Reference Kirkpatrick, Potischman and Dodd10,Reference Robson and Livingstone17,Reference Lazarte, Encinas and Alegre38,Reference Harris-Fry, Paudel and Karn45) or did not eat(Reference Szenczi-Cseh, Horváth and Ambrus5,Reference de Keyzer, Huybrechts and de Maeyer19,Reference Frobisher and Maxwell26,Reference Huybregts, Roberfroid and Lachat39) test foods. Meanwhile, studies on children have suggested that the amount of food consumed is estimated less accurately than the amount served because of the impact of errors in reporting leftovers(Reference Foster, Matthews and Lloyd14). Given the latter finding, the accuracy of food portion estimates may have been overestimated in this study. Therefore, for future development of a food atlas used in a 24-h recall method, the validity of food photographs should be assessed for actual food consumption using more photographs in a less controlled setting.

Conclusion

We assessed the accuracy of the estimates of the amount of foods self-served by participants using digital food photographs from a recently developed food atlas. The results suggest that, for some foods, the use of digital food photographs resulted in an estimation error at both the individual and group levels. Nevertheless, a digital photographic food atlas is a convenient portion size estimation aid, and the accuracy of estimates from food photographs could be improved through further modification of the portion sizes depicted in the food atlas. Moreover, future studies may consider combining food photographs with other portion size estimations (e.g. line diagrams and textual descriptions of portion sizes) that can flexibly estimate the portion sizes of complex dishes(Reference Ovaskainen, Paturi and Reinivuo22,Reference Thoradeniya, de Silva and Arambepola43,Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50) . A previous study reported that amorphous foods and liquids were more accurately estimated using textual descriptions (i.e. estimating in grams or millilitres, standard portion sizes and household measures) than using photographs(Reference Lucassen, Willemsen and Geelen50). Thus, it may be better to provide several options for reporting portion sizes. Therefore, further development, refinement and testing of digital food photographs are needed to improve the accuracy of estimating the food portion size.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/jns.2022.102.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Keiko Asakura, Shizuko Masayasu and Satoshi Sasaki for providing the dietary data used to develop the food atlas, and Takahiro Imashimizu for technical guidance on food photography.

N. S. formulated the research question, designed the study, collected and analysed the data, interpreted the findings and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. K. M. formulated the research question, designed the study, collected and analysed the data, interpreted the findings and provided critical input to the final draft of the manuscript. Both authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

This research was funded by a Grant-in-Aid for Research Activity Start-up (JSPS KAKENHI, Grant Number 20K23252) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this manuscript.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

The online version of this article has been updated since original publication. A notice detailing the change has been published at https://doi.org/10.1017/jns.2022.113.

References

Amoutzopoulos, B, Page, P, Roberts, C, et al. (2020) Portion size estimation in dietary assessment: a systematic review of existing tools, their strengths and limitations. Nutr Rev 78, 885900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almiron-Roig, E, Navas-Carretero, S, Emery, P, et al. (2018) Research into food portion size: methodological aspects and applications. Food Funct 9, 715739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutishauser, IH (2005) Dietary intake measurements. Public Health Nutr 8, 11001107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichelle, PG, Almeida, CCB, Camey, SA, et al. (2019) Subjects’ perception in quantifying printed and digital photos of food portions. Nutrients 11, 501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szenczi-Cseh, J, Horváth, Z & Ambrus, Á (2017) Validation of a food quantification picture book and portion sizes estimation applying perception and memory methods. Int J Food Sci Nutr 68, 960972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vlieger, NM, Weltert, M, Molenaar, A, et al. (2020) A systematic review of recall errors associated with portion size estimation aids in children. Appetite 147, 104522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharma, V & Chadha, R (2017) Effectiveness of food portion size estimation aids for diet assessment: a systematic review. Int J Food Sci Nutr 2, 106112.Google Scholar
Naska, A, Valanou, E, Peppa, E, et al. (2016) Evaluation of a digital food photography atlas used as portion size measurement aid in dietary surveys in Greece. Public Health Nutr 19, 23692376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacques, S, Lemieux, S, Lamarche, B, et al. (2016) Development of a web-based 24-h dietary recall for a French-Canadian population. Nutrients 8, 724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, SI, Potischman, N, Dodd, KW, et al. (2016) The use of digital images in 24-hour recalls may lead to less misestimation of portion size compared with traditional interviewer-administered recalls. J Nutr 146, 25672573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, E, Bradley, J, Poliakov, I, et al. (2017) Iterative development of an online dietary recall tool: INTAKE24. Nutrients 9, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, M, Atkinson, M & Darbyshire, S (1994) Food photography I: the perception of food portion size from photographs. Br J Nutr 72, 649663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, M, Atkinson, M & Darbyshire, S (1996) Food photography II: portion size and the nutrient content of meals. Br J Nutr 76, 3149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, E, Matthews, JNS, Lloyd, J, et al. (2008) Children's estimates of food portion size: the development and evaluation of three portion size assessment tools for use with children. Br J Nutr 99, 175184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, J, Rowland, MK, Matthews, JNS, et al. (2021) A comparison of food portion size estimation methods among 11–12 year olds: 3d food models vs an online tool using food portion photos (Intake24). BMC Nutr 7, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, E, Hawkins, A, Barton, KL, et al. (2017) Development of food photographs for use with children aged 18 months to 16 years: comparison against weighed food diaries – The Young Person's Food Atlas (UK). PLoS ONE 12, e0169084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robson, PJ & Livingstone, MBE (2000) An evaluation of food photographs as a tool for quantifying food and nutrient intakes. Public Health Nutr 3, 183192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vereecken, C, Dohogne, S, Covents, M, et al. (2010) How accurate are adolescents in portion-size estimation using the computer tool young adolescents’ nutrition assessment on computer (YANA-C)? Br J Nutr 103, 18441850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Keyzer, W, Huybrechts, I, de Maeyer, M, et al. (2011) Food photographs in nutritional surveillance: errors in portion size estimation using drawings of bread and photographs of margarine and beverages consumption. Br J Nutr 105, 10731083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lillegaard, ITL, Øverby, NC & Andersen, LF (2005) Can children and adolescents use photographs of food to estimate portion sizes? Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 611617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turconi, G, Guarcello, M, Berzolari, FG, et al. (2005) An evaluation of a colour food photography atlas as a tool for quantifying food portion size in epidemiological dietary surveys. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 923931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ovaskainen, ML, Paturi, M, Reinivuo, H, et al. (2008) Accuracy in the estimation of food servings against the portions in food photographs. Eur J Clin Nutr 62, 674681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trolle, E, Vandevijvere, S, Ruprich, J, et al. (2013) Validation of a food quantification picture book targeting children of 0–10 years of age for pan-European and national dietary surveys. Br J Nutr 110, 22982308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nikolić, M, Milešević, J, Zeković, M, et al. (2018) The development and validation of food atlas for portion size estimation in the Balkan Region. Front Nutr 5, 78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vilela, S, Lopes, C, Guiomar, S, et al. (2018) Validation of a picture book to be used in a pan-European dietary survey. Public Health Nutr 21, 16541663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frobisher, C & Maxwell, SM (2003) The estimation of food portion sizes: a comparison between using descriptions of portion sizes and a photographic food atlas by children and adults. J Hum Nutr Diet 16, 181188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nissinen, K, Korkalo, L, Vepsäläinen, H, et al. (2018) Accuracy in the estimation of children's food portion sizes against a food picture book by parents and early educators. J Nutr Sci 7, e35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biltoft-Jensen, A, Holmgaard Nielsen, T, Hess Ygil, K, et al. (2018) Accuracy of food photographs for quantifying food servings in a lunch meal setting among Danish children and adults. J Hum Nutr Diet 31, 131140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subar, AF, Crafts, J, Zimmerman, TP, et al. (2010) Assessment of the accuracy of portion size reports using computer-based food photographs aids in the development of an automated self-administered 24-hour recall. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 5564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baranowski, T, Baranowski, JC, Watson, KB, et al. (2011) Children's accuracy of portion size estimation using digital food images: effects of interface design and size of image on computer screen. Public Health Nutr 14, 418425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernández, T, Wilder, L, Kuehn, D, et al. (2006) Portion size estimation and expectation of accuracy. J Food Compost Anal 19, 1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernal-Orozco, MF, Vizmanos-Lamotte, B, Rodríguez-Rocha, NP, et al. (2013) Validation of a Mexican food photograph album as a tool to visually estimate food amounts in adolescents. Br J Nutr 109, 944952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, DA, Allen, HR, Martin, PD, et al. (2003) Comparison of digital photography to weighed and visual estimation of portion sizes. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 11391145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ali, HI, Platat, C, El Mesmoudi, N, et al. (2018) Evaluation of a photographic food atlas as a tool for quantifying food portion size in the United Arab Emirates. PLoS ONE 13, e0196389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tueni, M, Mounayar, A & Birlouez-Aragon, I (2012) Development and evaluation of a photographic atlas as a tool for dietary assessment studies in Middle East cultures. Public Health Nutr 15, 10231028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flax, VL, Thakwalakwa, C, Schnefke, CH, et al. (2019) Validation of a digitally displayed photographic food portion-size estimation aid among women in urban and rural Malawi. Public Health Nutr 22, 31403150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnefke, CH, Thakwalakwa, C, Muth, MK, et al. (2019) Optimizing portion-size estimation aids: a formative evaluation in Malawi. Public Health Nutr 22, 31273139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarte, CE, Encinas, ME, Alegre, C, et al. (2012) Validation of digital photographs, as a tool in 24-h recall, for the improvement of dietary assessment among rural populations in developing countries. Nutr J 11, 61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huybregts, L, Roberfroid, D, Lachat, C, et al. (2008) Validity of photographs for food portion estimation in a rural West African setting. Public Health Nutr 11, 581587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amougou, N, Cohen, E, Mbala, ML, et al. (2016) Development and validation of two food portion photograph books to assess dietary intake among adults and children in Central Africa. Br J Nutr 115, 895902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korkalo, L, Erkkola, M, Fidalgo, L, et al. (2013) Food photographs in portion size estimation among adolescent Mozambican girls. Public Health Nutr 16, 15581564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venter, CS, MacIntyre, UE & Vorster, HH (2000) The development and testing of a food portion photograph book for use in an African population. J Hum Nutr Diet 13, 205218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoradeniya, T, de Silva, A, Arambepola, C, et al. (2012) Portion size estimation aids for Asian foods. J Hum Nutr Diet 25, 497504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ding, Y, Yang, Y, Li, F, et al. (2021) Development and validation of a photographic atlas of food portions for accurate quantification of dietary intakes in China. J Hum Nutr Diet 34, 604615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris-Fry, H, Paudel, P, Karn, M, et al. (2016) Development and validation of a photographic food atlas for portion size assessment in the southern plains of Nepal. Public Health Nutr 19, 24952507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saeki, K, Otaki, N, Kitagawa, M, et al. (2020) Development and validation of nutrient estimates based on a food-photographic record in Japan. Nutr J 19, 104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almiron-Roig, E, Aitken, A, Galloway, C, et al. (2017) Dietary assessment in minority ethnic groups: a systematic review of instruments for portion-size estimation in the United Kingdom. Nutr Rev 75, 188213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shinozaki, N, Murakami, K, Asakura, K, et al. (2022) Development of a digital photographic food atlas as a portion size estimation aid in Japan. Nutrient 14, 2218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, E, O'Keeffe, M, Matthews, JNS, et al. (2008) Children's estimates of food portion size: the effect of timing of dietary interview on the accuracy of children's portion size estimates. Br J Nutr 99, 185190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucassen, DA, Willemsen, RF, Geelen, A, et al. (2021) The accuracy of portion size estimation using food images and textual descriptions of portion sizes: an evaluation study. J Hum Nutr Diet 34, 945952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, M & Haraldsdóttir, J (1998) Food photographs: practical guidelines I. Design and analysis of studies to validate portion size estimates. Public Health Nutr 1, 219230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shinozaki, N, Murakami, K, Asakura, K, et al. (2020) Identification of dish-based dietary patterns for breakfast, lunch, and dinner and their diet quality in Japanese adults. Nutrients 13, 67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, J, Simpson, E, Poliakov, I, et al. (2016) Comparison of INTAKE24 (an online 24-h dietary recall tool) with interviewer-led 24-hr recall in 11–24 year-old. Nutrients 8, 358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al Marzooqi, HM, Burke, SJ, Al Ghazali, MR, et al. (2015) The development of a food atlas of portion sizes for the United Arab Emirates. J Food Compost Anal 43, 140148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, M & Haraldsdóttir, J (1998) Food photographs: practical guidelines II. Development and use of photographic atlases for assessing food portion size. Public Health Nutr 1, 231237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin Bland, J & Altman, DG (1986) Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. The Lancet 327, 307310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montenij, LJ, Buhre, WF, Jansen, JR, et al. (2016) Methodology of method comparison studies evaluating the validity of cardiac output monitors: a stepwise approach and checklist. Br J Anaesth 116, 750758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafrenière, J, Lamarche, B, Laramée, C, et al. (2017) Validation of a newly automated web-based 24-hour dietary recall using fully controlled feeding studies. BMC Nutr 3, 34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, M, Atkinson, M & Darbyshire, S (1996) Food photography II: use of food photographs for estimating portion size and the nutrient content of meals. Br J Nutr 76, 3149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of foods and food photographs selected for this study

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the study.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Example screens from the web-based questionnaire, with English translation shown in the boxes. (a) Screenshot of the question to ask the type of cookies using a guide photograph and (b) screenshot of the question to ask the serving size of curry and rice using a series of photographs.

Figure 3

Table 2. Participant characteristics (n 54)

Figure 4

Table 3. Difference between the true and the estimated serving sizes of food and drink items (n 54)

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Bland–Altman plots assessing the agreement of the served and estimated food amounts in fifty-four Japanese adults: (a) a bowl of rice, (b) miso soup, (c) grilled mackerel, (d) simmered squash and (e) Japanese fried chicken. The solid line represents the mean difference, and the dotted line represents lower and upper limits of agreement with a solid regression line added.

Supplementary material: PDF

Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material

Figure S1

Download Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 2.7 MB
Supplementary material: PDF

Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material

Figure S2

Download Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.2 MB
Supplementary material: PDF

Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material

Figure S3

Download Shinozaki and Murakami supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.1 MB