Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T17:23:46.728Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Profiles of early family environments and the growth of executive function: Maternal sensitivity as a protective factor

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 September 2021

Seulki Ku*
Affiliation:
Department of Population Health, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Clancy Blair
Affiliation:
Department of Population Health, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Seulki Ku, 227 East 30th Street, New York, NY 10016, USA; E-mail: seulki.ku@nyulangone.org

Abstract

We identified family risk profiles at 6 months using socioeconomic status (SES) and maternal mental health indicators with data from the Family Life Project (N = 1,292). We related profiles to executive function (EF) at 36 months (intercept) and growth in EF between 36 and 60 months. Latent profile analysis revealed five distinct profiles, characterized by different combinations of SES and maternal mental health symptoms. Maternal sensitivity predicted faster growth in EF among children in the profile characterized by deep poverty and the absence of maternal mental health symptoms. Maternal sensitivity also predicted higher EF intercept but slower EF growth among children in the profile characterized by deep poverty and maternal mental health symptoms, and children in the near poor (low SES), mentally healthy profile. Maternal sensitivity also predicted higher EF intercept but had no effect on growth in EF in the near poor, mentally distressed profile. In contrast, maternal sensitivity did not predict the intercept or growth of EF in the privileged SES/mentally healthy profile. Our findings using a person-centered approach provide a more nuanced understanding of the role of maternal sensitivity in the growth of EF, such that maternal sensitivity may differentially affect the growth of EF in various contexts.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 7182. doi:10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashman, S., Dawson, G., & Panagiotides, H. (2008). Trajectories of maternal depression over 7 years: Relations with child psychophysiology and behavior and role of contextual risks. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 5577. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21, 329341. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azak, S. (2012). Maternal depression and sex differences shape the infants’ trajectories of cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 803814. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakk, Z., Tekle, F. B., & Vermunt, J. K. (2013). Estimating the association between latent class membership and external variables using bias-adjusted three-step approaches. Sociological Methodology, 43, 272311. doi:10.1177/0081175012470644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakk, Z., & Vermunt, J. K. (2016). Robustness of stepwise latent class modeling with continuous distal outcomes. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23, 2031. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.955104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 326339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindman, S. W., Hindman, A. H., Bowles, R. P., & Morrison, F. J. (2013). The contributions of parental management language to executive function in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 529539. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., … Fortunato, C. K. (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development, 82, 19701984. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., Kuzawa, C. W., & Willoughby, M. T. (2020). The development of executive function in early childhood is inversely related to change in body mass index: Evidence for an energetic tradeoff? Developmental Science, 23, e12860. doi:10.1111/desc.12860CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, R. H. (1994). The home inventory: Review and reflections. In Reese, H. W. (Ed.), Advances in child behavior and development (Vol. 25, pp. 241288). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brandes-Aitken, A., Braren, S., Gandhi, J., Perry, R. E., Rowe-Harriott, S., & Blair, C. (2020). Joint attention partially mediates the longitudinal relation between attuned caregiving and executive functions for low-income children. Developmental Psychology, 56, 18291841. doi:10.1037/dev0001089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 602654. doi:10.1037/a0038662CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brophy-Herb, H. E., Martoccio, T. L., Hillaker, B., Stansbury, K. E., Harewood, T., Senehi, N., & Fitzgerald, H. (2013). Profiles of low-income maternal well-being and family climate: Relations to toddler boys’ and girls’ behaviors. Family Relations, 62, 326340. doi:10.1111/fare.12001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, C., Davis, E. P., Hobel, C. J., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Maternal pregnancy-specific anxiety is associated with child executive function at 6–9 years age. Stress, 14, 665676. doi:10.3109/10253890.2011.623250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Matestic, P., von Stauffenberg, C., Mohan, R., & Kirchner, T. (2007). Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and children's functioning at school entry. Developmental Psychology, 43, 12021215. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clavarino, A. M., Mamun, A. A., O'Callaghan, M., Aird, R., Bor, W., O'Callaghan, F., & Alati, R. (2010). Maternal anxiety and attention problems in children at 5 and 14 years. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 658667. doi:10.1177/1087054709347203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Comas, M., Valentino, K., & Borkowski, J. G. (2014). Maternal depressive symptoms and child temperament: Longitudinal associations with executive functioning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 156167. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2014.03.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1992). A developmental and clinical model for the prevention of conduct disorder: The FAST track program. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 509527. doi:10.1017/S0954579400004855CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting to change in rural America. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Cooper, B. R., & Lanza, S. T. (2014). Who benefits most from head start? Using latent class moderation to examine differential treatment effects. Child Development, 85, 23172338. doi:10.1111/cdev.12278Google Scholar
Cox, M., & Crnic, K. (2002). Qualitative ratings for parent-child interaction at 3–12 months of age. Unpublished manuscript. Chapel Hill, NC: Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Crouter, A. C., Lanza, S. T., Pirretti, A., Goodman, W. B., Neebe, E., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2006). The O*Net jobs classification system: A primer for family researchers. Family Relations, 55, 461472. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00415.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, L. (2000). Brief Symptom Inventory 18. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dill, B. T., & Myers, S. L. (2004). Rediscovering rural America. In Blau, J. R. (Ed.), Blackwell companion to sociology (pp. 196210). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 430457. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., Ricciuti, H. N., Hope, S., Schoon, I., Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., & Hazan, C. (2010). Crowding and cognitive development: The mediating role of maternal responsiveness among 36-month-old children. Environment and Behavior, 42, 135148. doi:10.1177/0013916509333509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 258264. doi:10.1111/cdep.12095CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. (2018). Postnatal anxiety prevalence, predictors and effects on development: A narrative review. Infant Behavior and Development, 51, 2432. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Diego, M. (2006). Intrusive and withdrawn depressed mothers and their infants. Developmental Review, 26, 1530. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2005.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, S. L., Scholnick, E. K., Bender, R. H., Vandergrift, N., Spieker, S., Hirsh Pasek, K., … NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2014). Planning in middle childhood: Early predictors and later outcomes. Child Development, 85, 14461460. doi:10.1111/cdev.12221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 3160. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, K., McMahon, C., Reilly, N., & Austin, M. (2010). Maternal sensitivity moderates the impact of prenatal anxiety disorder on infant mental development. Early Human Development, 86, 551556. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gueron-Sela, N., Camerota, M., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Cox, M. J. (2018). Maternal depressive symptoms, mother–child interactions, and children's executive function. Developmental Psychology, 54, 7182. doi:10.1037/dev0000389.suppCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Hurt, H., & Farah, M. J. (2012). Neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent stress reactivity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 277. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Gallop, R., Romer, D., Brodsky, N. L., Hurt, H., & Farah, M. J. (2014). Mapping the trajectory of socioeconomic disparity in working memory: Parental and neighborhood factors. Child Development, 85, 14331445. doi:10.1111/cdev.12242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 6573. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18, 686702. doi:10.1111/desc.12246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Robert, S. A., Grübel, J., Weibel, R. P., Anagnostou, E., Hölscher, C., & Schinazi, V. R. (2019). Neighborhood environments influence emotion and physiological reactivity. Scientific Reports, 9, 9498. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-45876-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, E., Feng, X., Christian, L., & Slesnick, N. (2015). Emotion expression, emotionality, depressive symptoms, and stress: Maternal profiles related to child outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 13191331. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0019-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2011). Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 664676. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2010). Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 2036. doi:10.1080/87565640903325691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Roman, G., Hart, M. J., & Ensor, R. (2013). Does maternal depression predict young children's executive function? – A 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 169177. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iceland, J., & Bauman, K. J. (2007). Income poverty and material hardship: How strong is the association? Journal of Socio-Economics, 36, 376396. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2006.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The flexible item selection task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 573591. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2003_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jedidi, K., Ramaswamy, V., & Desarbo, W. S. (1993). A maximum likelihood method for latent class regression involving a censored dependent variable. Psychometrika, 58, 375394. doi:10.1007/BF02294647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. A. S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 39, 302317. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J., & Charney, D. (2000). Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 12, 6976. doi:10.1002/1520-6394(2000)12:1+<69::AID-DA9>3.0.CO;2-K3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiernan, K. E., & Huerta, M. C. (2008). Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children's cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. The British Journal of Sociology, 59, 783806. doi:10.1111/j.1468-4446.2008.00219.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluczniok, D., Boedeker, K., Fuchs, A., Hindi, A. C., Fydrich, T., Fuehrer, D., … Bermpohl, F. (2016). Emotional availability in mother–child interaction: The effects of maternal depression in remission and additional history of childhood abuse. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 648657. doi:10.1002/da.22462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ku, S., & Feng, X. (2021). Maternal depressive symptoms and the growth of child executive function: Mediation by maternal sensitivity. Journal of Family Psychology, Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/fam0000832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ku, S., Feng, X., Hooper, E. G., Wu, Q., & Gerhardt, M. (2019). Interactions between familial risk profiles and preschoolers’ emotionality in predicting executive function. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 63, 7686. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2019.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Blair, C. B. (2016). The contribution of children's time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 2034. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanza, S. T., Tan, X., & Bray, B. C. (2013). Latent class analysis with distal outcomes: A flexible model-based approach. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20, 126. doi:10.1080/10705511.2013.742377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y., Kaaya, S., Chai, J., McCoy, D., Surkan, P., Black, M., … Smith-Fawzi, M. (2017). Maternal depressive symptoms and early childhood cognitive development: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 47, 680689. doi:10.1017/S003329171600283XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767778. doi:10.1093/biomet/88.3.767CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manning, L. G., Davies, P. T., & Cicchetti, D. (2014). Interparental violence and childhood adjustment: How and why maternal sensitivity is a protective factor. Child Development, 85, 22632278. doi:10.1111/cdev.12279Google ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425444. doi:10.1017/S0954579400005812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masyn, K. (2013). Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In Little, T. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of quantitative methods (pp. 551611). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McCoy, D. C., Raver, C. C., & Sharkey, P. (2015). Children's cognitive performance and selective attention following recent community violence. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 56, 1936. doi:10.1177/0022146514567576CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Centifanti, L. C. M., Fernyhough, C., & Fishburn, S. (2013). Maternal mind-mindedness and children's behavioral difficulties: Mitigating the impact of low socioeconomic status. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 543553. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9699-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 13731386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gariepy, J., Sutton, K., & Cox, M. J. (2008). Changes in maternal sensitivity across the first three years: Are mothers from different attachment dyads differentially influenced by depressive symptomatology? Attachment & Human Development, 10, 299317. doi:10.1080/14616730802113612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, B. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth mixture modeling: Comment on Bauer and Curran (2003). Psychological Methods, 8, 369377. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, B. O., & Muthén, L. K. (2000). Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 882891. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user's guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1994). Updating job prestige and socioeconomic scores: How the new measures measure up. Sociological Methodology, 24, 172. doi:10.2307/270978CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology, 35, 12971310. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicol-Harper, R., Harvey, A. G., & Stein, A. (2007). Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 161167. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.08.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 7487. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 535569. doi:10.1080/10705510701575396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, E. B., & Shaw, D. S. (2003). Predicting growth curves of externalizing behavior across the preschool years. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 575590. doi:10.1023/A:1026254005632CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxford, M. L., & Lee, J. O. (2011). The effect of family processes on school achievement as moderated by socioeconomic context. Journal of School Psychology, 49, 597612. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2011.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, M. E., McClelland, M. M., Swanson, J., & Lipscomb, S. T. (2016). Family risk profiles and school readiness: A person-centered approach. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 462474. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raver, C. C., Blair, C., Willoughby, M., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2013). Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds’ executive function: New perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Developmental Psychology, 49, 292304. doi:10.1037/a0028343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinecke, J. (2006). Longitudinal analysis of adolescents’ deviant and delinquent behavior. Methodology, 2, 100112. doi:10.1027/1614-2241.2.3.100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., & Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 638662. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rief, W., Hennings, A., Riemer, S., & Euteneuer, F. (2010). Psychobiological differences between depression and somatization. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 68, 495502. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6, 461464. Retrieved from www.jstor.org/stable/2958889Google Scholar
Stein, A., Malmberg, L., Sylva, K., Barnes, J., & Leach, P. (2008). The influence of maternal depression, caregiving, and socioeconomic status in the post-natal year on children's language development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34, 603612. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00837.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 122128. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00209.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis, 18, 450469. doi:10.1093/pan/mpq025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernon-Feagans, L., Cox, M., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2013). The Family Life Project: An epidemiological and developmental study of young children living in poor rural communities. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78, 1150, vii. doi:10.1111/mono.12046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuong, Q. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-nested hypotheses. Econometrica, 57, 307333. doi:10.2307/1912557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, F., Christ, S. L., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Garrett-Peters, P., & Cox, M. J. (2013). Association between maternal sensitivity and externalizing behavior from preschool to preadolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34, 89100. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., Blair, C. B., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2012). Executive function in early childhood: Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological Assessment, 24, 418431. doi:10.1037/a002577CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Ku and Blair supplementary material

Ku and Blair supplementary material

Download Ku and Blair supplementary material(File)
File 29.3 KB