Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T06:41:54.465Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hot and cool executive function in the development of behavioral problems in grade school

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2024

Yiji Wang*
Affiliation:
Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Psychological Crisis Intervention, Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at New York University Shanghai, Shanghai, China Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai, China
Huayu Ji
Affiliation:
Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Psychological Crisis Intervention, Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at New York University Shanghai, Shanghai, China Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai, China
*
Corresponding author: Yiji Wang; Email: yjwang@psy.ecnu.edu.cn

Abstract

Despite the well-established link between children’s executive function and behavioral adjustment, it remains unclear whether the hot and cool aspects of executive function are uniquely associated with children’s behavioral problems. Using longitudinal data spanning in the grade school (N = 1,140), this study aimed to examine whether hot and cool executive function skills may be uniquely related to the development of behavioral problems. Hot and cool executive function skills were measured with tasks, standardized tests, and questionnaires at 54 months and in the first grade, respectively. Internalizing and externalizing problems were evaluated by teachers using questionnaires throughout the grade school. The results indicated that, independent of each other, hot and cool executive function skills were uniquely and negatively related to the development of internalizing and externalizing problems over time at the between-individual level, adjusting for within-individual fluctuations. Moreover, internalizing and externalizing problems were positively related at the between-individual level across the grade school. Findings provide needed evidence to clarify the relations between hot and cool executive function and children’s behavioral problems, emphasizing the importance of both aspects of executive function in understanding the development of behavioral problems in school-age children.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the teacher’s report form and 1991 profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Rescorla, L. A., Turner, L. V., & Althoff, R. R. (2016). Internalizing/externalizing problems: Review and recommendations for clinical and research applications. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(8), 647656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Ahmed, S. F., Tang, S., Waters, N. E., & Davis-Kean, P. (2019). Executive function and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations from early childhood to adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 446458. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000296 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Exploring dimensionality of effortful control using hot and cool tasks in a sample of preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 3347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 3040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20(5), 343350. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043220 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blain-Arcaro, C., & Vaillancourt, T. (2019). Longitudinal associations between externalizing problems and symptoms of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 48(1), 108119. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1270830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2008). Serotonergic function, two-mode models of self-regulation, and vulnerability to depression: What depression has in common with impulsive aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 912943. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013740 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831844. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121383 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Commisso, M., Temcheff, C., Orri, M., Poirier, M., Lau, M., Côté, S., Vitaro, F., Turecki, G., Tremblay, R., & Geoffroy, M.-C. (2023). Childhood externalizing, internalizing and comorbid problems: Distinguishing young adults who think about suicide from those who attempt suicide. Psychological Medicine, 53(3), 10301037. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002464 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 97104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danneel, S., Nelemans, S., Spithoven, A., Bastin, M., Bijttebier, P., Colpin, H., Van Den Noortgate, W., Van Leeuwen, K., Verschueren, K., & Goossens, L. (2019). Internalizing problems in adolescence: Linking loneliness, social anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(10), 16911705. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00539-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deighton, J., Humphrey, N., Belsky, J., Boehnke, J., Vostanis, P., & Patalay, P. (2018). Longitudinal pathways between mental health difficulties and academic performance during middle childhood and early adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 110126. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denham, S. A., Warren-Khot, H. K., Bassett, H. H., Wyatt, T., & Perna, A. (2012). Factor structure of self-regulation in preschoolers: Testing models of a field-based assessment for predicting early school readiness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 386404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.10.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959964. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duprey, E. B., Oshri, A., & Liu, S. (2020). Developmental pathways from child maltreatment to adolescent suicide-related behaviors: The internalizing and externalizing comorbidity hypothesis. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 945959. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000919 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 495525. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eslinger, P. J., Flaherty-Craig, C. V., & Benton, A. L. (2004). Developmental outcomes after early prefrontal cortex damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 84103. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00281-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanti, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. (2010). Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: Findings from the national institute of child health and human development study of early child care. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 11591175. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020659 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Nettle, D. (2020). The strengths of people in poverty. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 1621. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419881154 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. S. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16(02), 313333. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404044530 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. W. (2003). Adding missing-data-relevant variables to FIML-based structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 10(1), 80100. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutman, L. M., & Codiroli McMaster, N. (2020). Gendered pathways of internalizing problems from early childhood to adolescence and associated adolescent outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48(5), 703718. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00623-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, J. F. (2015). Increases in maternal education and low-income children’s cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 583599. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038920 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatoum, A. S., Rhee, S. H., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., & Friedman, N. P. (2018). Do executive functions explain the covariance between internalizing and externalizing behaviors? Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 13711387. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilt, L. M., Armstrong, J. M., & Essex, M. J. (2017). Rumination and moderators of multifinality: Predicting internalizing symptoms and alcohol use during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 46(5), 746753. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1070354 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostinar, C. E., & Cicchetti, D. (2020). Emotion dysregulation and internalizing spectrum disorders. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Crowell, S. E. (Eds.), The oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 249263). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huang-Pollock, C., Shapiro, Z., Galloway-Long, H., & Weigard, A. (2017). Is poor working memory a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 14771490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0219-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, J. S., Mezulis, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (2008). The ABCs of depression: Integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychological Review, 115(2), 291313. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iselin, A.-M. R., McVey, A. A., & Ehatt, C. M. (2016). Externalizing behaviors and attribution biases. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), The oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders (pp. 347359). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keskin, G., Lougheed, J. P., & Duncan, R. (2022). Within- and between-person associations among internalizing and externalizing problems during middle childhood. Social Development, 32(3), 889904. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12660 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children’s behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1), 4356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lensing, N., & Elsner, B. (2018). Development of hot and cool executive functions in middle childhood: Three-year growth curves of decision making and working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 187204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, C., Hart, E. R., Duncan, R. J., & Watts, T. W. (2023). Bi-directional relations between behavioral problems and executive function: Assessing the longitudinal development of self-regulation. Developmental Science, 26(3), e13331. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeilly, E. A., Peverill, M., Jung, J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2021). Executive function as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status to internalizing and externalizing psychopathology in children and adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 89(1), 149160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.04.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehsen, V., Morag, L., Chesta, S., Cleaton, K., & Burgos, H. (2022). Hot executive function assessment instruments in preschool children: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(1), 95. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19010095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller‐Cotto, D., Smith, L. V., Wang, A. H., & Ribner, A. D. (2022). Changing the conversation: A culturally responsive perspective on executive functions, minoritized children and their families. Infant and Child Development, 31(1), e2286. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2286 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1992). Delay of gratification in children. In Loewenstein, G., & Elster, J. (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 147164). Rusell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montroy, J. J., Merz, E. C., Williams, J. M., Landry, S. H., Johnson, U. Y., Zucker, T. A., Assel, M., Taylor, H. B., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Barnes, M. A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T., Valiente, C., de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (2019). Hot and cool dimensionality of executive function: Model invariance across age and maternal education in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 49, 188201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.06.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perou, R., Bitsko, R. H., Blumberg, S. J., Pastor, P., Ghandour, R. M., Gfroerer, J. C., Hedden, S. L., Crosby, A. E., Visser, S. N., Schieve, L. A., Parks, S. E., Hall, J. E., Brody, D., Simile, C. M., Thompson, W. W., Baio, J., Avenevoli, S., Kogan, M. D., Huang, L. N., & Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2013).Mental health surveillance among children–United States, 2005-2011. Morbidity and Mortality weekly Report, 62(2), 135.Google ScholarPubMed
Moriguchi, Y. (2021). Relationship between cool and hot executive function in young children: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Developmental Science, 25(2), e13165. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2017). Mplus user’s guide. (8th ed.), Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Nelson, T. D., Kidwell, K. M., Nelson, J. M., Tomaso, C. C., Hankey, M., & Espy, K. A. (2018). Preschool executive control and internalizing symptoms in elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(7), 15091520. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0395-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 12971310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1297 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2003). Do children’s attention processes mediate the link between family predictors and school readiness? Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 581593. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.581 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nivard, M. G., Lubke, G. H., Dolan, C. V., Evans, D. M., St. Pourcain, B., Munafo, M. R., & Middeldorp, C. M. (2017). Joint developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing disorders between childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 919928. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000572 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otero, T. M., & Barker, L. A. (2014). The frontal lobes and executive functioning. In Goldstein, S., & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 2944). Springer New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, E., & Welsh, M. C. (2014). The development of hot and cool executive functions in childhood and adolescence: Are we getting warmers?. In Goldstein, S., & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of executive function (pp. 4565). Springer Science+Business Media.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 621637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rea-Sandin, G., Clifford, S., Doane, L. D., Davis, M. C., Grimm, K. J., Russell, M. T., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2023). Genetic and environmental links between executive functioning and effortful control in middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(3), 780793. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001298 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 1129. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 2139.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2013). Developmental psychopathology: A paradigm shift or just a relabeling? Development and Psychopathology, 25(4), 12011213. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000564 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 265296. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, H., Daseking, M., Gawrilow, C., Karbach, J., & Kerner auch Koerner, J. (2022). Self-regulation in preschool: Are executive function and effortful control overlapping constructs? Developmental Science, 25(6), e13272. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13272 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, Q., & Ettekal, I. (2021). Co-occurring trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems from grades 1 to 12: Longitudinal associations with teacher-child relationship quality and academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(4), 808829. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000525 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1992). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(1), 1523. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulik, M. J., Huerta, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Giunta, L. D., Pina, A. A., Eggum, N. D., Sallquist, J., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Wilson, S. B., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Taylor, H. B. (2010). The factor structure of effortful control and measurement invariance across ethnicity and sex in a high-risk sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(1), 822. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-009-9164-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thöne, A.-K., Junghänel, M., Görtz-Dorten, A., Dose, C., Hautmann, C., Jendreizik, L. T., Treier, A.-K., Vetter, P., von Wirth, E., Banaschewski, T., Becker, K., Brandeis, D., Dürrwächter, U., Geissler, J., Hebebrand, J., Hohmann, S., Holtmann, M., Huss, M., Jans, T., …Döpfner, M. (2021). Disentangling symptoms of externalizing disorders in children using multiple measures and informants. Psychological Assessment, 33(11), 10651079. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001053 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von der Embse, N., Kim, E., Ross, D., Kilgus, S., & Koza, T. (2023). Multi-informant assessment of internalizing concerns: Rater concordance and implications for decision-making. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 45(1), 234246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-023-10026-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., & Liu, Y. (2021). The development of internalizing and externalizing problems in primary school: Contributions of executive function and social competence. Child Development, 92(3), 889903. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., & Zhou, X. (2019). Longitudinal relations between executive function and internalizing problems in grade school: The role of peer difficulty and academic performance. Developmental Psychology, 55(10), 21472158. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000790 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, H., & Luciana, M. (2022). Neurobehavioral maturation of motor response inhibition in adolescence—A narrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 137, 104646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104646 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willner, C. J., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., & Bray, B. C. (2016). The dynamics of internalizing and externalizing comorbidity across the early school years. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 10331052. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000687 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M., Kupersmidt, J., Voegler-Lee, M., & Bryant, D. (2011). Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(2), 162180. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.549980 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-johnson psycho-educational battery-revised. DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2020). Executive function and psychopathology: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 431454. https://doi.org/ CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar