Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T02:59:02.238Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Caregiving relationships are a cornerstone of developmental psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2024

Kathryn L. Humphreys*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Julia Garon-Bissonnette
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Kaylin E. Hill
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Lauren G. Bailes
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Whitney Barnett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Megan M. Hare
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kathryn L. Humphreys; Email: k.humphreys@vanderbilt.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The interdisciplinary field of developmental psychopathology has made great strides by including context into theoretical and empirical approaches to studying risk and resilience. Perhaps no context is more important to the developing child than their relationships with their caregivers (typically a child’s parents), as caregivers are a key source of stimulation and nurturance to young children. Coupled with the high degree of brain plasticity in the earliest years of life, these caregiving relationships have an immense influence on shaping behavioral outcomes relevant to developmental psychopathology. In this article, we discuss three areas within caregiving relationships: (1) caregiver–child interactions in everyday, naturalistic settings; (2) caregivers’ social cognitions about their child; and (3) caregivers’ broader social and cultural context. For each area, we provide an overview of its significance to the field, identify existing knowledge gaps, and offer potential approaches for bridging these gaps to foster growth in the field. Lastly, given that one value of a scientific discipline is its ability to produce research useful in guiding real-world decisions related to policy and practice, we encourage developmental psychopathology to consider that a focus on caregiving, a modifiable target, supports this mission.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The field of developmental psychopathology has experienced tremendous growth and acceptance over the past 50 years. It has been interdisciplinary from the start, pushing against long-standing beliefs about the origin of mental illness (Rutter & Werker, Reference Rutter and Werker2021). Though there are many aspects that differentiate the field and approaches of developmental psychopathology from related disciplines (e.g., child clinical psychology, child and adolescent psychiatry), perhaps one of the most important distinguishing aspects is the explicit focus on an individual’s context as a key feature in shaping the onset and progression of psychological difficulties (and thriving) (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1993). From Cicchetti’s perspective, developmental psychopathology has also led in efforts to consider how context shapes an individual’s development (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti, Curtis and Cicchetti2006; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Hartup and Weinberg2002; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010).

Given the importance of context, as well as its broad nature, the question is what “context” most merits our attention. Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory (Reference Bronfenbrenner1977) outlines a method to consider individual development within interconnected environmental systems (for a brief review of additional theories regarding contextual influences on development, see Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998). However, it is plausible that greater weight may be given to those systems that, on average, have the greatest influence on psychopathology and thriving over time. Here, we argue that the context with the greatest potential for impact is caregiving relationships. This stance is far from controversial. Given the plasticity of the developing brain early in life and the reliance of infants and young children on their adult caregivers (often, though not exclusively, their biological parents) for survival, it stands to reason that relationship quality (and quantity) should be a central feature of future research in developmental psychopathology.

Related fields more clearly embrace this target. For example, in the interdisciplinary field of infant and early childhood mental health (IECMH) (Zeanah & Zeanah, Reference Zeanah, Zeanah and Zeanah2018), their primary diagnostic manual (DC: 0–5; Zero to Three, 2016) allows for diagnoses to be made at the level of the relationship. In fact, DC: 0–5 includes a diagnostic category termed “Relationship Specific Disorder of Early Childhood” in order to capture psychopathology that is evident within a specific caregiver–child pair and not present when the child is with other caregivers. Diagnosing pathology that exists “between” individuals, rather than “within” the child, is unique to DC: 0–5 relative to other diagnostic manuals (e.g., Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [American Psychiatric Association, 2013]; International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems [World Health Organization, 2019]) and may stem from the developmental and theoretical perspectives of the IEMCH field on caregiving relationships. Such an approach acknowledges that caregiving relationships are (1) critically important to children, (2) can be pathological, and (3) are specific to the relational partners involved.

Most developmental psychopathologists would not be comfortable categorizing disorders on the basis of their hypothesized etiology (e.g., originating from within an individual or from a specific relationship), given that etiology is typically considered as an ongoing transaction between genetic and environmental factors (Cicchetti & Lynch, Reference Cicchetti and Lynch1993). The degree to which children evoke caregiving behaviors typically considered to be promotive of positive outcomes and risk factors for negative outcomes has been documented in both genetically informed (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Harden and Tucker-Drob2016; Harold et al., Reference Harold, Leve, Barrett, Elam, Neiderhiser, Natsuaki, Shaw, Reiss and Thapar2013; Klahr et al., Reference Klahr, Thomas, Hopwood, Klump and Burt2013; Tucker-Drob & Harden, Reference Tucker-Drob and Harden2012) and experimental research (Pelham et al., Reference Pelham, Lang, Atkeson, Murphy, Gnagy, Greiner, Vodde-Hamilton and Greenslade1997). Considering characteristics and behaviors of the caregiver, the child, and of their interactions across time is ideal for studies examining mechanisms and pathways to, and from, psychopathology. However, if we believe that understanding risk and resilience is more than just an academic exercise, our work should strive to inform practice and policy. Considering levers of change may take priority over explanation, if a goal is to translate research to real-world decision to promote adaptive child and family functioning, especially with regard to what to do, when, and with whom. As such, prioritizing specific targets that encompass a transactional element most open to change (i.e., caregiving) may be a more practical approach for a field seeking to both understand and aid the subjects of study.

The knowledge of the role of caregiving relationships, particularly in the early developmental stages of infants and children, stems from extensive research conducted over decades. This research is interdisciplinary, drawing insights from IECMH, developmental science, neuroscience, human development, family studies, and developmental psychopathology. These fields are now enriched through collaborations with specialists in computer science, electrical engineering, and artificial intelligence, all of which offer innovative tools to test and refine theoretical frameworks. With this, the execution of this research often presents complex challenges. These include working with unique populations, conducting comprehensive longitudinal studies, designing and implementing interventions, employing diverse assessment methodologies, and coordinating information from various sources associated with each child. Often, research studies integrate several of these challenging elements.

We are grateful for the collective research efforts that amassed the evidence that demonstrates that caregiving is a cornerstone of developmental psychopathology. This research establishes a basis for further investigation that could clarify the factors influencing children’s developmental trajectories and pinpoint effective interventions to nurture the conditions that allow children and their caregivers to thrive. A focus on caregiving is also relevant for those specifically concerned with adversity and trauma, given that caregivers are the most commons perpetrators of child maltreatment (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2023) and that caregiving behaviors that promote secure attachment relationships mitigate the risk for psychopathology following exposure to traumatic events (Feldman & Vengrober, Reference Feldman and Vengrober2011). A note before proceeding: although relationships with our caregivers are important throughout development, the physical dependence on caregivers in early life makes infancy and early childhood stages particularly important. Thus, we consider the first years of life (e.g., prenatal through the preschool-age years) to be the ideal target for this focus of research. In this article, we discuss three areas pertinent to the advancement of developmental psychopathology (for a conceptual model, see Figure 1):

Figure 1. A conceptual model of relevant domains for the study of caregiving relationships. Note. While we acknowledge domains are bidirectional in influence, single headed arrows were selected in order to indicate the hypothesized direction of most interest. The child’s effect on caregiver social cognitions is proposed to be not static, but rather change as a function of experiences with the child and the child’s growing competencies and individual characteristics. Figure created in BioRender.com.

  1. 1. Assessment of caregiver–child interactions in naturalistic settings – “in the wild,” or the environments where children reside and grow, such as homes and community spaces. This approach allows us to examine both the quality and frequency of caregiving interactions within the context of the child’s daily life.

  2. 2. Understanding caregivers’ social cognitions, particularly regarding their perceptions of their children’s mental states, their own roles as caregivers, and the dynamics of their relationship with the child. These cognitive frameworks are critical and malleable factors that influence caregiving behaviors in daily interactions.

  3. 3. Influences of caregiver context on functioning. Caregivers possess multiple roles and identities beyond their connection to the child and are shaped by various factors such as partner relationships, the robustness of social networks, government and workplace policies, and culture. We consider how these contextual elements inform caregiver cognitions and behaviors, which are essential considerations in developmental psychopathology.

For each domain, we provide a concise overview of its essence and significance to developmental psychopathology, identify existing knowledge gaps, and propose approaches to bridge these gaps.

Assessment of caregiver–child interactions in naturalistic settings

Definition and value of everyday interactions

Early caregiving experiences set the foundation for developmental processes that unfold across the lifespan (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1982). According to attachment theory and the organizational perspective on development, everyday interactions between a caregiver and child have cascading effects on attachment and other developmental processes (Doyle & Cicchetti, Reference Doyle and Cicchetti2017). Specifically, everyday interactions are characterized as social exchanges between the caregiver and child, and include the frequency, duration, and quality of these exchanges. Interactions occur over short bursts of time (e.g., microlevel interactions occurring within seconds) or can be characterized as long-standing patterns that accumulate over years (macrolevel) (Ram & Gerstorf, Reference Ram and Gerstorf2009). Assessing everyday interactions enables researchers to gain a snapshot of microlevel interactions that, over time, may represent the longer-term macro view of the relationship.

Laboratory assessments of caregiver–child interactions are the field standard, given the complexity of naturalistic environments and value of standardizing assessment methods. In laboratory assessments, the observations tend to be relatively brief in duration (∼5-45 minutes) and either feature behavioral instructions for the caregiver to act in a prespecified manner (e.g., be non-responsive to the infant during episodes of the Still Face Paradigm; Tronick et al., Reference Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise and Brazelton1979) or to play with their child as they typically would (e.g., during “free play” interactions where a set of toys are made available to a caregiver–child pair; see Baldwin et al., Reference Baldwin, Baldwin and Cole1982). One value of these highly controlled settings is that variations in caregiving interactions are observed free from other external distractions that can influence the engagement of the participants (e.g., preparing meals, phones and tablets, other children). In addition, laboratory tasks allow researchers to conduct experiments that test specific research questions (e.g., how do caregiver–child dyads navigate more challenging tasks, including observing how caregivers instruct and support children to independently “clean up” appealing toys [see Kochanska & Aksan, Reference Kochanska and Aksan1995], or how the dyad responds to a brief separation [see Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978; Crowell & Feldman, Reference Crowell and Feldman1988]). Highly controlled lab paradigms have provided profound insight into developmental processes, including identifying the quality of interactions that precede the development of secure attachments (e.g., Braungart-Rieker et al., Reference Braungart-Rieker, Zentall, Lickenbrock, Ekas, Oshio and Planalp2014).

Knowledge gaps

As with any method, there are limitations to what can be gained via laboratory observations. First, behaviors observed in the laboratory capture the child’s experience with that caregiver under very specific confines. The caregiver is often acutely aware of being observed either in real-time by members of the research team or through cameras recording the interactions. Further, the removal of the necessary household tasks, phones, and other children that may otherwise pull the caregiver’s attention are likely meaningful moderators of the behaviors observed in the laboratory (e.g., McDaniel, Reference McDaniel2019), which may constitute the caregiver’s “best case scenario” of behavior. In addition, the observation is brief, and the degree to which the quality of behavior can be maintained over time is not typically tested, as social interactions are taxing energetically. In a study of mothers with and without sleep disturbances, those with poor sleep continuity demonstrated a notable decline in sensitivity across a 10-minute free play with their 18-week-old infants (King et al., Reference King, Rangel, Simpson, Tikotzky and Manber2020). Further, this higher effort during observations is evident from comparisons of caregiver behavior in these settings compared to daylong observations. Caregivers are likely to speak to their infants throughout the entire duration of a laboratory task, however 20–30% of everyday activities that infants engage in do not contain any adult speech (Cristia et al., Reference Cristia, Lavechin, Scaff, Soderstrom, Rowland, Räsänen, Bunce and Bergelson2021; Tamis-LeMonda et al., Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko, Luo, Escobar and Bornstein2017). Taken together, although we know that laboratory assessments can provide insight into developmental processes, it is still unclear the degree to which the behaviors observed in the lab map on to what the child experiences in terms of caregiving in their everyday life. Although there is some recent work examining the role of caregiving “in the wild” in developmental processes (e.g., Madden-Rusnak et al., Reference Madden-Rusnak, Micheletti, Dominguez and de Barbaro2023; de Barbaro et al., Reference de Barbaro, Khante, Maier and Goodman2023), we know little about the level (and impact) of variation in caregiving (e.g., at home vs. at daycare; during meals vs. at bedtime; with mom vs. dad vs. when both are present; and in other combinations) on children’s functioning.

Recommendations for next steps

Infants' daily experiences encompass a wide array of interactions with various objects, individuals, and activities across settings. The deployment of tools to document these experiences can significantly enhance our understanding of how actions, proximity, tactile engagements, and exposure to varying environments influence child development (de Barbaro & Fausey, Reference de Barbaro and Fausey2022). Researchers may pursue these questions by considering first-person recordings in naturalistic contexts. Advances in wearable technology, such as the Looxcie 2 camera, which fits over one of the adult wearer’s ears – in this case, the caregiver – like a pair of glasses (e.g., Jayaraman et al., Reference Jayaraman, Fausey and Smith2015; Teti et al., Reference Teti, Kim, Mayer and Countermine2010), offer innovative ways to record infants' interactions without the discomfort associated with traditional head-mounted devices. Audio recording tools like the Language Environment Analysis system (Xu et al., Reference Xu, Yapanel and Gray2009), in which the device is typically housed in a wearable vest, gathers audio data from the infant’s environment, including speech and background sounds. Their “black box” algorithms produce estimates of adult words, conversation turns, and child vocalizations, offering the potential to use the data to obtain information such as which conversational partner initiated a conversation, and whether a greater number of adult- vs. infant-initiated conversations better explain children’s later expressive and receptive language (Salo, King et al., Reference Salo, King, Gotlib and Humphreys2022). Open-source tools that allow investigators to explore and manipulate the algorithms used are ideal for promoting collaboration and allowing for innovations that improve measurement.

Advances in wearable technologies also allow for new areas of inquiry. Specifically, physical proximity is an important aspect of the caregiver–child relationship (see Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Hansen, Bailes and Humphreys2022), and can be recorded via touch and proximal distance recordings (Brzozowska et al., Reference Brzozowska, Longo, Mareschal, Wiesemann and Gliga2021; Salo, Pannuto et al., Reference Salo, Pannuto, Hedgecock, Biri, Russo, Piersiak and Humphreys2022). Such recordings provide valuable data on infants’ movement, spatial relations, and tactile interactions. Our team developed the TotTag (run using the SociTrack platform [Biri et al., Reference Biri, Jackson, Thiele, Dutta and Pannuto2020]), which employs time-of-flight technology to record continuous distance measurements between all individuals in a network. Importantly, this means that each pairwise relationship (not only mother–infant, for example) is captured. For example, in the case of TotTags worn by all members of a family of five, proximity information is produced for ten pairwise relationships. Data analysis options include examining combinations of close contact and considering the family unit’s patterns of proximity as a network. Critical aspects of caregiver–child relationship dynamics, including the duration of close proximity, frequency of caregiver–child “check-ins,” and which actor initiates interactions using the on-board accelerometer, may be important for evaluating differences between families, changes in behaviors across development, risk for developmental delays, and assessments of intervention success. Moreover, it can detect periods when a caregiver is out of the child’s range, providing insights into the physical (with implications for emotional) availability and responsiveness of the caregiver (for an example using another method to assess availability, see King et al., Reference King, Querdasi, Humphreys and Gotlib2021). Being able to include these details about everyday interactions may be essential for better understanding how caregiver physical contact influences whether and when children meet various developmental goals (e.g., establishing independence, providing children with feelings of security) and what may be the most effective behaviors to target for intervention.

Research to date has begun to exemplify how understanding physical proximity can support targeted intervention. For instance, studies highlight the potential impact of physical touch on the formation of secure attachment (Williams & Turner, Reference Williams and Turner2020) and demonstrate how an infant’s physical location within their environment can influence their exposure to language (Malachowski et al., Reference Malachowski, Salo, Needham and Humphreys2023; Mireault et al., Reference Mireault, Rainville and Laughlin2018) – all of which are vital to their developmental trajectory. This is only the tip of the iceberg in terms of important areas for research into everyday interactions, however. For example, research examining emotion socialization, or the ways in which caregivers respond to children’s emotions, which subsequently teach children how to understand and regulate their own emotions (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg2020), has suggested that supportive emotional responses from caregivers (e.g., emotion labeling) have been associated with the development of self-regulation (P. Tan et al., Reference Tan, Oppenheimer, Ladouceur, Butterfield and Silk2020), sympathy for others (Curtis et al., Reference Curtis, Zhou and Tao2020), lower levels of behavior problems (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Dollar, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2020; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Lee, White and Qiu2020), and greater well-being (Brown & Fredrickson, Reference Brown and Fredrickson2021). However, the degree to which caregiver emotion socialization measured in the laboratory or via questionnaires extends to everyday experiences – and the degree to which caregiver physical proximity is a prerequisite for these experiences – is unknown.

Pursuit of these questions will be enhanced by the use of better software and analytic approaches now available for the rich data discussed here. Machine learning and computer vision technologies are revolutionizing the analysis of video data, enabling researchers to rapidly detect and analyze faces, objects, and movements within video frames (e.g., Bambach et al., Reference Bambach, Crandall, Smith and Yu2018). These advancements significantly decrease the time and effort needed to analyze extensive video recordings. As a result, video coding, known for its time and labor intensity, becomes more accessible to researchers who lack the resources for hiring and training coders, as well as those seeking to access large volumes of recording. Moreover, advancements in the widespread availability and ease of ecological momentary assessment (EMA) provide an opportunity to advance work pertaining to everyday interactions and capturing caregiving in moments of heightened interest (e.g., during emotionally charged situations). These approaches would support further understanding of lingering questions surrounding in-the-moment caregiving. For example, in the case of emotion socialization, EMA can be applied to obtain information in shortened timescales (e.g., children’s emotions during the last 2 hours compared to longer timeframes specified in questionnaires). This would provide more accurate insight into how caregivers’ own emotions and behaviors in response to their children’s different affective states impact caregiving quality, potentially identifying targets to help support families in more challenging moments.

Compared to traditional laboratory observations, advances in statistical approaches and software, proximal distance recordings, and wearable technologies (i.e., camera and audio recorders) enable a more comprehensive and precise portrayal of early experiences that occur daily and in naturalistic contexts. Bridging the gap between laboratory observations and real-world experiences better captures the subtleties of daily life that contribute to infant and child development. Specifically, with these tools researchers can more accurately measure the quality and impact of caregiver–child interactions in broad and specific naturalistic contexts, paving the way for interventions that support healthy developmental processes in everyday life.

Understanding caregivers’ social cognition

Definitions and value of caregiver social cognition

Mind-mindedness and parental reflective functioning

One compelling explanation for why caregivers differ stems from their awareness of, and attunement to, their children’s own mind, thoughts, and emotions. In adults, the extent to which one thinks about self and others’ cognitions and emotions (i.e., mental states), including their child’s, is suggested to be crucial for everyday behaviors and healthy relationships (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002). This may be especially important within the caregiver–child relationship, such that caregivers who envision and consider their child’s own internal world are more likely to have developmentally-appropriate expectations and be attuned and responsive when interacting with their child (King et al., Reference King, Querdasi, Humphreys and Gotlib2021).

Over the past two decades, researchers in this area turned to several approaches to examine caregivers’ ability and tendency to perceive, consider, and reflect upon infants’ affective and cognitive experiences (e.g., mentalizing, empathy, intentionality, insightfulness, and representations). Constructs related to the general capacity for mentalizing (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002), including parental reflective functioning (Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008; Slade, Reference Slade2005), mind-mindedness (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley and Tuckey2001), and parental embodied mentalizing (Shai & Belsky, Reference Shai and Belsky2011), have often been studied in the context of caregiving relationships. First, parental reflective functioning refers to a caregiver’s ability to think about and envision their child’s mental states and attribute meaning to their own experience as caregivers (Slade, Reference Slade2005). Historically measured using interviews (Slade et al., Reference Slade, Aber, Berger, Bresgi and Kaplan2003) and, more recently, questionnaires, the operationalization of parental reflective functioning measures a controlled and conscious process of thinking about children’s mental states. Second, mind-mindedness refers to caregivers’ tendency to attribute mental states to their children (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley and Tuckey2001). Measured by the frequency of appropriate mental states language in observations during caregiver–child interactions or speech samples (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley and Tuckey2001; Meins, Reference Meins1998), the operationalization of mind-mindedness is suggested to tap into a less explicit yet more “in action” reflective process than most assessments of parental reflective functioning (Rosenblum et al., Reference Rosenblum, McDonough, Sameroff and Muzik2008). Finally, a new approach to implicitly assess caregiver mentalizing (i.e., parental embodied mentalizing) has been suggested to assess the construct beyond verbal and semantic measures (Shai & Belsky, Reference Shai and Belsky2017). This approach acknowledges parental mentalizing through the bodily movements and gestures that demonstrate how caregivers adjust to infants’ mental states (e.g., a caregiver who gently rocks their baby after they show slight signs of distress).

Empirical studies consistently reinforce the notion that a caregiver’s capacity to perceive and consider their child’s mental and emotional states are fundamental to responsive caregiving (see reviews by Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017; McMahon & Bernier, Reference McMahon and Bernier2017; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). For instance, meta-analytic evidence indicates there are moderate-size associations of both parental reflective functioning and mind-mindedness with caregiver sensitivity (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). Similarly, an important body of literature documented prospective associations between better parental reflective functioning, embodied mentalizing, or mind-mindedness, and attachment security (Fonagy & Target, Reference Fonagy, Target, Lewis and Harris2005; Gagné et al., Reference Gagné, Lemelin and Tarabulsy2021; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017).

Caregiver representations of the child

Caregivers differ in the internal working models, or mental representations (hereafter: representations) that they hold about their child (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1982), which shape their interpretations of, expectations for, and behaviors towards their child. Caregiver representations are typically coded in caregiver semi-structured interviews and are characterized across a number of dimensions such as richness of perceptions and acceptance, as well as categorized into balanced, disengaged, distorted (Zeanah & Benoit, Reference Zeanah and Benoit1986; Zeanah, Reference Zeanah1996), or disrupted (Crawford & Benoit, Reference Crawford and Benoit2009) classifications. Rather than focusing on a caregiver’s ability and tendency to reflect upon their child’s mental states, representations refer to caregiver’s mental constructs of how well they know, tend to enjoy, and accept their child and their relationship with that child, among other aspects.

The way caregivers represent their role as a caregiver (i.e., that part of their identity and what it means to be a caregiver to this child) may shape the way they think and feel about caring for their child (Bornstein, Reference Bornstein, Lerner, Bornstein and Leventhal2015). In that regard, balanced caregiving representations (i.e., coherent, relationship- and child-focused, rich in details) are linked to higher levels of sensitivity to infants’ needs and cues (Vreeswijk et al., Reference Vreeswijk, Maas and van Bakel2012). Positive and balanced representations are in turn associated with better child development (Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Feldman and Keren2010; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Hoffenkamp, Tooten, Braeken, Vingerhoets and van Bakel2015; Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Hawkins, Plamondon, Moran and Benoit2015). Infants with caregivers who had balanced, in contrast with distorted, representations, were more able to regulate their frustration (Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Dollberg and Nadam2011) and demonstrated better attentional skills (Korja et al., Reference Korja, Ahlqvist-Björkroth, Savonlahti, Stolt, Haataja, Lapinleimu, Piha and Lehtonen2010).

Representations are also linked to caregiver–child outcomes (Benoit et al., Reference Benoit, Parker and Zeanah1997; Huth-Bocks et al., Reference Huth-Bocks, Theran, Levendosky and Bogat2011; Rosenblum et al., Reference Rosenblum, McDonough, Muzik, Miller and Sameroff2002), such that non-balanced (disengaged [i.e., more distancing or intellectualized, lacking in richness] or distorted [i.e., some inconsistencies, not child-focused]) caregiving representations are associated with more difficulties within the dyad. For example, previous work demonstrates that mothers classified as having distorted mental representations were more likely to exhibit atypical caregiving behaviors such as intrusive, withdrawn, or hostile behaviors than mothers with balanced representations (Schechter et al., Reference Schechter, Coates, Kaminer, Coots, Zeanah, Davies, Schonfeld, Marshall, Liebowitz, Trabka, McCaw and Myers2008). A recent study reported that prenatal disrupted representations of the child were prospectively associated with atypical caregiving behaviors at infant age 12 months and socioemotional functioning at 24 months (Guyon-Harris et al., Reference Guyon-Harris, Ahlfs-Dunn, Madigan, Bronfman, Benoit and Huth-Bocks2022), suggesting that the representations that develop during gestation are a salient risk factor, and one that is potentially causal, in the development of psychopathology.

Knowledge gaps

Although promising, there are several gaps in our current understanding of caregivers’ social cognitions. First, despite theoretical support for the idea that social cognitions unfold over time with caregiver and child development, empirical studies focusing on the reciprocal development between caregivers’ cognitions and children’s functioning and development remain very limited. As children advance through different stages of development, the synchrony in the mental engagement between caregiver and child – this “meeting of the minds” – is likely to transform (Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008). Thus, it is reasonable to propose that as children acquire new abilities, the nature and elaborateness of the caregiver’s social cognitions would plausibly adapt in response to these developmental progressions. Yet which changes or cues signal to caregivers that the child is capable of more mature considerations is not clear.

Congruently, recent studies have focused on the progression of caregivers’ mind-mindedness (Bigelow et al., Reference Bigelow, Power and Dadgar2023; Foley et al., Reference Foley, Devine and Hughes2023). These studies have found a degree of consistency, as well as some variability, in caregivers’ comments about their child’s mental world from the prenatal and newborn phases to three to four months postpartum. Specifically, one study found an uptick in mothers’ attuned mind-minded remarks toward their infants over the initial three months, while non-attuned comments remained unchanged (Bigelow et al., Reference Bigelow, Power and Dadgar2023). Despite these findings, the field predominantly treats reflective functioning as a stable characteristic. Whether social cognitions are truly fixed or subject to change in response to a child’s developmental progress remains an area poised for further exploration. For instance, caregivers communicate differently with infants as a function of whether they have achieved certain motor milestones (Schneider & Iverson, Reference Schneider and Iverson2022; West & Iverson, Reference West and Iverson2021). The pattern, nature, and individual differences in caregivers’ “updating” their models is an important gap in our understanding of the bidirectional relationship between caregiver cognitions and children’s evolving capabilities.

Second, in theoretical models, social cognitions have been understood as dynamic, capable of fluctuating with changes in context (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Steele, Steele, Moran and Higgitt1991). Yet, in practice, our study designs and measurements typically capture the relationship at a set point in development, or, if longitudinal, represent either snapshots of time assessed across development or capture more global tendencies. What remains to be considered are specific timescales of the ideal assessments, and further, whether there are some contexts in which mental state reflection may be more or less important (for an example of the importance of caregiver sensitivity varying based on the affective context, see Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Blankson and O’Brien2009). Evidence from research on empathy further suggests effects of psychological symptoms specifically on parental cognitions. Depressive symptoms were linked to lower levels of empathy toward one’s own child over and above the association between depression and dispositional, or general, empathy (Salo et al., Reference Salo, Schunck and Humphreys2020). Further, given that the ability and tendency to consider a child’s mental states may be influenced by the caregiver’s own temperamental or developmental attributes (Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008), understanding how caregivers’ mental and emotional states change as a function of context, including the effects of acute stress as well as functionally impairing depression, for example, is critical for future research.

Third, the measurement of such complex constructs represents a sizeable challenge for research, and current scoring schemes and self-reported assessments have limitations. For parental reflective functioning specifically, existing coding systems produce a single, unidimensional score, which may not fully capture the nuances of caregivers’ abilities to reflect upon the thoughts and feelings of both themselves and their children (Garon-Bissonnette et al., Reference Garon-Bissonnette, Dubois-Comtois, St-Laurent and Berthelot2023; Sleed et al., Reference Sleed, Isosävi and Fonagy2021). This scoring limitation can obscure significant variances in impairments in reflective functioning among caregivers, who might exhibit comparable scores but possess distinct mental content and conceptualizations of their child. For instance, a caregiver who distances themself from thinking in mental states terms would receive a low score of reflective functioning, and at present the same score could also be assigned to a caregiver who attempts reflection but provides confused, diffuse, or inconsistent responses (Garon-Bissonnette et al., Reference Garon-Bissonnette, Dubois-Comtois, St-Laurent and Berthelot2023). Similarly, as parental reflective functioning considers both reflection regarding the child as well as self as a parent, differences in caregiver- versus child-focused reflective functions specifically may emerge (see Smaling et al., Reference Smaling, Huijbregts, van der Heijden, van Goozen and Swaab2016). These qualitative differences may be critical in informing intervention targets for a given caregiver–child pair (Sleed et al., Reference Sleed, Isosävi and Fonagy2021) and relatedly in providing more personalized interventions.

Finally, caregivers’ social cognitions and representations may promote responsiveness to caregiving interventions or evolve as a result of interventions. For instance, one recent study indicated improvement in parental reflective functioning after a short-term intervention aimed at bolstering attachment security, improvements that explained significant variance in positive caregiving behaviors (Dexter & Wong, Reference Dexter and Wong2023). Relatedly, a separate study indicated that pre- to post-intervention changes in caregivers’ beliefs about their importance for child development were linked to more positive dyadic interactions, higher learning, and socioemotional skills in children (List et al., Reference List, Pernaudet and Suskind2021). While this preliminary research is promising, the impact of this work would be further enhanced with pre-registered replications and extensions of this work.

Recommendations for next steps

Current measurement techniques might not adequately differentiate aspects of caregivers’ reflective capacities, potentially leading to uniform treatment approaches when more personalized strategies may be advantageous. Recent advances have suggested adaptations to existing coding schemes that have the ability to tap into the representational and perceptual heterogeneity of caregiver cognitions (Garon-Bissonnette et al., Reference Garon-Bissonnette, Dubois-Comtois, St-Laurent and Berthelot2023; Sleed et al., Reference Sleed, Isosävi and Fonagy2021). These approaches pave the way for tailored methods to support caregiver–child relationships. Specific forms of failures to mentalize were shown to have different determinants in the caregiver’s own developmental history; differences otherwise masked using the usual approach to score reflective functioning (Garon-Bissonnette et al., Reference Garon-Bissonnette, Dubois-Comtois, St-Laurent and Berthelot2023). Similarly, the different representational contents (classified as hostile, helpless, or idealized) of caregivers’ discourse when asked to reflect on their children differentially correlated with the attunement and mutuality between caregiver and child. For instance, hostile and idealized representations, characterized by lower reflective functioning, were associated with poorer caregiver–child attunement, and helpless representations were not strongly associated with measures of caregiver–child relationship quality during a brief laboratory assessment (Sleed et al., Reference Sleed, Isosävi and Fonagy2021). Such “splitting” (e.g., rather than lumping) approaches may better yield information necessary to understand how child-related cognitions develop as well as which dimensions matter most for the caregiving relationship, and in turn the child’s cognitive, social, and emotional development. Future investigations into these complex dimensions of caregiver social cognitions hold promise for the development of more sophisticated and effective assessment tools (King, Salo, et al., Reference King, Salo, Kujawa and Humphreys2021).

Similarly, narrowing the focus from a caregiver’s general- versus child-specific ability for social cognitions (e.g., reflective functioning and parental reflective functioning, dispositional empathy and empathy towards child specifically), and perhaps even in specific moments within the relationship, merits attention in future research (see Salo et al., Reference Salo, Schunck and Humphreys2020; Smaling et al., Reference Smaling, Huijbregts, van der Heijden, van Goozen and Swaab2016). Research from our team demonstrated important variation in caregiving behaviors even within the relationship with a given child (Bailes et al., Reference Bailes, Ennis, Lempres, Cole and Humphreys2023). This study found that caregivers varied in the degree they felt anger and frustration in response to their child’s emotional displays, which were also dependent on whether the emotional expression was considered to be justified or unjustified. Adopting a more granular lens – one that focuses on a given child, a specific relationship, a moment in time, and subtleties of processes, rather than adopting a general, or even caregiver context–specific approach – may be critical in understanding the interrelatedness and complexity of social cognitive and emotional processes.

As previously noted, caregiver–child dynamics are constantly evolving over time, both as the relational partners learn about one another and as the relationship changes in response to growing competencies for both caregiver and child, especially due to the dramatic physical and mental changes occurring within the developing child. Exploring caregivers’ understanding of their child’s mental capacity offers valuable insights into how this awareness may shift with the child’s development. The concept of “mind perception” from cognitive science relates to our judgments about others’ mental capabilities and has been shown to correlate with a greater liking and valuing of that entity (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Gray and Wegner2007). Making more accurate judgments about children’s capabilities, such as understanding their intentions, is crucial for caregivers. It guides the support they provide in areas like motor skills, language, and cognitive tasks. For example, infant communicative behaviors perceived as intentional by caregivers were shown to be more likely to elicit a contingent behavioral response from caregivers than communicative behaviors perceived as non-intentional (Donnellan et al., Reference Donnellan, Bannard, McGillion, Slocombe and Matthews2020). Furthermore, interpreting behaviors more generously – for instance, considering non-blame-oriented reasons for infant night wakings or toddler temper tantrums – can prevent harsh responses or abuse (Bugental et al., Reference Bugental, Ellerson, Lin, Rainey, Kokotovic and O’Hara2002). This connection between a caregiver’s perceptions of their child’s evolving capacities and the caregiver’s responsive and constructive behaviors has been posited to influence the quality of the caregiving relationship, enhancing reflective functioning. Our research group has assessed mind perception in adults as they consider the minds of infants and young children throughout development (Weisman et al., Reference Weisman, King and Humphreys2024) and among caregivers (Salo et al., Reference Salo, Hare, Letterie, King, Weisman and Humphreys2024). We found estimates of children’s mental capacities increased non-linearly as a function of chronological age, with greater changes observed in infancy relative to the preschool-age years. Further, higher levels of estimated capacities were associated with higher levels of parental reflective functioning and more positive caregiving behaviors in parents of preschool-age children (Salo et al., Reference Salo, Hare, Letterie, King, Weisman and Humphreys2024).

Caregivers also develop in their new role, and experience in caregiving may further influence their cognitions regarding their child. For instance, pregnant individuals differ in their beliefs about children’s needs based on whether or not they have other children (Mascheroni et al., Reference Mascheroni, Grassi, Bonanomi, Sperotto, Deeg, Hung, Xia, Ionio, Kit‐fong Au and Gattis2022). Specifically, multiparous individuals prioritize attunement, while primiparous individuals place greater value on structure. Within-person, longitudinal research (e.g., studying the same adult across years to follow their journey as a parent with multiple children) is needed to evaluate how to disentangle caregiving experience with child age/developmental stage in their influence on a caregiver’s cognitions and behaviors.

In sum, much evidence suggests that social cognitions are central contributors to behaviors and development and are important modifiable targets for caregiver–child interventions. Still, however, many gaps in the literature remain to be addressed. Future research would benefit from exploring caregiver social cognitions regarding their child using assessments and coding schemes that capture different dimensions and distinguish between general, child-specific, and even situation-specific abilities and tendencies. In addition, inclusion of longitudinal designs that permit exploration of how perceptions and behaviors change in concert across the first years of life, including how they relate to contextual factors, is an important area for future research.

Influences of caregiver context on functioning

Definition and value of considering context for caregiving relationships

Developmental psychopathology, as mentioned above, has long considered children’s contexts in influencing their outcomes (Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998). In keeping with this framework, we consider how the broader context in which a caregiver lives impacts their behavior toward, cognitions about, and ultimately, their relationship with their child (bridging from the macro- to the microsystem; Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998). Further, caregiver context specifically impacts the real-life everyday interactions the child has in terms of physical access to their caregivers and the emotional availability of the caregiver during those opportunities for interactions. Notably, we specify caregiver context here, rather than family context, to specify factors that influence the child through their impact on caregiver functioning and availability. While caregiving relationships are influenced by a wide range of environmental, societal, cultural, and personal factors, here we discuss caregiver’s (1) relationships with their romantic partners, (2) social support networks, (3) policies regarding post-birth (paid and unpaid) leave, and (4) culture. These factors represent only a few areas to consider within the broader context that impacts caregivers and their relationship with their children. While each is discussed independently, these factors are often related and have bidirectional impacts. For example, cultural norms about caregiver roles may influence expectations regarding dividing childcare between partners (Craig & Mullan, Reference Craig and Mullan2010; Fernández & Fogli, Reference Fernández and Fogli2009); partner relationships provide an important source of social support (Goldberg & Carlson, Reference Goldberg and Carlson2014; McRae et al., Reference McRae, Overall, Henderson, Low and Chang2021); and some configurations of parental leave policies may reinforce expectations that birthing parents are most responsible for a child’s caregiving (Schober, Reference Schober2011).

Partner relationships

Relationships between romantic partners, including the provision of emotional and instrumental support and the quality of the relationship, influence parenting style and interactions with their children (Millings et al., Reference Millings, Walsh, Hepper and O’Brien2013; Overbeek et al., Reference Overbeek, Stattin, Vermulst, Ha and Engels2007), with implications for attachment and child development (Tan et al., Reference Tan, McIntosh, Kothe, Opie and Olsson2018). Here we use “partner relationships” as an umbrella term that includes domestic and marital romantic relationships (i.e., excluding the non-romantic co-parent relationships). Substantial literature has shown that higher partner relationship quality is linked to more positive caregiver–child interactions (Erel & Burman, Reference Erel and Burman1995). Specifically, partner supportiveness is associated with a reduced risk for children’s behavioral problems (Goldberg & Carlson, Reference Goldberg and Carlson2014). Along the same continuum, meta-analytic reviews indicate that negative aspects of partners’ relationships, in particular conflict, increase children’s risk of behavior problems, including externalizing behavior (Buehler et al., Reference Buehler, Anthony, Krishnakumar, Stone, Gerard and Pemberton1997; Reid & Crisafulli, Reference Reid and Crisafulli1990). For example, parents’ behavior toward one another may spill over to affect interactions with their children, including levels of emotional availability or sensitivity to their child (Buehler & Gerard, Reference Buehler and Gerard2002; Coln et al., Reference Coln, Jordan and Mercer2013).

Social support networks

Social support refers to the assistance available (perceived and actual) from other people; that is, to one being part of a supportive social network that provides care, including instrumental (e.g., providing childcare, financial assistance) and emotional (e.g., providing care, advice) support (Ceballo & McLoyd, Reference Ceballo and McLoyd2002; Kang, Reference Kang2013). Both instrumental and emotional support have been linked to more involved and nurturing caregiving (Burchinal et al., Reference Burchinal, Roberts, Nabors and Bryant1996; Hashima & Amato, Reference Hashima and Amato1994). Social support has been found to moderate stress in caregivers and is negatively correlated with postpartum anxiety, depression, and post-traumatic stress symptoms (Cirino & Knapp, Reference Cirino and Knapp2019; Razurel et al., Reference Razurel, Bruchon-Schweitzer, Dupanloup, Irion and Epiney2011). Moreover, when caregivers feel supported by social networks, they are more likely to feel effective in their caregiving role and are more likely to foster a stimulating home environment (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Noonan, McCartney, Marx and Keefe2001).

Parental leave

Policies supporting parental leave (or lack thereof) also impact the context in which caregivers raise their children, influencing caregiver availability and caregiver–child relationships. Policies associated with opportunities for parental leave, including whether such leave is provided with pay, influence family dynamics and relationships. Provision of parental leave, whether by the employer, state, or federal government, is related to both the amount of time spent with a young child and more equitable domestic labor division between mothers and fathers (Craig & Mullan, Reference Craig and Mullan2010). Gender disparities in paid parental leave may reinforce the idea that women are primarily responsible for caregiving, given that opportunities for leave have typically included only the birthing parent. Providing leaves to non-birthing parents may yield more equitable shares of caregiving tasks. Studies supporting this claim found that fathers who take paid leave are more involved in childcare, not only during that leave, but later in the child’s life (Nepomnyaschy & Waldfogel, Reference Nepomnyaschy and Waldfogel2007; O’Brien, Reference O’Brien2009). Policies regarding paid parental leave can, therefore, either facilitate or hinder the availability of one or both caregivers for children both early on and later in life.

Culture

Culture—defined as a pattern of beliefs and behaviors that are shared by a group of people that serve to inform their daily living (Bornstein, Reference Bornstein2012)—provides a shared system in which individuals live. Culture shapes expectations and hopes caregivers have for their children, how they interact with their children, and inform caregivers’ values. Cultural context is influential in the establishment of caregiver cognitions that in turn shape caregiving practices (Harkness et al., Reference Harkness, Rha, Blom and Johnston2007). For example, cultural goals related to desired developmental outcomes influence variation in interaction styles between caregivers and infants (Keller et al., Reference Keller, Yovsi, Borke, Kärtner, Jensen and Papaligoura2004). Western cultures tend to encourage autonomy and independence, with caregivers often engaging in frequent verbal and face-to-face interactions with their infants. However, this style differs from caregivers in non-Western cultures, who tend to value social sensitivity and interconnectedness, where caregiver–child interactions are characterized by close physical contact and affective tuning between caregivers and infants (Tronick et al., Reference Tronick, Morelli and Winn1987). Others have documented differences in caregiver social cognitions based on country of origin. For example, studies have found that after accounting for verbosity, caregivers from cultures that value interdependence (e.g., Chinese mothers compared to mothers from the US, the UK, or Australia; Japanese compared to British mothers) display fewer comments about their children’s internal mental states when interviewed about their child (Doan & Wang, Reference Doan and Wang2010; Fujita & Hughes, Reference Fujita and Hughes2021; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Devine and Wang2018) and when interacting with their child (Dai et al., Reference Dai, McMahon and Lim2020). These results suggest that there are cultural variations in the amount and content of caregiver social cognitions about their children. Caregivers’ cultural background influences socialization goals, caregiver social cognitions, and caregiver–child interactions, all of which are likely to shape child outcomes.

Knowledge gaps

Partner relationships

Several studies have established that relationship quality between caregiving partners, including both marital satisfaction and the co-parenting relationship, impacts caregiving behavior. Yet, many studies tend to include observations of mother–child or father–child interactions, assessing caregiving behavior independently for each dyad. Isolated interactions may miss critical dynamics of partner relationships that impact interactions with their children in everyday, naturalistic interactions. Dyadic relationships within a family system likely spill over to affect other relationships and behavior within those systems, influencing individual development. It would be useful to better understand how the quality of the relationship between partners changes over time, how this impacts the co-parenting relationship, and dyadic relationships with children.

Social support networks

Social support provides an important instrumental and emotional resource for caregivers, but the extent to which configurations of caregiver support networks (i.e., type and source) translate to caregiving behavior and child developmental outcomes remains an important gap in the literature. Some research has compared perceived social support (i.e., the subjective appraisal of adequacy of support network) to received social support (i.e., quantity received) and found that perceived social support is more consistently linked to better psychosocial outcomes (Haber et al., Reference Haber, Cohen, Lucas and Baltes2007). Yet, less is known about how different forms of social support influence behaviors supportive of child development.

Parental leave

Parental leave presents in many different forms; involving one or both parents, as paid or unpaid, and with varying lengths depending on one’s employer and state or federal policies. An important gap in our current knowledge is understanding how these different configurations of parental leave may impact parents’ relationship with their child, the co-parenting relationship, and ultimately how these may interact to impact child development. Work has begun to differentiate the impact of paid versus unpaid leave on child outcomes. For example, a recent systematic review found that longer duration of paid parental leave was associated with improvements in child health, particularly through reduced infant mortality, and that unpaid leave did not confer the same advantages (Nandi et al., Reference Nandi, Jahagirdar, Dimitris, Labrecque, Strumpf, Kaufman, Vincent, Atabay, Harper, Earle and Heymann2018). Further, paid maternity leave is also associated with electroencephalography profiles indicative of more mature brain function by 3 months of age (Brito et al., Reference Brito, Werchan, Brandes-Aitken, Yoshikawa, Greaves and Zhang2022). Few studies have investigated differences in paid versus unpaid parental leave and its potential impact on child development. One such study found that even after adjusting for differences in level of income and leave duration, children of mothers who took paid leave had higher language scores than children of mothers who took unpaid leave (Kozak et al., Reference Kozak, Greaves, Waldfogel, Angal, Elliott, Fifier and Brito2021).

Culture

Research on the cultural context within which families exist has provided insight into which factors influence caregiver cognitions and behaviors. However, a large majority of the work considering culture as a determinant of caregiver–child relationships and how culture impacts children’s development has been conducted in contexts that are not representative of human’s evolutionary history or the cultural contexts in which the majority of the human population currently resides. Some have termed the focus of research on the populations nearest to the investigators who tend to study caregiving in academic institutions as WEIRD (i.e., Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) cultural conditions (Lockman & Tamis-LeMonda, Reference Lockman and Tamis-LeMonda2020; Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Bornstein, Marlow and Swartz2014). This has meant that the majority of our understanding of caregiver–child relationships is derived from families from similar cultural contexts and has precluded our understanding of how different cultural contexts may impact caregiver–child relationships and child development.

Recommendations for next steps

Although the role of context in influencing child development has long been prominent in the field of developmental psychopathology (e.g., a special issue dedicated to the matter was published twenty-five years ago in this journal; Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998), a greater consideration of caregiver context in influencing caregiver functioning and related impacts on child development will move the field forward. Greater consideration of caregiver context lends itself to research which could ultimately inform support and intervention at multiple levels – including garnering support from one’s broader cultural context, informing policies on parental leave, and implementation of partner- or family-based interventions. Here, we propose next steps in this line of research that would actualize these aims.

First, social relationships (including both partner relationships and broader social networks) influence one another, are nested and multidimensional (Boyce et al., Reference Boyce, Frank, Jensen, Kessler, Nelson and Steinberg1998), and have implications for caregiving. Research seeking a more nuanced understanding of the interrelatedness of these aspects of partner relationships and where to target intervention efforts may provide opportunities to improve caregiving practices and caregiver–child interactions. Studies providing observational measures of physical proximity and partner availability may be a fruitful next step for this line of inquiry, for example. Further, investigating how variation in caregiver social support throughout a child’s life impacts developmental trajectories, possible for instance with the use of intensive or accelerated longitudinal designs (Willett et al., Reference Willett, Singer and Martin1998), may provide important guidance regarding sensitive periods (and more specific targets) for providing caregivers with additional support.

Second, accumulating evidence suggests that combined maternal and paternal leave, in particular paid leave, supports engaged caregiving from both partners. Exploring how paid versus unpaid leave, and differences in parental duration leave between partners, impacts provision of childcare, as well as the quality and quantity of caregiver–child interactions, will lead to greater understanding of developmental impacts on children. Studies leveraging technologies as described above (e.g., the TotTag, or Looxcie 2 camera) may provide detailed, objective measures in naturalistic settings to better quantify the impact of differences in parental leave and their impact on caregiver–child relationships. It is important for future work to investigate the parameters and impacts of parental leave to inform specific policy decisions, and garner further support to advocate for change in current policies.

Third, while previous work demonstrates that cultural background influences caregivers’ expectations for their children, social cognitions regarding their children, and their related caregiving behaviors, future work could investigate how culture brings individuals together to pursue similar and different goals for their children, as well as how culture may impact social networks, partner relationships, and broader views on childrearing and use of instrumental supports. Culture is an important aspect of the contextual dimensions inherent to the field of developmental psychopathology, including the careful consideration of how cultural variation impacts child development.

In summary, caregiver context is a critical and often overlooked factor impacting families, relationships, interactions, and goals for children. Here, we discuss partner relationships, social support, parental leave, and culture, yet these are only a few examples of components of the contexts which influence caregivers. Each of these contexts is interrelated (Boyce et al., Reference Boyce, Frank, Jensen, Kessler, Nelson and Steinberg1998) impacting each other to influence caregivers’ roles and identities, the family system, and child development. Echoing calls by Cicchetti and Aber (Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998), future work should consider how this macrosystem impacts microcontextual features of the caregiver–child relationship and, in turn, child development over time. By understanding more about how partner relationships, social support, parental leave, and culture (provided here as examples) impact everyday caregiving interactions, developmental psychologists can identify (and potentially target) contextual factors to improve caregiver and child outcomes.

Summary and conclusions

Developmental psychopathology is a dynamic discipline with scholarly work key to deepening our knowledge of: (1) how people develop, (2) what drives changes in risk and resilience related to mental health, and (3) how we can use this knowledge to inform prevention and intervention at the individual, family, community, and societal levels. Developmental psychopathology leads the way in exploring the complex individual and interpersonal interactions that impact mental health (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1993; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010), and here, we posit that the mission of the field will be best actualized by focusing on contexts that are the most fundamental to individuals’ development related to mental health. Specifically, we argue that no context may be more fundamental to psychopathology and thriving than one’s early life relationships with their caregivers.

Historically, infants and young children have tended to spend the most time with their biological mother, a reality that has led the specific mother–infant pair to be the most commonly studied dyad in research on developmental psychopathology. However, there are many reasons to deepen our understanding of the caregiver–child relationship (in a push toward “relationship-centered” rather than “parent-centered” approaches) and extend our work using a “child-centered” lens, in which all adults who care for a child are considered as part of the child’s caregiving relationship context. From a relationship-centered perspective, a better understanding of caregivers’ tendency and ability to reflect on their child’s mental states (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002; Meins et al., Reference Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley and Tuckey2001; Slade, Reference Slade2005) would help identify personalized targets for intervention to meet the needs of a given caregiver–child dyad. Additionally, within-person, longitudinal studies tracking the evolution of caregiver social cognitions and representations in concert with both caregiver and child development stages would offer valuable insights into the dynamic nature of these relationships, including when intervention may be warranted, how to anticipate struggles, and boost positive practices in the caregiver. From a child-centered lens, a better understanding of the experiences a child has had, including the configuration of quality and quantity of care received from multiple caregivers, would help to identify the various ways that society successfully navigates the high level of care required to raise a child.

Moving the field of developmental psychopathology forward will involve embracing innovative approaches that capture the complexity of caregiver–child interactions beyond the laboratory setting. Although laboratory assessments provide valuable insights, their limits in terms of ecological validity and generalizability underscore the need to complement these assessments with research conducted in real-world contexts. To address these limitations, researchers can leverage technological advancements and intensive longitudinal/EMA approaches to study everyday interactions more authentically. Such advancements can offer real-time observations of caregiver–child interactions, allowing researchers to detect meaningful changes during naturalistic interactions and gain valuable insights into the developmental trajectories of these relationships (de Barbaro & Fausey, Reference de Barbaro and Fausey2022). Furthermore, wearable technologies can extend researchers’ capacity to explore contextual factors impacting caregiver–child dynamics, providing a more representative understanding across diverse environments. These advancements not only enhance the specificity of studying shared social exchanges, but also open avenues for investigating components like touch, language, and emotion socialization. As we move beyond the confines of laboratory assessments, embracing these tools and concepts enables a more comprehensive understanding of the nuanced dynamics shaping the development of caregiver–child relationships “in the wild.”

Lastly, caregivers, with multifaceted roles and identities, are influenced by various factors, including their relationships, community resources, and cultural backgrounds (Bornstein, Reference Bornstein2012). To advance the field, when possible, research should consider tests of the interdependence of relationships within the family system, policy landscape, cultural variations, and community-level resources that collectively shape the caregiver–child relationship and child development over time. Using approaches that allow us to better understand how caregivers obtain support, what barriers exist, and the tools that may be able to be used to support relationships is needed and will ultimately contribute to our understanding of child development.

Here, we think it is important to wear multiple hats, as scientists who may yield insights useful to individual families, clinicians, and policy-makers to guide what options they need to make available and draw from these varied perspectives (e.g., see Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, King, Guyon-Harris and Zeanah2022). Caregiving behaviors are amenable to change and are useful targets given the influence of caregiving (including the continuum of behaviors from maltreatment to those enriching and nurturing) on child development. Research on caregiving relationships allows us to not only advance our knowledge, but also contribute to information useful for action to support individuals and families. By grounding our research in questions that have tangible implications for real-world scenarios, we aim to bridge the gap between academic insights and actionable solutions in various decision-making spheres. Balancing both the scientific goals of knowledge generation and considering how to use such knowledge to address practical challenges within real-world contexts is critical for the future translational impact of our work.

Funding statement

Author effort was partially supported by the National Science Foundation (CAREER Award #2042285) and the National Institute of Mental Health (K23MH131753; T32MH18921; R01MH129634).

Competing interests

None to disclose.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Bailes, L. G., Ennis, G., Lempres, S. M., Cole, D. A., & Humphreys, K. L. (2023). Parents’ emotion socialization behaviors in response to preschool-aged children’s justified and unjustified negative emotions. PLOS One, 18(4), e0283689. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283689 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, A. L., Baldwin, C. P., & Cole, R. E. (1982). Family free-play interaction: Setting and methods. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47(5), 3644. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bambach, S., Crandall, D., Smith, L., & Yu, C. Toddler-inspired visual object learning. In: NeurIPS Proceedings, 2018.Google Scholar
Barnett, W., Hansen, C. L., Bailes, L. G., & Humphreys, K. L. (2022). Caregiver-child proximity as a dimension of early experience. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 647665. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001644 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benoit, D., Parker, K. C. H., & Zeanah, C. H. (1997). Mothers’ representations of their infants assessed prenatally: Stability and association with infants’ attachment classifications. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 38(3), 307313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01515.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigelow, A. E., Power, M., & Dadgar, H. (2023). Maternal mind-mindedness over infants’ first three months. Infant Behavior & Development, 72, 101864. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101864 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biri, A., Jackson, N., Thiele, L., Dutta, P., & Pannuto, P. (2020). SociTrack: Infrastructure-free interaction tracking through mobile sensor networks. In: Proceedings of the 26th Annual International Conference on Mobile Computing and Networking, 33, 114. https://doi.org/10.1145/3372224.3419190 Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2012). Cultural approaches to parenting. Parenting: Science and Practice, 12(2-3), 212221. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2012.683359 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Lerner, R. M., Bornstein, M. H., & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Ecological settings and processes (vol. 4, 7th ed. pp. 55132). Wiley.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664678. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T., Frank, E., Jensen, P., Kessler, R., Nelson, C., & Steinberg, L. (1998). Social context in developmental psychopathology: Recommendations for future research from the MacArthur Network on Psychopathology and Development. Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 143164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579498001552 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Zentall, S., Lickenbrock, D. M., Ekas, N. V., Oshio, T., & Planalp, E. (2014). Attachment in the making: Mother and father sensitivity and infants’ responses during the Still-Face Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125(1), 6384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N. H., Werchan, D., Brandes-Aitken, A., Yoshikawa, H., Greaves, A., & Zhang, M. (2022). Paid maternal leave is associated with infant brain function at 3 months of age. Child Development, 93(4), 10301043. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13765 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C. L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2021). Characteristics and consequences of co-experienced positive affect: Understanding the origins of social skills, social bonds, and caring, healthy communities. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 5863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brzozowska, A., Longo, M. R., Mareschal, D., Wiesemann, F., & Gliga, T. (2021). Capturing touch in parent-infant interaction: A comparison of methods. Infancy, 26(3), 494514. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12394 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buehler, C., Anthony, C., Krishnakumar, A., Stone, G., Gerard, J., & Pemberton, S. (1997). Interparental conflict and youth problem behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 6(2), 233247. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025006909538 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. M. (2002). Marital conflict, ineffective parenting, and children’s and adolescents’ maladjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(1), 7892. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00078.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugental, D. B., Ellerson, P. C., Lin, E. K., Rainey, B., Kokotovic, A., & O’Hara, N. (2002). A cognitive approach to child abuse prevention. Psychology of Violence, 16(3), 243–106. https://doi.org/10.1037/2152-0828.1.S.84 Google ScholarPubMed
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Nabors, L. A., & Bryant, D. M. (1996). Quality of center child care and infant cognitive and language development. Child Development, 67(2), 606620. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131835 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceballo, R., & McLoyd, V. C. (2002). Social support and parenting in poor, dangerous neighborhoods. Child Development, 73(4), 13101321. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00473 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, A. K., Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2016). Multivariate behavioral genetic analysis of parenting in early childhood. Parenting, 16(4), 257283. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2016.1184926 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review, 13(4), 471502. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1993.1021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2002). How a child builds a brain: Insights from normality and psychopathology. In Hartup, W. W., & Weinberg, R. A. (Eds.), Child psychology in retrospect and prospect (pp. 2371). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1998). Contextualism and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 137141. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579498001540 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2006). The developing brain and neural plasticity: Implications for normality, psychopathology, and resilience. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Developmental neuroscience (vol. 2, 2nd ed. pp. 164). Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children’s development. Psychiatry-Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 56(1), 96118. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1993.11024624 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirino, N. H., & Knapp, J. M. (2019). Perinatal posttraumatic stress disorder: A review of risk factors, diagnosis, and treatment. Obstetrical & Gynecological Survey, 74(6), 369376. https://doi.org/10.1097/OGX.0000000000000680 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coln, K. L., Jordan, S. S., & Mercer, S. H. (2013). A unified model exploring parenting practices as mediators of marital conflict and children’s adjustment. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44(3), 419429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0336-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, L., & Mullan, K. (2010). Parenthood, gender and work-family time in the United States, Australia, Italy, France, and Denmark. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72(5), 13441361. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00769.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, A., & Benoit, D. (2009). Caregivers’ disrupted representations of the unborn child predict later infant-caregiver disorganized attachment and disrupted interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 30(2), 124144. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20207 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, A., Lavechin, M., Scaff, C., Soderstrom, M., Rowland, C., Räsänen, O., Bunce, J., Bergelson, E. (2021). A thorough evaluation of the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) system. Behavior Research Methods, 53(2), 467486. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01393-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S. S. (1988). Mothers’ internal models of relationships and children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of mother-child interaction. Child Development, 59(5), 12731285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01496.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, K., Zhou, Q., & Tao, A. (2020). Emotion talk in Chinese American immigrant families and longitudinal links to children’s socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 475488. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000806 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, Q., McMahon, C., & Lim, A. K. (2020). Cross-cultural comparison of maternal mind-mindedness among Australian and Chinese mothers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44(4), 365370. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025419874133 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Barbaro, K., & Fausey, C. M. (2022). Ten lessons about infants’ everyday experiences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(1), 2833. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211059536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Barbaro, K., Khante, P., Maier, M., & Goodman, S. (2023). Maternal contingent responses to distress facilitate infant soothing but not in mothers with depression or infants high in negative affect. Developmental Psychology, 60(2), 294305. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dexter, C. A., & Wong, K. (2023). Circle of security-parenting randomized waitlist control study: Change in reflective functioning explains positive caregiver behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-023-02710-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2010). Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: Relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and Immigrant Chinese children. Child Development, 81(5), 14901503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01487.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollberg, D., Feldman, R., & Keren, M. (2010). Maternal representations, infant psychiatric status, and mother-child relationship in clinic-referred and non-referred infants. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(1), 2536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-009-0036-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnellan, E., Bannard, C., McGillion, M. L., Slocombe, K. E., & Matthews, D. (2020). Infants’ intentionally communicative vocalizations elicit responses from caregivers and are the best predictors of the transition to language: A longitudinal investigation of infants’ vocalizations, gestures and word production. Developmental Science, 23(1), e12843. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12843 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, C., & Cicchetti, D. (2017). From the cradle to the grave: The effect of adverse caregiving environments on attachment and relationships throughout the lifespan. Clinical Psychology: A Publication of the Division of Clinical Psychology of the American Psychological Association, 24(2), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12192 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2020). Findings, issues, and new directions for research on emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 664670. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 118(1), 108132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R., Dollberg, D., & Nadam, R. (2011). The expression and regulation of anger in toddlers: Relations to maternal behavior and mental representations. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(2), 310320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.02.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R., & Vengrober, A. (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder in infants and young children exposed to war-related trauma. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(7), 645658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández, R., & Fogli, A. (2009). Culture: An empirical investigation of beliefs, work, and fertility. American Economic Journal: Macroeconomics, 1(1), 146177. http://www.jstor.org/stable/25760263 Google Scholar
Foley, S., Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2023). Mind-mindedness in new mothers and fathers: Stability and discontinuity from pregnancy to toddlerhood. Developmental Psychology, 59(1), 128140. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001468 Google ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. Other Press.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., & Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12(3), 201218. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(199123)12: 3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2005). Mentalization and the changing aims of child psychoanalysis, 1998. In Lewis, A., & Harris, A. (Eds.), Relational psychoanalysis: Innovation and expansion (vol. 2, pp. 253278). Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Fujita, N., & Hughes, C. (2021). Mind-mindedness and self-other distinction: Contrasts between Japanese and British mothers’ speech samples. Social Development, 30(1), 5772. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12454 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, K., Lemelin, J.-P., & Tarabulsy, G. M. (2021). Non-verbal and verbal parental mentalization as predictors of infant attachment security: Contributions of parental embodied mentalizing and mind-mindedness and the mediating role of maternal sensitivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 65, 101622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101622 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon-Bissonnette, J., Dubois-Comtois, K., St-Laurent, D., & Berthelot, N. (2023). A deeper look at the association between childhood maltreatment and reflective functioning. Attachment & Human Development, 25(3-4), 368389. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2023.2207558 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, J. S., & Carlson, M. J. (2014). Parents’ relationship quality and children’s behavior in stable married and cohabiting families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 76(4), 762777. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12120 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619619. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guyon-Harris, K. L., Ahlfs-Dunn, S. M., Madigan, S., Bronfman, E., Benoit, D., & Huth-Bocks, A. C. (2022). Disrupted caregiving behavior as a mediator of the relation between disrupted prenatal maternal representations and toddler social-emotional functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 34(3), 755763. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001674 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, M. G., Cohen, J. L., Lucas, T., & Baltes, B. B. (2007). The relationship between self-reported received and perceived social support: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 39(1-2), 133144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9100-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, R. A. S., Hoffenkamp, H. N., Tooten, A., Braeken, J., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & van Bakel, H. J. A. (2015). Longitudinal associations between maternal disrupted representations, maternal interactive behavior and infant attachment: A comparison between full-term and preterm dyads. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46(2), 320331. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-014-0473-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, S., Rha, J.-H., Blom, M. J. M., & Johnston, C. (2007). Cultural models and developmental agendas: Implications for arousal and self-regulation in early infancy. Journal of Developmental Processes, 2(1), 539.Google Scholar
Harold, G. T., Leve, L. D., Barrett, D., Elam, K., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Reiss, D., Thapar, A. (2013). Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD symptoms: Revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(10), 10381046. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashima, P. Y., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Poverty, social support, and parental behavior. Child Development, 65(2), 394403. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131391 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., & Wang, Z. (2018). Does parental mind-mindedness account for cross-cultural differences in preschoolers’ theory of mind? Child Development, 89(4), 12961310. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12746 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Guyon-Harris, K. L., & Zeanah, C. H. (2022). Caregiver regulation: A modifiable target promoting resilience to early adverse experiences. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 14(S1), S6371. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0001111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huth-Bocks, A. C., Theran, S. A., Levendosky, A. A., & Bogat, G. A. (2011). A social-contextual understanding of concordance and discordance between maternal prenatal representations of the infant and infant-mother attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32(4), 405426. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20304 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jayaraman, S., Fausey, C. M., & Smith, L. B. (2015). The faces in infant-perspective scenes change over the first year of life. PLOS ONE, 10(5), e0123780. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123780 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, J. (2013). Instrumental social support, material hardship, personal control and neglectful parenting. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(9), 13661373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2013.05.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Kärtner, J., Jensen, H., & Papaligoura, Z. (2004). Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: Self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. Child Development, 75(6), 17451760. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00814.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Querdasi, F. R., Humphreys, K. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Dimensions of the language environment in infancy and symptoms of psychopathology in toddlerhood. Developmental Science, 24(5), e13082. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Rangel, E., Simpson, N., Tikotzky, L., & Manber, R. (2020). Mothers’ postpartum sleep disturbance is associated with the ability to sustain sensitivity toward infants. Sleep Medicine, 65, 7483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2019.07.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Salo, V. C., Kujawa, A., & Humphreys, K. L. (2021). Advancing the RDoC initiative through the assessment of caregiver social processes. Development and Psychopathology, 33(5), 16481664. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942100064X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klahr, A. M., Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Klump, K. L., & Burt, S. A. (2013). Evocative gene-environment correlation in the mother-child relationship: A twin study of interpersonal processes. Development and Psychopathology, 25(1), 105118. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941200934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (1995). Mother-child mutually positive affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development, 66(1), 236254. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131203 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Piha, J., Lehtonen, L. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the quality of mother-infant interaction in preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(3), 330336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozak, K., Greaves, A., Waldfogel, J., Angal, J., Elliott, A. J., Fifier, W. P., & Brito, N. H. (2021). Paid maternal leave is associated with better language and socioemotional outcomes during toddlerhood. Infancy, 26(4), 536550. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12399 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
List, J. A., Pernaudet, J., & Suskind, D. L. (2021). Shifting parental beliefs about child development to foster parental investments and improve school readiness outcomes. Nature Communications, 12(1), 5765. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-25964-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockman, J. J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2020). The Cambridge handbook of infant development: Brain, behavior, and cultural context. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madden-Rusnak, A., Micheletti, M., Dominguez, A., & de Barbaro, K. (2023). Spontaneous infant crying modulates vagal activity in real time. Developmental Psychobiology, 65(7), e22428. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22428 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madigan, S., Hawkins, E., Plamondon, A., Moran, G., & Benoit, D. (2015). Maternal representations and infant attachment: An examination of the prototype hypothesis. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(5), 459468. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21527 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malachowski, L. G., Salo, V. C., Needham, A. W., & Humphreys, K. L. (2023). Infant placement and language exposure in daily life. Infant and Child Development, 32(3), e2405. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2405 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, N. L., Noonan, A. E., McCartney, K., Marx, F., & Keefe, N. (2001). It takes an urban village: Parenting networks of urban families. Journal of Family Issues, 22(2), 163182. https://doi.org/10.1177/019251301022002003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mascheroni, E., Grassi, M., Bonanomi, A., Sperotto, R., Deeg, S., Hung, S., Xia, R., Ionio, C., Kit‐fong Au, T., Gattis, M. (2022). The role of experience in parenting beliefs of British and Italian women during pregnancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 43(6), 835848. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.22014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, B. T. (2019). Parent distraction with phones, reasons for use, and impacts on parenting and child outcomes: A review of the emerging research. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 1(2), 7280. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.139 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, C. A., & Bernier, A. (2017). Twenty years of research on parental mind-mindedness: Empirical findings, theoretical and methodological challenges, and new directions. Developmental Review, 46, 5480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2017.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McRae, C. S., Overall, N. C., Henderson, A. M. E., Low, R. S. T., & Chang, V. T. (2021). Parents’ distress and poor parenting during a COVID-19 lockdown: The buffering effects of partner support and cooperative coparenting. Developmental Psychology, 57(10), 16231632. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001207 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E. (1998). The effects of security of attachment and maternal attribution of meaning on children’s linguistic acquisitional style. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2), 237252. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90004-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(5), 637648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millings, A., Walsh, J., Hepper, E., & O’Brien, M. (2013). Good partner, good parent: Responsiveness mediates the link between romantic attachment and parenting style. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(2), 170180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212468333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mireault, G. C., Rainville, B. S., & Laughlin, B. (2018). Push or carry? Pragmatic opportunities for language development in strollers vs. backpacks. Infancy, 23(4), 616624. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nandi, A., Jahagirdar, D., Dimitris, M., Labrecque, J., Strumpf, E., Kaufman, J., Vincent, I., Atabay, E., Harper, S., Earle, A., Heymann, S. (2018). The impact of parental and medical leave policies on socioeconomic and health outcomes in OECD countries: A systematic review of the empirical literature. The Milbank Quarterly, 96(3), 434471. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0009.12340 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nepomnyaschy, L., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). Paternity leave and fathers’ involvement with their young children: Evidence from the American ECLS-B. Community, Work & Family, 10(4), 427453. https://doi.org/10.1080/13668800701575077 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, M. (2009). Fathers, parental leave policies, and infant quality of life: International perspectives and policy impact. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 624(1), 190213. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716209334349 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overbeek, G., Stattin, H., Vermulst, A., Ha, T., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2007). Parent-child relationships, partner relationships, and emotional adjustment: A birth-to-maturity prospective study. Developmental Psychology, 43(2), 429437. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.429 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelham, W. E., Lang, A. R., Atkeson, B., Murphy, D. A., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Vodde-Hamilton, M., Greenslade, K. E. (1997). Effects of deviant child behavior on parental distress and alcohol consumption in laboratory interactions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(5), 413424. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025789108958 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, N. B., Dollar, J. M., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2020). Maternal socialization of child emotion and adolescent adjustment: Indirect effects through emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 541552. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000815 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2009). Time-structured and net intraindividual variability: Tools for examining the development of dynamic characteristics and processes. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 778791. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017915 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Razurel, C., Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., Dupanloup, A., Irion, O., & Epiney, M. (2011). Stressful events, social support and coping strategies of primiparous women during the postpartum period: A qualitative study. Midwifery, 27(2), 237242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2009.06.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, W. J., & Crisafulli, A. (1990). Marital discord and child behavior problems: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18(1), 105117. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00919459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, K. L., McDonough, S. C., Sameroff, A. J., & Muzik, M. (2008). Reflection in thought and action: Maternal parenting reflectivity predicts mind-minded comments and interactive behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29(4), 362376. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20184 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, K. L., McDonough, S., Muzik, M., Miller, A., & Sameroff, A. (2002). Maternal representations of the infant: Associations with infant response to the Still Face. Child Development, 73(4), 9991015. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00453 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., & Werker, J. (2021). A conversation with Michael Rutter. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-021821-044256 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salo, V. C., Hare, M., Letterie, M., King, L., Weisman, K., & Humphreys, K. L.Parents’ perceptions of young children’s minds are associated with their interest in and behaviour toward their own child 2024. [Manuscript submitted for publication].Google Scholar
Salo, V. C., King, L. S., Gotlib, I. H., & Humphreys, K. L. (2022). Infants who experience more adult-initiated conversations have better expressive language in toddlerhood. Infancy, 27(5), 916936. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12487 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salo, V. C., Pannuto, P., Hedgecock, W., Biri, A., Russo, D. A., Piersiak, H. A., & Humphreys, K. L. (2022). Measuring naturalistic proximity as a window into caregiver-child interaction patterns. Behavior Research Methods, 54(4), 15801594. https://doi.org/10.3758/S13428-021-01681-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salo, V. C., Schunck, S. J., & Humphreys, K. L. (2020). Depressive symptoms in parents are associated with reduced empathy toward their young children. PLOS One, 15(3), e0230636. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230636 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schechter, D. S., Coates, S. W., Kaminer, T., Coots, T., Zeanah, C. H. Jr., Davies, M., Schonfeld, I. S., Marshall, R. D., Liebowitz, M. R., Trabka, K. A., McCaw, J. E., Myers, M. M. (2008). Distorted maternal mental representations and atypical behavior in a clinical sample of violence-exposed mothers and their toddlers. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 9(2), 123147. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299730802045666 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, J. L., & Iverson, J. M. (2022). Cascades in action: How the transition to walking shapes caregiver communication during everyday interactions. Developmental Psychology, 58(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001280 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schober, S. (2011). Maternal labor market return, parental leave policies, and gender inequality in housework. SOEP Paper, 422, 36. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1980674 Google Scholar
Shai, D., & Belsky, J. (2011). When words just won’t do: Introducing parental embodied mentalizing. Child Development Perspectives, 5(3), 173180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00181.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shai, D., & Belsky, J. (2017). Parental embodied mentalizing: How the nonverbal dance between parents and infants predicts children’s socio-emotional functioning. Attachment & Human Development, 19(2), 191219. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1255653 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development, 17(3), 737754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00457.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, A., Aber, J. L., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., & Kaplan, M. The parent development interview - revised 2003. Unpublished protocol, The City Universiy of New York.Google Scholar
Sleed, M., Isosävi, S., & Fonagy, P. (2021). The assessment of representational risk (ARR): Development and psychometric properties of a new coding system for assessing risk in the parent-infant relationship. Infant Mental Health Journal, 42(4), 529545. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21932 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smaling, H. J. A., Huijbregts, S. C. J., van der Heijden, K. B., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Swaab, H. (2016). Maternal reflective functioning as a multidimensional construct: Differential associations with children’s temperament and externalizing behavior. Infant Behavior & Development, 44, 263274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Power in methods: Language to infants in structured and naturalistic contexts. Developmental Science, 20(6), e12456. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12456 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, E. S., McIntosh, J. E., Kothe, E. J., Opie, J. E., & Olsson, C. A. (2018). Couple relationship quality and offspring attachment security: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Attachment & Human Development, 20(4), 349377. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2017.1401651 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, P., Oppenheimer, C., Ladouceur, C., Butterfield, R., & Silk, J. (2020). A review of associations between parental emotion socialization behaviors and the neural substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in youth. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 516527. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000893 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teti, D. M., Kim, B.-R., Mayer, G., & Countermine, M. (2010). Maternal emotional availability at bedtime predicts infant sleep quality. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(3), 307315. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019306 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomlinson, M., Bornstein, M. H., Marlow, M., & Swartz, L. (2014). Imbalances in the knowledge about infant mental health in rich and poor countries: Too little progress in bridging the gap. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(6), 624629. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1979). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-7138(09)62273-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G. A., & Winn, S. (1987). Multiple caretaking of efe (Pygmy) infants. American Anthropologist, 89(1), 96106. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1987.89.1.02a00050 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Harden, K. P. (2012). Early childhood cognitive development and parental cognitive stimulation: Evidence for reciprocal gene-environment transactions. Developmental Science, 15(2), 250259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01121.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Department of Health & Human Services. Child Maltreatment 2021, 2023. https://www.acf.hhs.gov/cb/data-research/child-maltreatment Google Scholar
Vreeswijk, C. M. J. M., Maas, A. J. B. M., & van Bakel, H. J. A. (2012). Parental representations: A systematic review of the working model of the child interview. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(3), 314328. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisman, K., King, L. S., & Humphreys, K. L. Beliefs about the development of mental life, 2024. [Manuscript submitted for publication].Google Scholar
West, K. L., & Iverson, J. M. (2021). Communication changes when infants begin to walk. Developmental Science, 24(5), e13102. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willett, J. B., Singer, J. D., & Martin, N. C. (1998). The design and analysis of longitudinal studies of development and psychopathology in context: Statistical models and methodological recommendations. Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 395426. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579498001667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, L. R., & Turner, P. R. (2020). Infant carrying as a tool to promote secure attachments in young mothers: Comparing intervention and control infants during the still-face paradigm. Infant Behavior & Development, 58, 101413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101413 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2019). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th edn). World Health Organization. https://icd.who.int/en Google Scholar
Xu, D., Yapanel, U., & Gray, S. (2009). Reliability of the LENA Language Environment Analysis system in young children’s natural home environment (pp. 116). Lena Foundation.Google Scholar
Zeanah, C. (1996). Beyond insecurity: A reconceptualization of attachment disorders of infancy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 4252. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.64.1.42 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeanah, C. H., & Benoit, D. (1986). Working model of the child interview. https://doi.org/10.1037/t47439-000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., & Zeanah, P. D. (2018). Infant mental health: The clinical science of early experience. In Zeanah, C. H. (Eds.), Handbook of infant mental health. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G.-J. J. M., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant-parent attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 12451272. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zero to Three (2016). DC: 0-5: Diagnostic classification of mental health and developental disorders of infancy and early childhood. Zero to Three.Google Scholar
Zhang, H., Lee, Z. X., White, T., & Qiu, A. (2020). Parental and social factors in relation to child psychopathology, behavior, and cognitive function. Translational Psychiatry, 10(1), 80. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0761-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. A conceptual model of relevant domains for the study of caregiving relationships. Note. While we acknowledge domains are bidirectional in influence, single headed arrows were selected in order to indicate the hypothesized direction of most interest. The child’s effect on caregiver social cognitions is proposed to be not static, but rather change as a function of experiences with the child and the child’s growing competencies and individual characteristics. Figure created in BioRender.com.