Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T01:59:38.806Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tool-use and Language in Apes and Humans

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2008

Steven Mithen
Affiliation:
Department of ArchaeologyUniversity of ReadingReading RG6 2AA

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Review Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Byrne, R.W. & A., Whiten (eds.), 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of the Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., 1991. The archaeology of language origins: a review. Antiquity 65, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I. & W., Noble, 1989. The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language. Current Anthropology 30, 1255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibble, H., 1989. The implications of stone tool types for the presence of language during the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic, in The Human Revolution, eds. P., Mellars & C., Stringer. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 415–31.Google Scholar
Donald, M., 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Duff, A.I., G.A., Clark & T.J., Chadderon, 1992. Symbolism in the early Palaeolithic; a conceptual odyssey. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 2(2), 211–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, G., 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H., 1983. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York (NY): Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gould, S.J., 1977. Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Griffin, D.R., 1976. The Question of Animal Awareness. New York (NY): Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
Hewes, G.W., 1973. An explicit formulation of the relation between tool-using and early human language emergence. Visible Language 7, 102–27.Google Scholar
Hewes, G.W., 1976. The current status of the gestural theory of language origin, in Origins and Evolution of Language and Speech, eds. S.R., Harnad, H.D., Steklis & J., Lancaster. (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280.) New York (NY): Academy of Sciences, 482504.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N., 1976. The social function of intellect, in Growing Points in Ethology, eds. P.P.G., Bateson & R.A., Hinde. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 303–17.Google Scholar
Kuhn, S., 1993. The Lithic Assemblage from Yarimburgaz. Unpublished paper presented at the Palaeo-anthropology Society Meeting, Toronto, May 1993.Google Scholar
Mellars, P., 1991. Cognitive changes and the emergence of modern humans. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 1(1), 6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 1993a in press. From domain specific to generalised intelligence: a cognitive interpretation of the Middle/Upper Palaeolithic transition, in Ancient Minds, eds. C., Renfrew & E., Zubrow. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, S., 1993b in press. The origin of art: natural signs, mental modularity and visual symbolism, in Darwinian Archaeologies, eds. H., Maschner & S., Mithen. London: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, S., 1994 forthcoming. Technology & society during the middle Pleistocene. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 4(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, S.T. & K.R., Gibson, 1979. A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 2, 367408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelegrin, J., 1993. A framework for analysing prehistoric stone tool manufacture and a tentative application to some early stone industries, in The Use of Tools by Human and Non-human Primates, eds. A., Berthelet & J., Chavaillon. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 302–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pigeot, N., 1987. Magdaléniens d'Etiolles. Economie de Débitage et Organisation Sociale. (Gallia Préhistoire, Supplŕment 15.) Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
Roberts, M., 1986. Excavations of the lower Palaeolithic site at Amey's Eartham pit, Boxgrove, West Sussex. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 52, 215–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roebroeks, W., 1988. From flint scatters to early hominid behaviour: a study of middle Palaeolithic riverside settlements at Mastrict-Belvedere. Analecta Praehistorica Leidensai 1988.Google Scholar
Stringer, C. & C., Gamble, 1993. In Search of the Neanderthals. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Toth, N., K.D., Schick, E.S., Savage-Rumbaugh, R.A., Sevcik & D.M., Rumbaugh, 1993. Pan the tool-maker: investigations into the stone tool-making and using capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus). Journal of Archaeological Science 20, 8191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whallon, R., 1989. Elements of cultural change in the Late Palaeolithic, in The Human Revolution, eds. P., Mellars & C., Stringer. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 433–54.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., 1991. Natural Theories of Mind. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wynn, T., 1979. The intelligence of later Acheulian hominids. Man 14, 371–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1981. The intelligence of Oldowan hominids. Journal of Human Evolution 10, 529–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1989. The Evolution of Spatial Competence. (Illinois Studies in Anthropology, 17.) Urbana (IL): University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & W.C., McGrew, 1989. An ape's eye view of the Oldowan. Man 24, 383–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar