Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-08T10:10:01.060Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Evolving Mind

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2008

Abstract

Last year saw the publication of two major studies on the evolution of the human mind, Human Evolution, Language and Mind by William Noble and Iain Davidson, and The Prehistory of the Mind by Steven Mithen. Both draw on a number of disciplines including archaeology, psychology, philosophy and animal ethology, but their resulting scenarios of human development differ in several important respects. The question of mind is central to our understanding of the origin of humanness. As Noble and Davidson put it, ‘the mark of modern human behaviour is its self-consciousness, its “mindedness”’. They trace the origin of this behaviour back no further than 100,000 years ago. In Noble and Davidson's view, the behaviours of the ancestors of humans were similar in crucial respects to those of modern non-human primates. Mithen, on the other hand, ‘cannot accept … that language did not appear in a gradual fashion’. Mithen proposes a series of distinct intelligences, and puts forward a modular theory of the evolution of the human mind. In the following pages we present the issues in the form of a debate, inviting each of the three authors to review each other's books and then offering them the opportunity to defend their alternative standpoints. We begin with Mithen's review of Noble and Davidson, then Noble and Davidson's review of Mithen, and finish with their respective replies to the comments and criticisms which have been raised.

Type
Review Feature
Copyright
Copyright © The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashton, N. M., Cook, J., Lewis, S. G. & Rose, J. (eds.), 1992. High Lodge. London: British Museum Press.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (eds.), 1992. The Adapted Mind. New York (NY) & Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D., 1993. Understanding Other Minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bergman, C. A. & Roberts, M. B., 1988. Flaking technology at the Acheulean site of Boxgrove, West Sussex (England). Revue archéologique de Picardie 1–2, 105–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beyries, S., 1987. Variabilité de l'industrie lithique au Mouslérien. (British Archaeological Reports International Series 328.) Oxford: BAR.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D., 1990. Language and Species. Chicago (IL): Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D., 1996. Language and Human Behaviour. London: University College Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P., 1996. Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare, in Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and Man, eds. Runciman, W. G., Maynard-Smith, J. & Dunbar, R. I. M.. Proceedings of the British Academy 88, 7783.Google Scholar
Bradley, B. & Sampson, C. G., 1986. Analysis by replication of two Acheulian artefact assemblages, in Stone Age Prehistory, eds. Bailey, G. N. & Callow, P.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2945.Google Scholar
Bradshaw, J. L. & Rogers, L. J., 1993. The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries, Language, Tool Use, and Intellect. Sydney: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Byrne, R., 1995. The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. & Whiten, A., 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Callow, P., 1986a. Interpreting the La Cotte sequence, in Callow, & Cornford, (eds.), 7382.Google Scholar
Callow, P., 1986b. The La Cotte industries and the European Lower and Middle Palaeolithic, in Callow, & Cornford, (eds.), 377–88.Google Scholar
Callow, P. & Cornford, J. (eds.), 1986. La Cotte de St Brelade 1961–1978. Norwich: Geo Books.Google Scholar
Chase, P.G., 1991. Symbols and Paleolithic artifacts. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 10, 193214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M., 1990. How Monkeys See the World. Chicago (IL): Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M., 1991. Reading minds or reading behaviour? Tests for a theory of mind in monkeys, in Natural Theories of Mind, ed. Whiten, A.. Oxford: Blackwell, 175–94.Google Scholar
Corballis, M.C., 1991. The Lopsided Ape. New York (NY): Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., 1987. From evolution to behaviour: evolutionary psychology as the missing link, in The Latest on the Best: Essays on Evolution and Optimality, ed. Dupré, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 277306.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., 1994. Origins of domain specificity: the evolution of functional organization, in Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, eds. Hirschfeld, L.A. & Gelman, S.A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 85116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I., 1989. Freedom of information, in Animals into Art, ed. Morphy, H.. London: Unwin Hyman, 440–56.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., 1990. Bilzingsleben and early marking. Rock Art Research 7, 52–6.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., 1991. The archaeology of language origins. Antiquity 65, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I., in press. The game of the name: continuity and discontinuity in language origins, in From wrrs to Words, ed. King, B.J.. Santa Fe (NM): School of American Research.Google Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W., 1992. Why the first colonisation of the Australian region is the earliest evidence of modern human behaviour. Archaeology in Oceania 27, 135–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W., 1993. Tools and language in human evolution, in Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution, eds. Gibson, K. & Ingold, T.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 363–88.Google Scholar
Dennett, D., 1991. Consciousness Explained. New York (NY): Little Brown & Co.Google Scholar
Dennett, D., 1996. Granny versus Mother Nature — no contest. Mind & Language 11, 263–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibble, H.L., 1987. Reduction sequences in the manufacture of Mousterian implements of France, in The Pleistocene Old World, ed. Soffer, O.. New York (NY): Plenum Press, 3344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibble, H.L., 1989. The implications of stone tool types for the presence of language during the Lower and Middle Paleolithic, in The Human Revolution, eds. Mellars, P. & Stringer, C.. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 415–31.Google Scholar
Donald, M., 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M., 1991. Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatologica 57, 121–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M., 1992. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 20, 469–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M., 1993. Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 681735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, D., 1992. Braindance. New York (NY): Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Fodor, J., 1996. Deconstructing Dennett's Darwin. Mind & Language 11, 246–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fullagar, R.L.K., Price, D.M. & Head, L., 1996. Early human occupation of northern Australia: archaeology and thermoluminescence dating of Jinmium rock-shelter, Northern Territory. Antiquity 70, 751–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamble, C., 1986. The Palaeolithic Settlement of Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gargett, R.H., 1989. Grave shortcomings. Current Anthropology 30, 157–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C.M., 1994a. Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour 47, 909–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C.M., 1994b. Social cognition in primates, in Hand-book of Perception and Cognition, ed. Mackintosh, N.J.. New York (NY): Academic Press, 281305.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N., 1976. The social function of intellect, in Growing Points in Ethology, eds. Bateson, P.P.G. & Hinde, R.A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 303–17.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N., 1993. A History of the Mind. London: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
James, S.R., 1989. Hominid use of fire in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene. Current Anthropology 30, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshack, A., 1989. Evolution of the human capacity. Year-book of Physical Anthropology 32, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, N., Valladas, H., Valladas, G., Reyss, J.-L., Jelinek, A., Meignen, L. & Joron, J.-L., 1995. TL dates of burnt flints from Jelinek's excavations at Tabun and their implications. Journal of Archaeological Science 22, 495509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 1994. Technology and society during the Middle Pleistocene. Cambridge Arcltaeological Journal 4(1), 332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 1996. The Prehistory of the Mind: a Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London & New York (NY): Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1991. The evolutionary emergence of modern human behaviour: language and its archaeology. Man 26, 223–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1993. Tracing the emergence of modern human behavior: methodological pitfalls and a theoretical path. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 12, 121–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1996. Human Evolution, Language, and Mind: a Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., in press. Discovering the symbolic potential of communicative signs — the origins of speaking a language, in The Archaeology of Mind, ed. Nowell, A.. New York (NY): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., 1994. The Language Instinct. New York (NY): Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rigaud, J.-P. (ed.), 1988. La Grotte Vaufrey á Cenac et Saint-Julien (Dordogne). (Mémoires de la Société Préhistorique Française 19.) Paris: Société Préhistorique Française.Google Scholar
Roberts, M.B., 1986. Excavations of the Lower Palaeolithic site at Amey's Eartham pit, Boxgrove, West Sussex. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 52, 215–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, M.B., Stringer, C.B. & Parfitt, S.A., 1994. A hominid tibia from Middle Pleistocene sediments at Boxgrove, UK. Nature 369, 311–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roe, D.A., 1981. The Lower and Middle Palaeolithic Periods in Britain. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Rolland, N. & Dibble, H.L., 1990. A new synthesis of Middle Paleolithic variability. American Antiquity 55, 480–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawaguchi, T., 1992. The size of neocortex in relation to ecology and social structure in monkeys and apes. Folia primatologica 58, 131–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiner, M.C., 1994. Honor among Thieves: a Zooarchaeological Study of Neandertal Ecology. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Thieme, H., 1997. Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 385, 807–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., 1996. Do apes ape?, in Social Learning in Animals, eds. Heyes, C. & Galef, D.G.. London: Academic Press, 291318.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A.C. & Ratner, H.H., 1993. Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 495552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, N., 1985. The Oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artefacts. Journal of Archaeological Science 12, 101–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, N., Schick, K.D., Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S., Sevik, R.A. & Rumbaugh, D.M., 1993. Pan the tool-maker: investigations into the tool making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo (Pan piniscus). Journal of Archaeological Science 20, 8191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M.T., 1990. Coordination. American Psychologist 45, 938–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van, Peer P., 1992. The Levallois Reduction Strategy. Madison (WI): Prehistory Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Byrne, R., 1988. Tactical deception in pri-mates. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 11, 233–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, R.V.S., 1972. Imitative learning of a flaked tool technology. Mankind 8, 296306.Google Scholar
Wynn, T., 1995. Handaxe enigmas. World Archaeology 27, 1024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T. & McGrew, W.C., 1989. An ape's eye view of the Oldowan. Man 24, 383–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow, P.G., 1990. Socio-perceptual bases for the emergence of language. Developmental Psychobiology 23, 705–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zukow-Goldring, P. & Ferko, K.R., 1994. An ecological approach to the emergence of the lexicon: socializing attention, in Sociocultural Approaches to Language and Literacy: an Interactionist Perspective, eds. John-Steiner, V., Panofsky, C.P. & Smith, L.W.. New York (NY): Cambridge University Press, 170–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar