Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:57:50.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The influence of carbohydrate on cognitive performance: a critical evaluation from the perspective of glycaemic load

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 March 2009

Mary B. Gilsenan*
Affiliation:
Unilever Food and Health Research Institute, Unilever R&D Vlaardingen, Olivier van Noortlaan 120, PO Box 114, 3130 ACVlaardingen, The Netherlands
Eveline A. de Bruin
Affiliation:
Unilever Food and Health Research Institute, Unilever R&D Vlaardingen, Olivier van Noortlaan 120, PO Box 114, 3130 ACVlaardingen, The Netherlands
Louise Dye
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, LeedsLS2 9JT, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Mary B. Gilsenan, fax +44 1372 378209, email mary.gilsenan@unilever.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Links between nutrition and cognition are widely acknowledged. Within the context of short-term cognitive performance, carbohydrate has been the dietary component most commonly investigated. The majority of studies investigating the influence of carbohydrate on cognitive performance have employed oral glucose drink interventions followed by measures of performance on cognitive tests. More recently, studies have investigated the effect of different carbohydrates on cognitive performance rather than just pure glucose drinks. To date, studies have not been evaluated based on a standardised measure of glycaemic response, such as glycaemic load. The present review provides a critical evaluation of eight studies that have explored the relationships between food carbohydrate and cognitive performance and allow glycaemic load to be used as a basis for comparison. The key finding is that these provide insufficient evidence to support a consistent effect of glycaemic load on short-term cognitive performance. Future studies should employ consistent test methodologies and describe food interventions in more detail to facilitate meaningful comparisons and interpretations of results.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

The principle that foods can reliably modulate cognitive performance is receiving validation and experimental support(Reference Dye and Blundell1Reference Schmitt, Benton and Kallus3). As a consequence, the link between nutrition science and cognitive psychology is developing rapidly. Since glucose is the primary breakdown product of carbohydrate and the primary source of energy for the brain, its influence on cognitive performance has been the focus of much of the research in this area(Reference Dye and Blundell1, Reference Gibson and Green4Reference Riby6). The majority of studies investigating the link between glucose and cognitive performance have employed placebo-controlled oral glucose drink interventions followed by performance measures on behavioural tests (for example, memory, attention) with or without accompanying blood glucose measures(Reference Metzger7Reference Sunram-Lea, Foster and Durlach9). Administration of cognitive test batteries is commonly accompanied by measures of subjective states using visual analogue rating scales(Reference Dye and Blundell1, Reference Schmitt, Benton and Kallus3). Although the evidence is not consistent, a number of studies have reported beneficial effects of glucose on performance measures, in particular on delayed verbal memory(Reference Hoyland, Lawton and Dye10).

More recently, studies have investigated the effect of different carbohydrates on cognitive performance rather than just pure glucose drinks. Food interventions are typically described using terms such as glycaemic index (GI), glycaemic load (GL), the ratio of slowly to rapidly available glucose, the proportion of simple to complex carbohydrate, or the amount of rapidly v. slowly digested carbohydrate. Although more ecologically valid than pure glucose manipulations, different expressions used for the glycaemic potency of interventions render a direct comparison of results between studies difficult.

Both the quality (for example, type, nature, source) and quantity of a carbohydrate are important determinants of its glycaemic response. As the GI by definition compares equal quantities of available carbohydrate, it provides a measure of carbohydrate quality not quantity(11, Reference Barclay, Brand-Miller and Wolever12). The GL is the product of a food's GI and the amount of carbohydrate per serving(Reference Venn, Wallace and Monro13). GL imparts information about carbohydrate quantity and reflects the glycaemic response of actual food portions. In healthy individuals, stepwise increases in GL have been shown to predict stepwise elevations in postprandial blood glucose and/or insulin response to specific foods(Reference Brand-Miller, Thomas and Swan14).

Although a number of cognition studies have employed different carbohydrate interventions, these have not been evaluated based on a standardised measure of glycaemic response. The present review critically examines studies that have explored the relationships between carbohydrate and cognition, from the perspective of GL as a basis for comparison.

Methodology

We first carried out an inventory of the literature to identify studies that examined effects of ingestion of foods of specific carbohydrate type on cognitive performance. The following were used as key words in PubMed: ‘glyc(a)emic index’, ‘glyc(a)emic load’, ‘glyc(a)emic response’, ‘breakfast* [not] program*’, ‘carbohydrate*’, ‘GI’, ‘GL’, and ‘cognition’, ‘cognitive performance’, ‘cognitive$’, ‘memory’, ‘attention’. Relevant studies cited in review articles and in papers found in PubMed were also examined. Human studies were included if the GL of interventions being compared was stated, or where there was sufficient information from which the GL of interventions could be reliably calculated, and the study included objective measures of cognitive performance. Studies that measured subjective states only (for example, mood, fatigue) or that used non-energy placebo interventions were excluded.

Where GL data were not already stated in the original publications, these were calculated as a product of the GI of interventions employed and amount of available carbohydrate per serving: GL =  (GI ×  carbohydrate (g))/100. This allowed us to evaluate studies on cognitive performance from the perspective of GL.

Results and discussion

Eight studies were identified based on inclusion criteria (Table 1). Three studies were conducted in children(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17) and three in young adults(Reference Benton, Ruffin and Lassel18Reference Nabb and Benton20). Two were conducted in elderly subjects(Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21, Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22) of which the latter included type 2 diabetics. An overnight fast was employed in all studies. Five of the eight studies meeting the inclusion criteria used a within-subject design; three used a between-subject design. None of the studies reported whether physical activity levels or evening meals the day before test days were controlled for. In total, sixteen cognitive tests were employed. Tests involving word list recall, used as a measure of verbal episodic memory, were used most frequently (Table 1). In addition, tests of selective attention, spatial memory and immediate memory were used. In all studies, except one, cognitive testing commenced between 15 and 60 min post-interventions. In one study(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16) cognitive testing began between 110 and 180 min post-interventions.

Table 1 The influence of glycaemic load (GL) on cognitive performance

GI, glycaemic index; M, males; F, females; WS, within-subject; WLR, word list recall; PR, paragraph recall; Trails/B, trail making part B adult form; T2D, type 2 diabetics; VPS, verbal paired associates; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; Trails A/B, trail making part A and B adult form; BS, between-subject; SAG, slowly available glucose; RAG, rapidly available glucose; NS, not specified; NC, not calculated; RIPT, rapid information processing task; SR, spatial recall; NWM, numeric working memory.

* GL values were calculated as a product of the GI and the glycaemic fraction (sum of SAG and RAG). Other tasks were performed but their results were not reported.

Vigilance and reaction time tests were executed but not reported, as the nature of the breakfast did not influence the findings.

An arbitrary cut-off of 5 mmol/l (fasting blood glucose) was used to define ‘good’ and ‘poor’ glucose regulators. Given that the data were presented from the perspective of macronutrient compositions, it is difficult to provide a reliable interpretation of results in terms of GL. With regard to the vigilance test, ten cases were removed from the sample as they had responded in an indiscriminate way and produced a long series of errors. The results of simple and choice reaction times of the vigilance task were not reported.

§ Published GI values were based on breakfast cereals consumed with 250 ml milk. Results pertain to factor scores. All data from individual tests were not presented.

In two studies, meal interventions were described in terms of GL(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16, Reference Nabb and Benton20). For five studies, GL values were calculated(Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17Reference Benton and Nabb19, Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21, Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22) and for one(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15) GL values were estimated from international GI/GL tables(Reference Foster-Powell, Holt and Brand-Miller23). Of the five studies in which GL values were calculated, four documented the GI of food interventions(Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17Reference Benton and Nabb19, Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21) and GI values (estimated) from the fifth were provided by the authors of the original publication(Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22). The GL values of interventions ranged from 3 to 71 (Table 1).

Thus, eight studies were compared based on GL. In one study, there was no effect of three different test foods (GL 18 v. 59 v. 71) unless controlling post hoc for β cell function(Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21). In another (GL 28 v. 50), performance on three memory tests (digit span and delayed word list and paragraph recall) was significantly better in the condition with the lower absolute GL(Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22). However, this finding should be interpreted with caution as the study was conducted in elderly diabetics and might not be directly applicable to healthy subjects. Furthermore, the magnitude of the difference between absolute GL values of the conditions used is similar to that in the Kaplan et al. study(Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21) wherein no differential effects were reported in healthy elderly subjects.

Of the studies conducted in healthy young adults, breakfasts high in slowly available glucose (GL 44) had a positive effect on verbal memory compared with breakfasts high in rapidly available glucose (GL 66)(Reference Benton, Ruffin and Lassel18, Reference Benton and Nabb19). Although in both studies the effect on memory was reported similarly (namely combined scores for immediate and delayed recall), it is interesting to note that two interventions with similar GL (44 v. 66) elicited differential effects on memory recall. Furthermore, in the latter study(Reference Benton and Nabb19), the memory effects were only observed in subjects who had consumed alcohol the previous evening. It is unclear why this is the case. Whereas alcoholic beverage consumption has been shown to lower postprandial glycaemia before and during a meal(Reference Brand-Miller, Fatima and Middlemiss24), in the Benton & Nabb study(Reference Benton and Nabb19) alcohol consumed the previous evening did not influence blood glucose levels the following morning. One explanation could be that beneficial effects observed may relate to relief of hangover or withdrawal effects of alcohol rather than to a beneficial effect of one breakfast per se.

In the third study in young adults investigating the effect of eight different breakfasts on performance (insufficient information presented to provide a reliable estimate of GL of the individual conditions), subjects with better glucose tolerance performed better on a memory task but worse on a vigilance task following a lower-GL meal(Reference Nabb and Benton20).

However, it is important to note that an arbitrary cut-off (5 mmol/l) for fasting blood glucose was used to define whether subjects had poorer or better glucose tolerance, to provide an adequate sample size for statistical purposes. This is not aligned with international criteria used to define glycaemic states(25). Further, a between-subject design was employed.

Of the studies conducted in children, a positive effect on memory was reported in a lower-GL breakfast cereal condition (GL 7) compared with a higher-GL breakfast cereal condition (GL 23). This effect was based on combined scores from several memory tests(Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17). A second study showed that two different breakfast cereals (GL 15) both prevented a decline in memory over the course of the morning compared with a glucose drink and fasting conditions(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15). The effect was found on a factor score composed of several memory tests. In a third, no effect on memory was reported when three different test foods (GL 3 v. 12 v. 18) were compared(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16). It is interesting to note that the GL values of two conditions of the latter study (GL 12 and 18) are within a similar range to the GL in the two former studies, wherein positive effects on memory were reported(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15, Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17).

Of the five studies that measured attention, all three studies in children indicated a positive influence of lower-GL breakfasts on cognitive performance. One reported a positive effect of two breakfast cereals (GL 15)(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15) and another reported a positive effect on a lower GL intervention (GL 7)(Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy17); both effects were based on factor scores composed of several attention tests. In a third study in children, in-depth (correlation) analyses indicated that those consuming a lower GL were less likely to display lapses of attention(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16). Furthermore, when performance on a commercial video game was compared between three test foods (GL 3 v. 12 v. 18), performance was reportedly lower following one of the test foods (GL 18) but not the other two(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16). The two further studies performed in an elderly population reported no differential effects of GL on attention(Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21, Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22).

Besides memory and attention, almost no other tests were used in these studies. Effects of GL on other cognitive domains such as executive function remain unstudied. Indeed, many tests were selected without a clear rationale, and in four of the eight studies included in the present review, cognitive data from at least one test were not reported or not alluded to(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15, Reference Benton, Ruffin and Lassel18Reference Nabb and Benton20). One study, conducted with children, also monitored childhood behaviour as perceived by teachers as a subjective measure of cognitive performance(Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16). In the lowest GL condition (GL 3), more time was spent working on a classroom task at hand compared with two other conditions (GL 12 and 18).

Five studies included blood glucose measures. In two of the studies that reported a beneficial effect on memory, blood glucose levels had returned to baseline before a treatment effect was observed(Reference Benton, Ruffin and Lassel18, Reference Benton and Nabb19). In addition, in three studies without blood glucose measures(Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson15, Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams16, Reference Papanikolaou, Palmer and Binns22), cognitive effects were mostly reported between 2 and 4 h after the intervention, which is probably also after the return of blood glucose levels to baseline. In the study by Kaplan et al. (Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21), overall performance did not differ with consumption of the different test foods, all of which elicited significant differences in glucose response curves. Findings such as these indicate that blood glucose per se might not be a reliable biomarker of performance measures, and question the traditional and intuitively appealing hypothesis that ingested glucose improves memory by directly increasing uptake of glucose to the brain.

Inter-individual differences in glucose tolerance have been posited as important in mediating nutritional effects on cognitive function. Elderly subjects have been shown to have poorer glycoregulatory control than young subjects which may account for memory enhancement following a glucose drink in elderly subjects compared with younger counterparts(Reference Manning, Parsons and Cotter26). In the study by Kaplan et al. (Reference Kaplan, Greenwood and Winocur21), included in the present review, poor β cell function predicted improvements in memory performance of healthy elderly subjects. Differences in glycaemic response between children and adults are also worthy of consideration. However, there appears to be no published studies that allow objective comparison of glycaemic response between these two population samples.

Taken together, these results show that there is insufficient evidence to support a consistent effect of GL on short-term cognitive performance. There are several factors to bear in mind when interpreting these findings. First, a small number of studies with non-homogeneous population samples met the criteria for which behavioural measures could be compared based on GL. Second, there is a considerable amount of inter-study methodological variability. Moreover, there appears to be a lack of a compelling mechanistic hypothesis upon which GL might affect behaviour.

As apparent from Table 1, there is a considerable amount of inter-study methodological variability with regard to dietary restrictions the day before testing, the use of between- or within-subject design, the cognitive domain examined, the number and type of cognitive tasks in a given test battery, the temporal distribution of cognitive tests, and the temporal distribution of blood sampling. This variability serves to complicate direct comparisons of results across studies. Indeed, inter-study variability in methodological designs is frequently acknowledged as an inherent source of uncertainty when interpreting results within the general realm of nutrition and cognitive performance(Reference Gibson and Green4, Reference Riby6, Reference Dye, Lluch and Blundell27, Reference Lieberman28). Furthermore, it is unclear whether physical activity or the composition of food consumed the evening before testing was controlled for, both of which could influence glycaemic responses(Reference Brouns, Bjorck and Frayn29). Meals with a low GI produce better glucose tolerance the following morning compared with evening meals of a high GI(Reference Wolever, Jenkins and Ocana30, Reference Granfeldt, Wu and Bjorck31), and acute physical exercise can increase muscle uptake on the following day(Reference Malkova, Evans and Frayn32). As a compromise between the need to minimise respondent burden and the need to impose strict standardisation procedures before the test day, it is generally recommended that the same meal of choice be consumed the evening before each test day, and to avoid rigorous physical activity(Reference Brouns, Bjorck and Frayn29).

Despite methodological differences in the studies reviewed, the results described above allow us to speculate on the involvement of various physiological processes in the observed cognitive effects.

The capacity of the brain to store energy is limited and is strictly regulated within narrow boundaries(Reference Peters, Schweiger and Pellerin33). Further, as brain activity is unaffected by variation in brain extracellular glucose levels (except in the case of extreme hypoglycaemia), changes in brain extracellular glucose following changes in blood glucose are unlikely to affect overall brain function(Reference Messier5). In light of this, several hypotheses by which glucose might influence cognitive function have been proposed(Reference Messier5, Reference McNay and Gold34). There is convincing evidence that astrocytes might play an important role in energy regulation. These star-shaped glial cells, which surround neurons and lie in close proximity to the cerebral vasculature, are believed to constitute a likely site of glucose uptake as it crosses the blood–brain barrier(Reference Pellerin and Magistretti35). It is hypothesised that during neuronal activation, glucose is taken up by astrocytes, converted into lactate (by glycolysis), which is then released into the extracellular space to be taken up as an energy substrate by neurons(Reference Tsacopoulos and Magistretti36). The discovery of monocarboxylate (for example, lactate) transporters on both astrocytes and neurons(Reference Pierre and Pellerin37) lends support for this hypothesis.

As many of the brain's neurotransmitters are derived from glucose metabolism (for example, acetylcholine is derived from acetyl CoA, γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) is derived from glutamate), glucose may also influence cognitive function by enhancing neurotransmitter synthesis during periods of neuronal activity(Reference Messier, Durkin and Mrabet38). It has been hypothesised that neurons rely on glial supplies of tricarboxylic acid intermediates for this process(Reference McNay and Gold34).

A proposed peripheral action of glucose on memory could involve a neural signal triggered when glucose is transported into cells(Reference Messier5). This supposition is supported by the fact that peripheral injection of fructose, a monosaccharide sugar which does not cross the blood–brain barrier, and which does not elicit a significant rise in blood glucose, was shown to improve memory in rats(Reference Messier and White39). Further, injection of 3-o-methylglucose, a glucose analogue which has the same affinity for glucose transporters, but which is not metabolised once inside cells, was also shown to improve memory in rats(Reference Messier and White39).

GL is influenced by several factors that relate to the food itself (i.e. food components such as the nature of starch, content of fat, protein and fibre), eating behaviour (i.e. rate of ingestion, frequency of food intake, composition of a meal) and physiological factors (i.e. gastric emptying rate, intra- and inter-individual variation in glycaemic response and hormonal responses)(11, Reference Arvidsson-Lenner, Asp and Axelsen40). It is plausible that hormonal responses in particular have the potential to affect brain function and behaviour either through peripheral or central mechanisms. A vagotomy in rats was shown to attenuate memory-enhancing effects of peripherally injected peptide hormones, suggesting that gastrointestinal hormones could activate a detection mechanism which could relay neural signals to the central nervous system to influence cognitive processes(Reference Flood and Morley41). Recent evidence suggests that circulating ghrelin crosses the blood–brain barrier from the periphery where it binds to neurons, alters neuronal morphology, and affects the generation of long-term potentiation and behavioural outputs(Reference Diano, Farr and Benoit42).

Insulin also crosses the blood–brain barrier from the periphery(Reference Park43); improvements in cognitive function have been observed following the infusion of insulin in healthy adults(Reference Kern, Peters and Freuhwald-Schultes44). The corticosteroid hormone cortisol has also been suggested as a potential mediator of an association between glucose and cognition(Reference Gibson and Green4). Receptors binding cortisol are abundant in the hippocampus, a brain region strongly implicated in delayed memory, and there is evidence from both animal and human studies that glucocorticoids (for example, cortisol) influence memory(Reference Het, Ramlow and Wolf45). However, as several gastrointestinal hormones are typically released in response to food consumption, it is unclear to what extent all of them would exert an effect simultaneously.

Besides these, other factors could influence cognitive performance via an indirect effect on blood glucose or otherwise. Circulating glucose is higher after a palatable meal than after a meal composed of the same constituents presented in a non-palatable form(Reference Sawaya, Fuss and Dallal46). Furthermore, potential fluctuations in performance due to fatigue, hunger, physical discomfort, changes in mood and motivation are also acknowledged(Reference Schmitt, Benton and Kallus3). Thus, cognitive testing should ideally be accompanied by subjective measures of some or all of these states. As subjective evaluations of performance can interact with expectations and compensatory effort, these should ideally be measured as well.

Whereas a general consensus on likely underlying mechanism(s) appears far from being attained, the above-mentioned hypotheses and confounding factors illustrate that there is not a clear-cut relationship between glycaemic response, brain glucose and performance measures. This may account, at least in part, for an inconsistent effect of GL on short-term cognitive performance observed in the present review. Furthermore, studies investigating the effect of carbohydrate at the psychophysiological level using event-related potentials have not been able to provide further insights to help understand behavioural outcomes(Reference De Bruin and Gilsenan47).

To our surprise, few studies fulfilled the criteria to allow a comparison of performance measures from the perspective of GL. Of the studies selected based on our search criteria, two were excluded as both employed non-energy placebo-controlled interventions for comparisons(Reference Greenwood, Hebblethwaithe and Kaplan48, Reference Mahoney, Taylor and Kanarek49). In addition, two were excluded as interventions were described as having a high or low GI without specifying the absolute values, and there was insufficient product information documented to allow a reliable estimation of their GL from international tables(Reference Mahoney, Taylor and Kanarek50). Nevertheless, it is interesting that the results of the two latter studies indicate evidence of a beneficial effect of an oatmeal breakfast cereal (low to medium GI) compared with a ready-to-eat breakfast cereal (high GI) on tests of immediate memory (backward digit span only; in girls but not in boys) and on tests of attention. The beneficial effect on attention was only detected in two of four outcome measures of the auditory version of an attention test, not in a visual version.

The GL values estimated in the present review represent the best possible estimate based on available information. Whereas information on GI values (In the majority of studies, GI values were predicted from international tables. In some studies, it was not apparent whether GI values were predicted or measured.) and available carbohydrate content were provided in the majority of studies, in some cases, GL values were predicted from international GI/GL tables. In one study, GL values were calculated based on the amount of carbohydrate rather than the amount of available carbohydrate per se, an effect that could result in overestimation of GL values(Reference Nabb and Benton20). In addition, the mode of expression of available carbohydrate is a source of variation between studies(Reference Livesey51).

Finally, the above-mentioned studies refer to acute interventions. The extent to which any beneficial cognitive effects reported would persist following habitual consumption over a longer period is less clear. To date, few studies have investigated the effects of longer-term consumption of carbohydrate on cognitive performance. One study investigated the effect of 14 d consumption of inulin compared with a placebo in healthy adults(Reference Smith52). No differential effects on attention were found. In another, saccharide intake (estimated using a 3 d food diary) was positively correlated with verbal memory recall in middle-aged adults(Reference Best, Kemps and Bryan53) and, in a third, saccharide intake (as assessed by FFQ) was related to better self-reported memory functioning, after controlling for health and demographic factors(Reference Best, Kemps and Bryan54).

Conclusion and recommendations

At present, there is insufficient evidence to demonstrate a consistent directional effect of GL on short-term cognitive performance. Future studies should employ consistent methodologies to facilitate meaningful comparisons and interpretation of results. Such methodologies should include, as a minimum, a clear rationale for the selection of a given cognitive domain and/or test, sufficient detail about the carbohydrate composition (for example, GI, specification of carbohydrate type and supplier if possible) to allow reliable estimation of glycaemic response of the interventions employed, more transparency with regard to reporting of pre-test day standardisation procedures and more transparency when reporting results. Further, studies should include consideration of mechanistic hypotheses with respect to rationales and interpretation of results. This would facilitate comparison of findings across studies and help towards elucidation of underlying mechanisms to provide more robust scientific substantiation of claims in this area.

Acknowledgements

The present review was supported by Unilever, which has an interest in developing food products to improve cognitive performance. M. B. G. and E. A. de B. are employed by the Unilever Food and Health Research Institute. M. B. G. performed the literature search, and both M. B. G. and E. A. de B. wrote the review. L. D. provided expert input and guidance.

None of the authors declares a conflict of interest.

References

1Dye, L & Blundell, J (2002) Functional foods: psychological and behavioural functions. Br J Nutr 88, Suppl. 2, S187S211.Google Scholar
2Westenhoefer, J, Bellisle, F, Blundell, JE, et al. (2004) PASSCLAIM – mental state and performance. Eur J Nutr 43, S85S117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3Schmitt, JAJ, Benton, D & Kallus, KW (2005) General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr 44, 459464.Google Scholar
4Gibson, EL & Green, MW (2002) Nutritional influences on cognitive function: mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutr Res Rev 15, 169206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5Messier, C (2004) Glucose improvement of memory: a review. Eur J Pharmacol 490, 3357.Google Scholar
6Riby, LM (2004) The impact of age and task domain on cognitive performance: a meta-analytical review of the glucose facilitation effect. Brain Impairment 5, 145165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7Metzger, MM (2000) Glucose enhancement of a facial recognition task in young adults. Physiol Behav 68, 549553.Google Scholar
8Scholey, AB, Harper, S & Kennedy, DO (2001) Cognitive demand and blood glucose. Physiol Behav 73, 585592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Sunram-Lea, SI, Foster, JK, Durlach, P, et al. (2002) Investigation into the significance of task difficulty and divided allocation of resources on the glucose memory facilitation effect. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 160, 387397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Hoyland, A, Lawton, CL & Dye, L (2008) Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: a systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32, 7285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Food and Agricultural Organization & World Health Organization (1998) Carbohydrates in Human Nutrition. Report of a Joint FAO/WHO Expert Consultation, Rome, 14–18 April 1997. FAO Food and Nutrition Paperno. 66. Rome: FAO.Google Scholar
12Barclay, AW, Brand-Miller, J & Wolever, TMS (2005) Glycemic index, glycemic load, and glycemic response are not the same. Diabetes Care 28, 18391840.Google Scholar
13Venn, BJ, Wallace, AJ, Monro, JA, et al. (2006) The glycaemic load estimated from the glycaemic index does not differ greatly from that measured using a standard curve in healthy volunteers. J Nutr 136, 13771381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Brand-Miller, JC, Thomas, M, Swan, V, et al. (2003) Physiological validation of the concept of glycemic load in lean young adults. J Nutr 133, 27282732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Wesnes, KA, Pincock, C, Richardson, D, et al. (2003) Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite 41, 329331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Benton, D, Maconie, A & Williams, C (2007) The influence of the glycaemic load of breakfast on the behaviour of children in school. Physiol Behav 92, 717724.Google Scholar
17Ingwersen, J, Defeyter, MA, Kennedy, DO, et al. (2007) A low glycaemic index breakfast cereal preferentially prevents children's cognitive performance from declining throughout the morning. Appetite 49, 240244.Google Scholar
18Benton, D, Ruffin, MP, Lassel, T, et al. (2003) The delivery rate of carbohydrates affects cognitive performance in both rats and humans. Psychopharmacology 166, 8690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Benton, D & Nabb, S (2004) Breakfasts that release glucose at different speeds interact with previous alcohol intake to influence cognition and mood before and after lunch. Behav Neurosci 118, 936943.Google Scholar
20Nabb, S & Benton, D (2006) The influence on cognition of the interaction between the macro-nutrient content of breakfast and glucose tolerance. Physiol Behav 87, 1623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Kaplan, RJ, Greenwood, CE, Winocur, G, et al. (2000) Cognitive performance is associated with glucose regulation in healthy elderly persons and can be enhanced with glucose and dietary carbohydrates. Am J Clin Nutr 72, 825836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Papanikolaou, Y, Palmer, H, Binns, MA, et al. (2006) Better cognitive performance following a low-glycaemic-index compared with a high-glycaemic-index carbohydrate meal in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia 49, 855862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Foster-Powell, K, Holt, SHA & Brand-Miller, JC (2002) International table of glycemic index and glycemic load values: 2002. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Brand-Miller, JC, Fatima, K, Middlemiss, C, et al. (2007) Effect of alcoholic beverages on postprandial glycemia and insulinemia in lean, young adults. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 15451551.Google Scholar
25World Health Organization (2006) Definition and Diagnosis of Diabetes Mellitus and Intermediate Hyperglycaemia. Report of a WHO/IDF Consultation. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
26Manning, CA, Parsons, MW, Cotter, EM, et al. (1997) Glucose effects on declarative and nondeclarative memory in healthy elderly and young adults. Psychobiology 25, 103108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27Dye, L, Lluch, A & Blundell, JE (2000) Macronutrients and mental performance. Nutrition 16, 10211034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Lieberman, HR (2003) Nutrition, brain function and cognitive performance. Appetite 40, 245254.Google Scholar
29Brouns, F, Bjorck, I, Frayn, KN, et al. (2005) Glycaemic index methodology. Nutr Res Rev 18, 145171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Wolever, TM, Jenkins, DJ, Ocana, AM, et al. (1988) Second-meal effect: low-glycemic-index foods eaten at dinner improve subsequent breakfast glycemic response. Am J Clin Nutr 48, 10411047.Google Scholar
31Granfeldt, Y, Wu, X & Bjorck, I (2006) Determination of glycaemic index; some methodological aspects related to the analysis of carbohydrate load and characteristics of the previous evening meal. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 104112.Google Scholar
32Malkova, D, Evans, RD, Frayn, KN, et al. (2000) Prior exercise and postprandial substrate extraction across the human leg. Am J Physiol 279, 10201028.Google ScholarPubMed
33Peters, A, Schweiger, U, Pellerin, L, et al. (2004) The selfish brain: competition for energy resources. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28, 143180.Google Scholar
34McNay, EC & Gold, PE (2002) Food for thought: fluctuations in brain extracellular glucose provide insight into the mechanisms of memory modulation. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 1, 264280.Google Scholar
35Pellerin, L & Magistretti, PJ (1994) Glutamate uptake into astrocytes stimulates aerobic glycolysis: a mechanism coupling neuronal activity to glucose utilization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91, 1062510629.Google Scholar
36Tsacopoulos, M & Magistretti, PJ (1996) Metabolic coupling between glia and neurons. J Neurosci 16, 877885.Google Scholar
37Pierre, K & Pellerin, L (2005) Monocarboxylate transporters in the central nervous system: distribution, regulation and function. J Neurochem 94, 114.Google Scholar
38Messier, C, Durkin, T, Mrabet, O, et al. (1990) Memory-improving action of glucose: indirect evidence for a facilitation of hippocampal acetylcholine synthesis. Behav Brain Res 39, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39Messier, C & White, NM (1987) Memory improvement by glucose, fructose and two glucose analogues: a possible effect on peripheral glucose transport. Behav Neural Biol 48, 104127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Arvidsson-Lenner, R, Asp, NG, Axelsen, M, et al. (2004) Glycaemic index. Scan J Nutr 48, 8489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41Flood, JF & Morley, JE (1988) Effects of bombesin and gastrin-releasing peptide on memory processing. Brain Res 460, 314322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42Diano, S, Farr, SA, Benoit, SC, et al. (2005) Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nat Neurosci 9, 381388.Google Scholar
43Park, CR (2001) Cognitive effects of insulin in the central nervous system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25, 311323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44Kern, W, Peters, A, Freuhwald-Schultes, B, et al. (2001) Improving influence of insulin on cognitive function in humans. Clin Neuroendocrinol 74, 270280.Google Scholar
45Het, S, Ramlow, G & Wolf, OT (2005) A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 771784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46Sawaya, AL, Fuss, PJ, Dallal, GE, et al. (2001) Meal palatability, substrate oxidation and blood glucose in young and older men. Physiol Behav 72, 512.Google Scholar
47De Bruin, EA & Gilsenan, MB (2008) Effects of food energy on cognitive performance: no support from event-related potentials (yet?). Br J Nutr, doi:10.1017/S0007114508051702 (epublication ahead of print version 20 August 2008).Google Scholar
48Greenwood, CE, Hebblethwaithe, S, Kaplan, RJ, et al. (2003) Carbohydrate-induced memory impairment in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 26, 19611966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49Mahoney, CR, Taylor, HA & Kanarek, RB (2007) Effect of an afternoon confectionery snack on cognitive processes critical to learning. Physiol Behav 90, 344352.Google Scholar
50Mahoney, CR, Taylor, HA, Kanarek, RB, et al. (2005) Effect of breakfast composition on cognitive processes in elementary school children. Physiol Behav 85, 635645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51Livesey, G (2005) Low-glycaemic diets and health: implications for obesity. Proc Nutr Soc 64, 105113.Google Scholar
52Smith, AP (2005) The concept of well-being: relevance to nutrition research. Br J Nutr 93 Suppl. 1, S1S5.Google Scholar
53Best, T, Kemps, E & Bryan, J (2005) Effects of saccharides on brain function and cognitive performance. Nutr Rev 63, 409418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54Best, T, Kemps, E & Bryan, J (2007) A role for dietary saccharides in cognitive performance. Nutr Neurosci 10, 113120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 The influence of glycaemic load (GL) on cognitive performance