Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Predictors of lexical retrieval in Hindi–English bilingual speakers

  • Keerthi Ramanujan (a1) and Brendan S. Weekes (a1) (a2)

Abstract

When bilinguals name pictures while in ‘monolingual mode’, we expect that under conditions of language-constraint and no cognate facilitation, factors influencing lexical retrieval in monolinguals ought to exert similar effects on bilinguals. To this end, we carried out a L1-only naming task on early Hindi–English bilinguals. Results of linear mixed effects analysis reveal AoA, Familiarity, Image Agreement and Codability (availability of alternate names) to be the most significant predictors of lexical retrieval speed for early bilinguals, confirming our expectations. However, we report, for the first time, a by-subject variation in Codability for bilinguals. Implications of the results are discussed in the context of current theories of bilingual lexical access and competition. In preparation for this study, Hindi norms from bilinguals for items in the Snodgrass and Vanderwart set have been established, which will be of use for stimuli selection in experimental studies involving bilinguals.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Keerthi Ramanujan, Email: keerthi.ram@hku.hk

References

Hide All
Abutalebi, J and Green, DW (2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(4), 557582.
Alario, FX, Ferrand, L, Laganaro, M, New, B, Frauenfelder, UH and Segui, J (2004) Predictors of picture naming speed. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc 36(1), 140155.
Álvarez, B and Cuetos, F (2007) Objective age of acquisition norms for a set of 328 words in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods 39(3), 377383.
Baayen, RH, Davidson, DJ and Bates, DM (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4), 390412.
Bakhtiar, M, Nilipour, R and Weekes, BS (2013) Predictors of timed picture naming in Persian. Behavior Research Methods 45(3), 834–41.
Barr, DJ, Levy, R, Scheepers, C and Tily, HJ (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3), 255278.
Barry, C, Morrison, CM and Ellis, AW (1997) Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement. The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology 50A(3), 560585.
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2014) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R Package Version 1(7).
Bates, E, D'Amico, S, Jacobsen, T, Székely, A, Andonova, E, Devescovi, A, Herron, D, Lu, C, Pechmann, T and Pléh, C (2003) Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 10(2), 344380.
Bialystok, E and Viswanathan, M (2009) Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112(3), 494500.
Bonin, P, Chalard, M, Méot, A and Fayol, M (2002) The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies. British Journal of Psychology 93(1), 89114.
Boukadi, M, Zouaidi, C and Wilson, MA (2016) Norms for name agreement, familiarity, subjective frequency, and imageability for 348 object names in Tunisian Arabic. Behavior Research Methods 48(2), 585599.
Clingingsmith, D (2014) Industrialization and Bilingualism in India. Journal of Human Resources 49(1), 73109.
Costa, A, Miozzo, M and Caramazza, A (1999) Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual's two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language 41(3), 365397.
Costa, A, Caramazza, A and Sebastian-Galles, N (2000) The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26(5), 1283.
Craney, TA and Surles, JG (2002) Model-Dependent Variance Inflation Factor Cutoff Values. Quality Engineering 14(3), 391403.
Cuetos, F, Ellis, AW and Alvarez, B (1999) Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 31(4), 650658.
Davies, R, Barbón, A and Cuetos, F (2013) Lexical and semantic age-of-acquisition effects on word naming in Spanish. Memory & Cognition 41(2), 297311.
Dimitropoulou, M, Duñabeitia, JA, Blitsas, P and Carreiras, M (2009) A standardized set of 260 pictures for Modern Greek: Norms for name agreement, age of acquisition, and visual complexity. Behavior Research Methods 41(2), 584589.
Ellis, AW and Morrison, CM (1998) Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24(2), 515.
Finkbeiner, M, Gollan, TH and Caramazza, A (2006) Lexical access in bilingual speakers: What's the (hard) problem? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9(2), 153166.
Forster, KI and Forster, JC (2003) DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35(1), 116124.
Freedman, M, Alladi, S, Chertkow, H, Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM, Phillips, NA, Duggirala, V, Raju, SB and Bak, TH (2014) Delaying Onset of Dementia: Are Two Languages Enough? Behavioural Neurology 18.
Glaser, WR (1992) Picture naming. Cognition 42(1-3), 61105.
Gollan, TH and Acenas, L-AR (2004) What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and Tagalog-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30(1), 246.
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI, Cera, C and Sandoval, TC (2008) More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language 58(3), 787814.
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI, Fennema-Notestine, C and Morris, SK (2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition 33(7), 12201234.
Green, DW (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1(02), 67.
Grosjean, F (2001) The bilingual's language modes. In Nicol, J (Ed.). One Mind, Two Languages: Bilingual Language Processing (pp. 122). Oxford: Blackwell.
Hall, ML (2011) Bilingual picture–word studies constrain theories of lexical selection. Frontiers in psychology 2.
Hoshino, N and Kroll, JF (2008) Cognate effects in picture naming: does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition 106(1), 501–11.
Ivanova, I and Costa, A (2008) Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica 127(2), 277288.
Izura, C and Ellis, AW (2002) Age of acquisition effects in word recognition and production in first and second languages. Psicológica 23(2), 245281.
Johnson, CJ, Paivio, A and Clark, JM (1996) Cognitive components of picture naming. Psychological Bulletin 120(1), 113139.
Kessler, B, Treiman, R and Mullennix, J (2002) Phonetic Biases in Voice Key Response Time Measurements. Journal of Memory and Language 47(1), 145171.
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, PB and Christensen, RHB (2015) lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models, 2015. URL , 0–2.
La Heij, W (2005) Selection processes in monolingual and bilingual lexical access. Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, New York: Oxford University Press.
Levelt, WJM (1999) Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(6), 223232.
Liu, Y, Hao, M, Li, P and Shu, H (2011) Timed picture naming norms for Mandarin Chinese. PLoS One 6(1), e16505.
Marian, V and Spivey, M (2003) Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within-and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6(02), 97115.
Martin, CD, Dering, B, Thomas, EM and Thierry, G (2009) Brain potentials reveal semantic priming in both the ‘active'and the ‘non-attended'language of early bilinguals. NeuroImage 47(1), 326333.
Morrison, CM, Ellis, AW and Quinlan, PT (1992) Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory & Cognition 20(6), 705714.
Nag, S (2014) Alphabetism and the science of reading: from the perspective of the akshara languages. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 866.
Nishimoto, T, Miyawaki, K, Ueda, T, Une, Y and Takahashi, M (2005) Japanese normative set of 359 pictures. Behavior Research Methods 37(3), 398416.
Paivio, A, Clark, JM, Digdon, N and Bons, T (1989) Referential processing: reciprocity and correlates of naming and imaging. Memory & Cognition 17(2), 163–74.
R Core Team (2015) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/
Raman, I, Raman, E and Mertan, B (2014) A standardized set of 260 pictures for Turkish: Norms of name and image agreement, age of acquisition, visual complexity, and conceptual familiarity. Behavior Research Methods 46(2), 588595.
Raman, I, Raman, E, İkier, S, Kilecioğlu, E, Uzun Eroğlu, D and Zeyveli, Ş (2018) Differential effects of age of acquisition and frequency on memory: evidence from free recall of pictures and words in Turkish. Writing Systems Research. 114.
Rastle, K and Davis, MH (2002) On the complexities of measuring naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 28(2), 307314.
Roberts, PM, Garcia, LJ, Desrochers, A and Hernandez, D (2002) English performance of proficient bilingual adults on the Boston Naming Test. Aphasiology 16(4-6), 635645.
Rossion, B and Pourtois, G (2004) Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's object pictorial set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception 33(2), 217236.
Sahgal, A (1991) Patterns of language use in a bilingual setting in India. In Cheshire, J (Ed.), English Around the World: Sociolinguistic Perspectives (pp. 299306). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Severens, E, Van Lommel, S, Ratinckx, E and Hartsuiker, RJ (2005) Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychologica 119(2), 159–87.
Snodgrass, JG and Vanderwart, M (1980) A Standardized Set of 260 Pictures: Norms for Name Agreement, Image Agreement, Familiarity and Visual Complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6(2), 174215.
Snodgrass, JG and Yuditsky, T (1996) Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 28(4), 516536.
Vaid, J and Gupta, A (2002) Exploring Word Recognition in a Semi-Alphabetic Script: The Case of Devanagari. Brain and Language 81(1-3), 679690.
Volkovyskaya, E, Raman, I and Baluch, B (2017) Age of acquisition (AoA) effect in monolingual Russian and bilingual Russian (L1)-English (L2) speakers in a free recall task. Writing Systems Research 9(2), 148163. http://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2017.1405136
Weekes, BS, Shu, H, Hao, M, Liu, Y and Tan, LH (2007) Predictors of timed picture naming in Chinese. Behavior Research Methods 39(2), 335342.
Zevin, JD and Seidenberg, MS (2002) Age of acquisition effects in word reading and other tasks. Journal of Memory and Language 47(1), 129.
Zuur, AF, Ieno, EN and Elphick, CS (2010) A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 1(1), 314.

Keywords

Type Description Title
PDF
Supplementary materials

Ramanujan and Weekes supplementary material
Ramanujan and Weekes supplementary material 1

 PDF (427 KB)
427 KB

Predictors of lexical retrieval in Hindi–English bilingual speakers

  • Keerthi Ramanujan (a1) and Brendan S. Weekes (a1) (a2)

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed