Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-08T14:37:55.930Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grammatical gender in bilingual Norwegian–Russian acquisition: The role of input and transparency*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2015

YULIA RODINA*
Affiliation:
University of Oslo
MARIT WESTERGAARD
Affiliation:
UiT The Arctic University of Norway
*
Address for correspondence: Yulia Rodina, University of Oslo, Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, P.O. Box 1102 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norwayyulia.rodina@iln.uio.no

Abstract

This paper investigates the role of parental input and transparency in the acquisition of two different gender systems, Norwegian and Russian, by bilingual children living in Norway. While gender in Russian is generally predictable from the morphophonological shape of the noun (with some exceptions), gender assignment in Norwegian is opaque. An experimental production study was carried out with two groups of bilinguals, children with one or two Russian-speaking parents, and monolingual controls (age 4;1–7;11). The findings show that both groups of bilinguals perform similarly to monolinguals in Norwegian, the majority language, despite the lack of transparency. In Russian, on the other hand, not only quantitative, but also qualitative differences are found in the data of the bilingual children with the least exposure to the language. These qualitative differences indicate that early age of onset is not sufficient to acquire phenomena such as gender; extensive input is necessary.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank our participants, their parents and the research assistants who worked with us on this project. We are grateful to Martin Corley, Björn Lundquist and Jeffrey Chrabaszcz for help with statistical analysis of the data, to the audience of GALA 2013, MIMS 2, TromSlang, and IASCL 2014 for insightful comments, and to three anonymous reviewers for their feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript. This research was supported in part by a grant from the Tromsø Research Foundation and by the Research Council of Norway through its Centers of Excellence funding scheme, project number 223265.

References

Anderssen, M. (2006). The acquisition of compositional definiteness in Norwegian. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Tromsø.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M, Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. ArXiv e-print; submitted to Journal of Statistical Software, http://arxiv.org/abs/1406.5823.Google Scholar
Blom, E., Polisenskà, D., & Unsworth, S. (2008a). The acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch. Second Language Research, 24, 259265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., Polisenskà, D., & Weerman, F. (2008b). Articles, adjectives and the age of onset: The acquisition of Dutch grammatical gender. Second Language Research, 24, 297332.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1982). Gender in Russian: an account of gender specification and its relationship to declension. Russian Linguistics, 6, 197232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cornips, L., & Hulk, A. (2006). Between 2L1- and child L2 acquisition: An experimental study of bilingual Dutch. In Lleó, C. (eds.), Interfaces in Multilingualism: Acquisition and representation. Hamburg Studies on Multilingualism, pp. 155–137. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cornips, L., & Hulk, A. (2008). Factors of success and failure in the acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch. Second Language Research, 24, 267295.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2005). Early bilingual acquisition: Focus on morphosyntax and the separate development hypothesis. In Kroll, J. & De Groot, A. (eds.), The handbook of bilingualism, pp. 3048. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eichler, N., Jansen, V., & Müller, N. (2012). Gender acquisition in bilingual children: French-German, Italian-German, Spanish-German and Italian-French. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 550572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enger, H-O. (2004). On the relation between gender and declension: A diachronic perspective from Norwegian. Studies in Language, 28, 5182.Google Scholar
Faarlund, J. T., Lie, S., & Vannebo, K. I. (1997). Norsk referansegrammatikk. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002). Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children, pp. 207219. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2005). Minority language survival: Input factors influencing the acquisition of Welsh. In Cohen, J., McAlister, K., Rolstad, K. & MacSwan, J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, pp. 852874. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Genesee, F., & Nicoladis, E. (2007). Bilingual acquisition. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (eds.), Handbook of language development, pp. 324342. Oxford: England: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gvozdev, A. N. (1961). Formirovanie u rebenka grammatičeskogo stroja russkogo jazyka. [Language development of a Russian child] Moscow: APN RSFSR.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: MacMillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N. (2000). Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 227244.Google Scholar
Iosad, P. (2012). Vowel reduction in Russian: No phonetics in phonology. Journal of Linguistics, 48, 521571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasatkin, L. L. (eds.) (2005). Russkaja dialektologija [Russian dialiectology]. Moscow: Akademia.Google Scholar
Kempe, V., Brooks, P. J., Mironova, N., & Fedorova, O. (2003). Diminutivization supports gender acquisition in Russian children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 471485.Google Scholar
Klassert, A., & Gagarina, N. (2010). Der Einfluss des elterlichen Inputs auf die Sprachentwicklung bilingualer Kinder: Evidenz aus russischsprachigen Migrantenfamilien in Berlin [The influence of parental input on the language development of bilingual children: Evidence from Russian-speaking immigrant families in Berlin]. Diskurs Kindheits- und Jugendforschung 4, 413425.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., Müller, N., & Cantone, K. F. (2002). Gender in monolingual and bilingual first language acquisition: Comparing Italian and French. Lingue e Linguaggio, 1, 107149.Google Scholar
Lødrup, H. (2011). Hvor mange genus er det i Oslo-dialekten? [How many genders are there in the Oslo dialect?] Maal og Minne, 2, 120136.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (2011). First and second language acquisition: Parallels and differences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 2011.Google Scholar
Müller, N. (2000). Gender and number acquisition. In Unterbeck, B. & Rissanen, M. (eds.), Gender in grammar and cognition, pp. 351399. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Müller, N., & Hulk, A. (2001). Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 121.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2008). Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal, 6, 4071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popova, M. I. (1973). Grammatical elements of language in the speech of pre-preschool children. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development, pp. 269280. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Rodina, Y. (2007). What do asymmetries in children's performance tell us about the nature of their underlying knowledge? Nordlyd, 34, 230251.Google Scholar
Rodina, Y. (2014). Variation in the input: Child and caregiver in the acquisition of grammatical gender in Russian. Language Sciences, 43, 116132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2012). A cue-based approach to the acquisition of grammatical gender in Russian. Journal of Child Language, 39, 10771106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2013a). The acquisition of gender and declension class in a non-transparent system: Monolinguals and bilinguals. Studia Linguistica, 67, 4767.Google Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2013b). Two gender systems in one mind: The acquisition of grammatical gender in Norwegian–Russian bilinguals. In Siemund, P., Gogolin, I., Schulz, M. E. & Davydova, J. (eds.), Multilingualism and Language Diversity in Urban Areas: Acquisition, identities, space, education, pp. 95126, Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2015). Grammatical gender in Norwegian: Language acquisition and language change. Journal of Germanic Linguistics, 27.2, 145187.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M., Minkov, M., Dieser, E., Protassova, E., Moin, V., & Polinsky, M. (2014). Acquisition of Russian gender agreement by monolingual and bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, doi: 10.1177/1367006914544989.Google Scholar
Stöhr, A., Akpinar, D., Bianchi, G., & Kupisch, T. (2012). Gender marking in Italian–German heritage speakers and L2-learners of German. In Braunmueller, K. & Gabriel, C. (eds.), Multilingual Individuals Multilingual Societies (MIMS), pp. 153170, Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarasenkova, O. V. (2010). Acquisition of agreement and morphological features within the Noun Phrase in Russian. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Trosterud, T. (2001). Genustilordning i norsk er regelstyrt [Gender assignment in Norwegian is rule-based]. Norsk Lingvistisk Tidsskrift, 19, 2957.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., & Hulk, A. (2013). Grammatical gender and the notion of default: insights from language acquisition. Lingua, 137, 128144.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86110.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A., & Tsimpli, I. (2014). On the role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 765805.Google Scholar