Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

A comparison of verb and noun retrieval in Mandarin–English bilinguals with English-speaking monolinguals

  • RAN LI (a1), YASMEEN FAROQI-SHAH (a2) and MIN WANG (a2)

Abstract

The finding that noun production is slower and less accurate in bilinguals compared to monolinguals is well replicated, but not well understood. This study examined the two prominent theoretical accounts for this bilingual effect: weaker links and cross-language interference. Highly proficient Mandarin–English bilinguals and English-speaking monolinguals named pictures in which the effects of grammatical class, word frequency and translatability were examined. While bilinguals were slower overall than monolinguals in both L1 and L2, the magnitude of this bilingual effect was smaller for verbs than for nouns. Bilinguals showed a larger production advantage for high vs. low frequency words in their L2 relative to monolinguals and their L1. Bilinguals also showed an advantage for words with greater translatability, which did not differ across grammatical categories. The findings lend partial support to the weaker links account, and reveal cross-language facilitation rather than interference.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Ran Li, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, College of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences: Sargent College, 635 Commonwealth Avenue, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215rli92@bu.edu

Footnotes

Hide All

*This project was funded by the MCM fund for student research awarded to Ran Li by the Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of memory and language, 59 (4), 390412.
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3 (2), 1228.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of memory and language, 68 (3), 255278.
Barry, C., Hirsh, K. W., Johnston, R. A., & Williams, C. L. (2001). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the locus of repetition priming of picture naming. Journal of memory and language, 44 (3), 350375.
Bartram, D. J. (1974). The role of visual and semantic codes in object naming. Cognitive Psychology, 6 (3), 325356.
Bates, E., Federmeier, K., Herron, D., Iyer, G., Jacobsen, T., Pechmann, T., D'Amico, S., Devescovi, A., Wicha, N., Orozco-Figueroa, A., Kohnert, K., Gutierrez, G., Lu, C-C., Hung, D., Hsu, J., Tzeng, O., Andonova, E., Szekely, A., & Pléh, C. (2000). Introducing the CRL international picture-naming project (CRL-IPNP). Center for Research in Language Newsletter, 12 (1).
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child development, 70 (3), 636644.
Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 169181.
Birdsong, D., Gertken, L. M., & Amengual, M. (2012). Bilingual language profile: An easy-to-use instrument to assess bilingualism. COERLL, University of Texas at Austin.
Bloem, I., & La Heij, W. (2003). Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (3), 468488.
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22 (5), 633660.
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior research methods, 41 (4), 977990.
Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30 (5), 542579.
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese Word and Character Frequencies Based on Film Subtitles. PloS on, 5 (6), e10729.
Chen, H. C., & Ng, M. L. (1989). Semantic facilitation and translation priming effects in Chinese-English bilinguals. Memory & cognition, 17 (4), 454462.
Chen, P., Bobb, S. C., Hoshino, N., & Marian, V. (2017). Neural signatures of language co-activation and control in bilingual spoken word comprehension. Brain Research, 1665, 5064.
Colomé, A. (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals' speech production: Language-Specific or Language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language, 45 (4), 721736.
Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual's two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41 (3), 365397.
de Groot, A. M. (1992). Determinants of word translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18 (5), 10011018.
Dent, K., Johnston, R. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: a dual-task investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 34 (2), 282301.
Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: a lexical entrenchment account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 66 (5), 843863.
Duyck, W., Vanderelst, D., Desmet, T., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2008). The frequency effect in second-language visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 (4), 850855.
Emmorey, K., Petrich, J. A., & Gollan, T. H. (2013). Bimodal bilingualism and the frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of deaf studies and deaf education, 18 (1), 111.
Faroqi-Shah, Y. (2012). Grammatical category deficits in bilingual aphasia. Aspects of multilingual aphasia, 8, 158170. USA: Multilingual Matters.
Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Li, R. (in prep). The bilingual disadvantage differs by lexical category: a comparison of action and object naming.
Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Milman, L. (2015). Grammatical category mediates the bilingual disadvantage in word retrieval. Frontiers. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 53rd Annual Meeting.
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35 (1), 116124.
Gentner, D. (1981). Some interesting differences between verbs and nouns. Cognition and brain theory, 4 (2), 161178.
Gollan, T. H., & Acenas, L. A. R. (2004). What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish–English and Tagalog-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30 (1), 246269.
Gollan, T. H., & Silverberg, N. B. (2001). Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 4 (01), 6383.
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16 (4), 562576.
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (3), 787814.
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33 (7), 12201234.
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P. (2011). Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22 (9), 11551164.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 1 (02), 6781.
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., Hansen, P., Simonsen, H. G., Chiat, S., Bjekić, J., Blaziene, A., Chyl, K., Dabasinskiene, I., Engel de Abreu, P., Gagarina, N., Gavarro, A., Hakansson, G., Harel, E., Holm, E., Kapalkova, S., Kunnari, S., Levorato, C., Lindgren, J., Mieszkowska, K., Montes Salarich, L., Potgieter, A., Ribu, I., Ringblom, N., Rinker, T., Roch, M., Slancova, D., Southwood, F., Tedeschi, R., Tuncer, A. M., Unal-Logacev, O., Vuksanović, J. & Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Noun and verb knowledge in monolingual preschool children across 17 languages: Data from cross-linguistic lexical tasks (LITMUS-CLT). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 126.
Hermans, D. (2004). Between-language identity effects in picture-word interference tasks: A challenge for language-nonspecific or language-specific models of lexical access?. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8 (2), 115125.
Hermans, D., Bongaerts, T., De Bot, K., & Schreuder, R. (1998). Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language?. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 1 (03), 213229.
Hernández, M., Cano, A., Costa, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Juncadella, M., & Gascón-Bayarri, J. (2008). Grammatical category-specific deficits in bilingual aphasia. Brain and Language, 107 (1), 6880.
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 18 (4), 625658.
Huang, C-T. J. (1989). Pro-drop in Chinese: A generalized control theory. Springer Netherlands, 185214.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production?. Acta psychologica, 127 (2), 277288.
Jia, G., Kohnert, K., & Collado, J. (2006). Action naming in Spanish and English by sequential bilingual children and adolescents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49 (3), 588602.
Kauschke, C., & von Frankenberg, J. (2008). The differential influence of lexical parameters on naming latencies in German. A study on noun and verb picture naming. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 37 (4), 243257.
Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., & Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25 (4), 463492.
Klassert, A., Gagarina, N., & Kauschke, C. (2014). Object and action naming in Russian-and German-speaking monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (01), 7388.
Kohnert, K. (2002). Picture Naming in Early Sequential BilingualsA 1-Year Follow-Up. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45 (4), 759771.
Kohnert, K. J., Bates, E., & Hernandez, A. E. (1999). Balancing Bilinguals Lexical-Semantic Production and Cognitive Processing in Children Learning Spanish and English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42 (6), 14001413.
Kohnert, K. J., Hernandez, A. E., & Bates, E. (1998). Bilingual performance on the Boston Naming Test: preliminary norms in Spanish and English. Brain and language, 65 (3), 422440.
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of memory and language, 33 (2), 149174.
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44 (4), 978990.
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). Package ‘lmerTest’. R package version, 2 (0).
Lee, M. W., & Williams, J. N. (2001). Lexical access in spoken word production by bilinguals: Evidence from the semantic competitor priming paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (03), 233248.
Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44 (2), 325343.
Li, C.C., Wang, M., & Idsardi, W. (2015). The effect of orthographic form-cuing on phonological preparation unit in spoken word production. Memory and Cognition, 43 (4), 563578.
Li, P. (1996). Spoken word recognition of code-switched words by Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of memory and language, 35 (6), 757774.
Li, P., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Neural representations of nouns and verbs in Chinese: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21 (4), 15331541.
Macizo, P., Bajo, T., & Martín, M. C. (2010). Inhibitory processes in bilingual language comprehension: Evidence from Spanish–English interlexical homographs. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (2), 232244.
Mätzig, S., Druks, J., Masterson, J., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Noun and verb differences in picture naming: Past studies and new evidence. Cortex, 45 (6), 738758.
McCulloch, C. E., & Neuhaus, J. M. (2001). Generalized linear mixed models. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Monaghan, P., Chang, Y.-N., Welbourne, S., & Brysbaert, M. (2017). Exploring the relations between word frequency, language exposure, and bilingualism in a computational model of reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 121.
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 273281.
Prior, A., MacWhinney, B., & Kroll, J. F. (2007). Translation norms for English and Spanish: The role of lexical variables, word class, and L2 proficiency in negotiating translation ambiguity. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 10291038.
R Core Team (2015). RStudio: integrated development for R. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA URL http://www.rstudio.com.
Roberts, P. M., Garcia, L. J., Desrochers, A., & Hernandez, D. (2002). English performance of proficient bilingual adults on the Boston Naming Test. Aphasiology, 16 (4–6), 635645.
Romaine, S. (2008). 15 The Bilingual and Multilingual Community. The handbook of bilingualism, 8, 385403.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V. A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., & Ostrosky-Solí, F. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish–English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7 (1), 1724.
Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (02), 231252.
Schmid, M. S., & Köpke, B. (2009). L1 attrition and the mental lexicon. The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches, 209238.
Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65 (10), 19271944.
Sheng, L., McGregor, K. K., & Marian, V. (2006). Lexical-semantic organization in bilingual children: Evidence from a repeated word association task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49 (3), 572587.
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory, 6 (2), 174.
Swender, E., Conrad, D., & Vicars, R. (2012). ACTFL proficiency guidelines 2012. Alexandria, VA: American Council for the Teaching of Foreign Languages.
Szekely, A., D'Amico, S., Devescovi, A., Federmeier, K., Herron, D., Iyer, G., Jacobsen, T., & Bates, E. (2005). Timed action and object naming. Cortex, 41 (1), 725.
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies. Developmental psychology, 32 (3), 492504.
Van Hell, J. G., & de Groot, A. (1998). Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (03), 193211.
Van Wijnendaele, I., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: phonological priming from the second to the first language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28 (3), 616627.
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Druks, J., Barber, H., & Cappa, S. F. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35 (3), 407426.
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107 (3), 829853.
Wen, Y., & van Heuven, W. J. (2017). Chinese translation norms for 1,429 English words. Behavior research methods, 49 (3), 10061019.
Wolff, P., & Ventura, T. (2009). When Russians learn English: How the semantics of causation may change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12 (02), 153176.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
  • ISSN: 1366-7289
  • EISSN: 1469-1841
  • URL: /core/journals/bilingualism-language-and-cognition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed