Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-fnpn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-27T23:20:21.616Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social bonding and credible signaling hypotheses largely disregard the gap between animal vocalizations and human music

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Marcel Zentner*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Innsbruck, 6020Innsbruck, Austria. marcel.zentner@uibk.ac.at

Abstract

Mehr et al. propose a theory of the evolution music that can potentially account for most animal vocalizations as precursors to human music. Therein lies its appeal but also its Achilles' heel, for the wider the range of animal vocalizations treated as premusical expressions, the wider the gap to human music. Here, I offer a few critical observations and constructive suggestions that I hope will help the authors strengthen their case.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Dalla Bella, S., Farrugia, N., Benoit, C. E., Begel, V., Verga, L., Harding, E., & Kotz, S. A. (2017). BAASTA: Battery for the assessment of auditory sensorimotor and timing abilities. Behavior Research Methods, 49(3), 11281145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feyerabend, P. (1984). Wissenschaft als Kunst. [Science as art]. Suhrkamp Verlag.Google Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. Journal of New Music Research, 33, 217238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Law, L. N., & Zentner, M. (2012). Assessing musical abilities objectively: Construction and validation of the profile of music perception skills. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e52508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettl, B. (2015). The study of ethnomusicology. Thirty-three discussions (pp. 463472). University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Randall, W. M., & Rickard, N. S. (2017). Reasons for personal music listening: A mobile experience sampling study of emotional outcomes. Psychology of Music, 45(4), 479495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Teie, D. (2010). Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music. Biology Letters, 6(1), 3032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentner, M. (in preparation). Music-mindedness: The musical dimension of personality.Google Scholar
Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 8(4), 494521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentner, M., & Strauß, H. (2017). Assessing musical ability quickly and objectively: Development and validation of the Short-PROMS and the Mini-PROMS. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1400(1), 3345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed