Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T09:55:28.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Classical social reward signatures in infants with later ASD

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2019

Teodora Gliga
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom. T.Gliga@uea.ac.ukhttps://people.uea.ac.uk/en/persons/t-gliga Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada, H3A 2B4
Mayada Elsabbagh
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom. mayada.elsabbagh@mcgill.ca

Abstract

Autistic individuals can be socially motivated. We disagree with the idea that self-report is sufficient to understand their social drive. Instead, we underscore evidence for typical non-verbal signatures of social reward during the early development of autistic individuals. Instead of focusing on whether or not social motivation is typical, research should investigate the factors that modulate social drives.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E. & Aldridge, J. W. (2009) Dissecting components of reward: “Liking,” “wanting,” and learning. Current Opinion In Pharmacology 9(1):6573. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014.Google Scholar
Blechert, J., Klackl, J., Miedl, S. F. & Wilhelm, F. H. (2016) To eat or not to eat: Effects of food availability on reward system activity during food picture viewing. Appetite 99:254–61.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Bedford, R., Senju, A., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Johnson, M. H. & BASIS Team (2014) What you see is what you get: Contextual modulation of face scanning in typical and atypical development. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9(4):538–43.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H. & BASIS Team. (2013) The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behavioural Brain Research 251:147–54.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M. & Johnson, M. H. (2016) Autism and the social braIn: The first-year puzzle. Biological Psychiatry 80(2):9499.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Jones, E. J. & Gliga, T. (2015) Brain adaptation and alternative developmental trajectories. Development and psychopathology 27(2):425–42.Google Scholar
Jones, E. J., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T. & Johnson, M. H. (2014) Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 39:133.Google Scholar
Jones, W. & Klin, A. (2013) Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504(7480):427–31.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., Rogers, S. J., Rozga, A., Sangha, S., Sigman, M., Steinfeld, M. B., Young, G. S. (2010) A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 49(3):256–66.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Carver, L. J., Constantino, J.N., Dobkins, K., Hutman, T., Iverson, J.M., Landa, R., Rogers, S.J., Sigman, M. & Stone, W. L. (2011) Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics 128(3):e488e495.Google Scholar
Vernetti, A., Senju, A., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., Gliga, T. & BASIS Team. (2018) Simulating interaction: Using gaze-contingent eye-tracking to measure the reward value of social signals in toddlers with and without autism. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 29:2129.Google Scholar