Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-jwnkl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T02:27:09.439Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Schema Theory: An Information Processing Model of Perception and Cognition*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

Robert Axelrod*
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley

Abstract

The world is complex, and yet people are able to make some sense out of it. This paper offers an information-processing model to describe this aspect of perception and cognition. The model assumes that a person receives information which is less than perfect in terms of its completeness, its accuracy, and its reliability. The model provides a dynamic description of how a person evaluates this kind of information about a case, how he selects one of his pre-existing patterns (called schemata) with which to interpret the case, and how he uses the interpretation to modify and extend his beliefs about the case. It also describes how this process allows the person to make the internal adjustments which will serve as feedback for the interpretation of future information. A wide variety of evidence from experimental and social psychology is cited to support the decisions which went into constructing the separate parts of the schema theory, and further evidence is cited supporting the theory's system-level predictions. Since the schema theory allows for (but does not assume) the optimization of its parameters, it is also used as a framework for a normative analysis of the selection of schemata. Finally, a few illustrations from international relations and especially foreign-policy formation show that this model of how people make sense out of a complex world can be directly relevant to the study of important political processes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 1973

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

For their help I wish to thank Helga Novotny of Cambridge University; A. K. Sen of the London School of Economics; Brian Barry of Oxford University; Barry R. Schlenker of the State University of New York at Albany; Ian Budge, Michael Bloxam, Norman Schofield and Michael Taylor of the University of Essex; Ole Holsti of the University of British Columbia; Ernst Haas and Jeffrey Hart of the University of California at Berkeley; Cedric Smith of the University of London; Zolton Domotor of the University of Pennsylvania; and my research assistants Jacob Bercovitch, Ditsa Kafry, and William Strawn. Parts of this paper have been presented at research seminars at Tel Aviv University, Hebrew University (Jerusalem), Technion (Haifa), the London School of Economics, and the University of Essex. I am grateful to the discussants at all these places. For their generous financial support I wish to thank the Institute of International Studies of the University of California at Berkeley, the Fellowship Program of the Council on Foreign Relations, and the NATO Postdoctoral Fellowship Program of the National Science Foundation. Of course the author is solely responsible for the views presented in this paper.

In reviewing the psychological literature I have relied heavily on Gardner Lindzey and Elliot Aronson, eds., The Handbook of Social Psychology, second edition (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968, 1969), especially the chapters by Berger and Lambert “Stimulus-Response Theory in Contemporary Social Psychology,” I, 81–178; Seymour Rosenberg, “Mathematical Models of Social Behavior,” I, 179–244; Robert B. Zajonc, “Cognitive Theories in Social Psychology,” I, 320–411; Leonard Berkowitz, “Social Motivation,” III, 50–135; Henri Tajfel, “Social and Cultural Factors in Perception,” III, 315–394; and George A. Miller and David McNeill, “Psycholinguistics,” III, 666–794.

References

1 Singer, Jerome E., “Consistency as a Stimulus Process Mechanism,” in Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A Sourcebook, ed. Abelson, Robert P., Aronson, Elliot, McGuire, William, Newcomb, Theodore, Rosenberg, Milton and Tannenbaum, Percy (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968)Google Scholar.

2 For a formal treatment of balance see Harary, Frank, Norman, Robert and Cartwright, Dorwin, Structural Models (New York: Wiley, 1965)Google Scholar. For a treatment of clusters see Davis, James, “Clustering and Structural Balance in Graphs,” Human Relations, 20 (May, 1967), 181187 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Riley, James A., “An Application of Graph Theory to Social Psychology,” in The Many Facets of Graph Theory, Lecture Notes in Mathematics, No. 110 (New York and Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1969)Google Scholar.

3 Axelrod, Robert, Framework for a General Theory of Cognition and Choice (Berkeley: Institute of International Studies, University of California, 1972)Google Scholar. Also to appear in Theories of Collective Behavior, ed. Julius Margolis and Henry Teune, forthcoming.

4 Axelrod, Robert, “Psycho-Algebra: A Mathematical Theory of Cognition and Choice with an Application to the British Eastern Committee in 1918,” Papers of the Peace Research Society (International), 18 (1972), 113131 Google Scholar.

5 For simplicity, the present model makes the unrealistic assumption that each of the separate relationships within a specification is believed with the same degree of confidence.

6 The present model assumes that new information can be assigned to a case and that each case can be assigned to a known case type. Quite possibly these assignments involve a process similar to the interpretation process described within the model.

7 Simon, Herbert, Models of Man (New York: Wiley, 1957)Google Scholar.

8 Asch, S. E., “Forthcoming Impressions of Personality,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41 (July, 1946), 258290 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Anderson, N. H., “Primacy Effects in Personality Impression Formation Using a Generalized Order Effect Paradigm,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2 (July, 1965), 19 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

9 Note that the present model does not treat the effects of limited or inaccurate memory.

10 Dietrich, J. E., “The Relative Effectiveness of Two Modes of Radio Delivery in Influencing Attitudes,” Speech Monographs, 13 (1946), 5865 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Hovland, C. I., Harvey, O. J., and Sherif, M., “Assimilations and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55 (September, 1957), 242252 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and McKillop, Anne S., The Relationship Between the Reader's Attitudes and Some Types of Reading Response (New York: Columbia University Press, 1952)Google Scholar.

11 Anderson, L. R., “Discrediting Sources as a Means of Belief Defense of Cultural Truisms,” American Psychologist, 21 (July, 1966), 708 Google Scholar.

12 A more complex model would take account of other source factors as well as credibility. The new information could then be regarded as having a confidence parameter which was determined by factors including the attractiveness and the power of the source as well as the source's credibility. For a review of source factors in attitude change see McGuire, William J., “The Nature of Attitudes and Attitude Change,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, ed. Lindzey, and Aronson, , III, 136314 Google Scholar.

13 Hovland, C. I. and Mandell, W., “An Experimental Comparison of Conclusion-drawing by the Communicator and by the Audience,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47 (July, 1952), 581588 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; and Hovland, C. I. and Weiss, W., “The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication Effectiveness,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 15 (Winter, 1951), 635650 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

14 Hovland, Carl I., Janis, Irving L., and Kelley, Harold H., Communication and Persuasion (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1953)Google Scholar.

15 Osgood, C. E. and Tannenbaum, P. H., “The Principle of Congruity in the Prediction of Attitude Change,” Psychological Review, 62 (January, 1955), 4255 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

16 Miller, Neal E. and Dollard, John, Social Learning and Imitation (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941)Google ScholarPubMed.

17 Bjorkman, Mats, “Predictive Behavior: Some Aspects Based on Ecological Orientation,” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 7 (1966), 4357 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

18 Anderson, “Primacy Effects”; Zajonc, “Cognitive Theories”; and Rosenberg, “Mathematical Models.”

19 DeSoto, Clinton B. and Kuethe, James L., “Subjective Probabilities of Interpersonal Relationships,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59 (September 1959), 290294 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

20 Attneave, Fred, “Multistability in Perception,” Scientific American (December, 1971), pp. 6371 Google Scholar.

21 This effect was found by McGuire, William J., “Cognitive Consistency and Attitude Change,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60 (May, 1960), 345353 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

22 Only weak indications of such an effect were found by Dillehay, R. C., Insko, C. A., and Smith, M. B., “Logical Consistency and Attitude Change,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 (June, 1966), 646654 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

23 Bartlett, Frederic, A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1969)Google Scholar.

24 Simon, Models of Man.

25 March, James G. and Simon, Herbert A., Organizations (New York: Wiley, 1958)Google Scholar.

26 Cyert, Richard M. and March, James G., A Behavioral Theory of the Firm (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1963)Google Scholar.

27 Kuethe, James, “Pervasive Influence of Social Schemata,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68 (May, 1964), 248254 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

28 Kuethe, James and Weingartner, Herbert, “Male-Female Schemata of Homosexual and Non-Homosexual Penitentiary Inmates,” Journal of Personality, 32 (March, 1964), 2331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

29 DeSoto, Clinton B., “Learning a Social Structure,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60 (May, 1960), 417421 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

30 Sherif, Muzafer and Hovland, Carl I., Social Judgment: Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1961)Google Scholar; Sherif, Carolyn W., Sherif, Muzafer, and Nebergall, R. E., Attitude and Attitude Change (Philadelphia: Saunders, 1965)Google Scholar; and Sherif, Carolyn W. and Muzafer, Sherif, eds., Attitude, Ego-Involvement and Change (New York: Wiley, 1967)Google Scholar.

31 Sherif and Hovland, Social Judgment.

32 Berkowitz, Leonard, “Judgmental Processes in Personality Functioning,” Psychological Review, 67 (March, 1960), 130142 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

33 Berkowitz, Leonard and Goranson, R. E., “Motivational and Judgmental Determinants of Social Perception,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69 (September, 1964), 296302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

34 Tajfel, Henri and Wilkes, A. L., “Classification and Quantitative Judgement,” British Journal of Psychology, 54 (May, 1963), 101114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

35 Campbell, D. T., “Enhancement of Contrast as Composite Habit,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53 (November, 1956), 350355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

36 Ervin, Susan, “Imitation and Structural Change in Children's Language,” in New Directions in the Study of Language, ed. Lenneberg, Eric H. (Cambridge: M.I.T. Press, 1964), pp. 163190 Google Scholar.

37 Woodworth, R. S., Experimental Psychology (New York: Holt, 1938)Google ScholarPubMed; Miller, George A., “Some Psychological Studies of Grammar,” American Psychologist, 17 (November, 1962), 748762 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

38 Berelson, Bernard R. and Steiner, Gary A., Human Behavior (Burlingame: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1964)Google Scholar.

39 Lenneberg, Eric H. and Roberts, John M., The Language of Experience (Baltimore: Waverly Press, 1956)Google Scholar. Also appeared as International Journal of American Linguistics, 22, No. 2; and in Saporta, Sol, ed., Psycholinguistics (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1961)Google Scholar.

40 Brown, Roger and Lenneberg, Eric H., “A Study in Language and Cognition,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49 (July, 1954), 454462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

41 Glanzer, Murray and Clark, William H., “Accuracy of Perceptual Recall: An Analysis of Organization,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1 (January, 1963), 289299 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

42 Kogan, Nathan and Tagiuri, Renato, “Interpersonal Preference and Cognitive Organization,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56 (January, 1958), 113116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

43 DeSoto, Clinton E. and Kuethe, James L., “Perception of Mathematical Properties of Interpersonal Relations,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8 (December, 1958), 279286 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and DeSoto and Kuethe, “Subjective Probabilities.” For a quantitative extension, see Wellens, A. Rodney and Thistlethwaite, Donald L., “Comparison of Three Theories of Cognitive Balance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20 (October, 1971), 8292 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

44 Morrissette, J., “An Experimental Study of the Theory of Structural Balance,” Human Relations, 11 (August; 1958), 239254 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Schrader, Elizabeth G. and Lewit, D. W., “Structural Factors in Cognitive Balancing Behavior,” Human Relations, 15 (August, 1962), 265276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

45 Swets, J. A., “Is There a Sensory Threshold?Science, 134 (July 21, 1961), 168177 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Green, D. M., “Psychoacoustics and Detection Theory,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29 (1960), 11801203 Google Scholar. For a review see Coombs, Clyde H., Dawes, Robyn M., and Tversky, Amos, Mathematical Psychology (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1970)Google Scholar.

46 Gerard, Harold B. and Fleischer, Linda, “Recall and Pleasantness of Balanced and Unbalanced Structures,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 (November, 1967), 332337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

47 Kogan and Tagiuri, “Interpersonal Preference.”

48 Zajonc, R. B. and Burnstein, E., “Structural Balance, Reciprocity, and Positivity as Sources of Cognitive Bias,” Journal of Personality, 33 (December, 1965), 570583 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed. For related results on the learning of liking structures, see Zajonc, R. B. and Burnstein, E., “The Learning of Balanced and Unbalanced Social Structures,” Journal of Personality, 33 (June, 1965), 153163 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

49 DeSoto, “Learning a Social Structure.” For more recent verification that learning a set of relationships is easier when they fit the expected structure, see DeSoto, Clinton B., Henley, Nancy M., and London, Marvin, “Balance and the Grouping Schema,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8 (July, 1968), 17 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Henley, Nancy M., Horsfall, Robert B. and DeSoto, Clinton B., “Goodness of Figure and Social Structure,” Psychological Review, 76 (1969), 194204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Horsfall, Robert B. and Henley, Nancy M., “Mixed Social Structures: Strain and Probability Ratings,” Psychonomic Science, 15 (May, 1969), 186187 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Mosher, Donald L., “The Learning of Congruent and Noncongruent Social Structures,” Journal of Social Psychology, 73 (December, 1967), 285290 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

50 Berelson, and Steiner, , Human Behavior, p. 204 Google Scholar.

51 Berelson, and Steiner, , Human Behavior, p. 204 Google Scholar.

52 Rosenberg, Milton J. and Abelson, Robert P., “An Analysis of Cognitive Balancing,” in Attitude Organization and Change, Rosenberg, Milton J., Hovland, Carl I., McGuire, William J., Abelson, Robert P. and Brehm, Jack W. (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1960), pp. 112163 Google Scholar.

53 Berlyne, D. E., Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960)CrossRefGoogle Scholar, and Sokolov, Evgenii N., Perception and the Conditioned Reflex, trans. Waydenfeld, Stephen W. (Oxford and New York: Pergamon, 1963)Google Scholar.

54 McGuire, , “The Nature of Attitudes and Attitude Change,” p. 169 Google Scholar.

55 Sherif, “A Study of Some Social Factors in Perception.”

56 See McGuire, , “The Nature of Attitudes and Attitude Change,” p. 223 Google Scholar, for a review.

57 Solomon, R. L., Kamin, L. J., and Wynne, L. C., “Traumatic Avoidance Learning: The Outcomes of Several Extinction Procedures with Dogs,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48 (April, 1953), 291302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

58 Miller and Dollard, Social Learning and Imitation.

59 McGuire, William J., “The Current Status of Cognitive Consistency Theories,” in Cognitive Consistency: Motivational Antecedents and Behavioral Consequents, ed. Feldman, Shel (New York: Academic Press, 1966)Google Scholar; Abelson, Robert P., “Psychological Implication,” in Abelson, et al., Theories of Cognitive Consistency, pp. 112139 Google Scholar; Abelson, et al., Theories of Cognitive Consistency, chapters 6471 Google Scholar; and Tannenbaum, Percy H., Macaulay, Jacqueline R., and Norris, Eleanor L., “Principle of Congruity and Reduction of Persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 (February, 1966), 233238 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

60 But for some interesting simulations of mental processes see Tomkins, Silvan and Messick, Samuel (eds.), Computer Simulation of Personality (New York: Wiley, 1963)Google Scholar, Abelson, Robert P. and Carroll, J. Douglas, “Computer Simulation of Individual Belief Systems,” American Behavioral Scientist, 8 (May, 1965), 2430 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Loehlin, John C., Computer Models of Personality (New York: Random House, 1968)Google Scholar; Moser, U., von Zeppelen, I. S. and Schneider, W., “Computer Simulation of a Model of Neurotic Defence Processes,” Behavioral Science, 15 (March 1970), 194202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Newell, Allen and Simon, Herbert, “Simulation of Human Thought,” in Computer Simulation of Human Behavior, ed. Dutton, John M. and Starbuck, William (New York: Wiley, 1971)Google Scholar.

61 Wertheimer, Max, Productive Thinking (New York: Harper, 1945)Google Scholar.

62 Garner, Wendell R., Uncertainty and Structure as Psychological Concepts (New York: Wiley, 1962)Google Scholar.

63 Watanabe, Satosi, Knowing and Guessing: A Quantitative Study of Inference and Information (New York: Wiley, 1969)Google Scholar.

64 Abelson, Robert P. and Rosenberg, Milton J., “Symbolic Psychologic: A Model of Attitudinal Cognition,” Behavioral Science, 3 (January, 1958), 113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

65 For a basic introduction to balance theory, see Brown, Roger, Social Psychology (Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1956)Google Scholar; for a review of most of the experimental evidence see Zajonc, “Cognitive Theories”; for a fund of source material, see Abelson et al., Theories of Cognitive Consistency.

66 In fact, much of the experimental work on schemata cited in this paper has been inspired more or less directly by the study of interpersonal relationships contained in Heider, Fritz, The Psychology of Inter-personal Relations (New York: Wiley, 1958)Google Scholar.

67 Rosenberg and Abelson, “An Analysis of Cognitive Balancing.”

68 Morrissette, “An Experimental Study.”

69 Kogan and Tagiuri, “Interpersonal Preference.”

70 De Soto, Clinton B., Henley, Nancy M. and London, Marvin, “Balance and the Grouping Schema,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8 (January, 1968), 17 CrossRefGoogle Scholar. Cottrell, Nicolas B., Ingraham, Larry H. and Monfort, Franklin W., “The Retention of Balanced and Unbalanced Cognitive Structures,” Journal of Personality, 39 (March, 1971), 112131 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

71 Burnstein, Eugene, “Sources of Cognitive bias in the Representation of Simple Social Structures; Balance, Minimal Change, Positivity, Reciprocity, and the Respondent's Own Attitude,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 (September, 1967), 3648 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed. Miller, Harold and Geller, Dennis, “Structural Balance in Dyads,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21 (February, 1972), 135138 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

72 Johnson, Nicholas B., “Some Models of Balance Applied to Children's Perceptions of International Relations,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 2 (1972), 5564 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

73 Blanchard, Edward B., Vickers, Marilyn and Price, Katina C., “Balance Effects in Image Formation,” The Journal of Social Psychology, 87 (June, 1972), 3744 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

74 Zajonc and Burnstein, “The Learning of Balanced and Unbalanced Social Structures” and “Structural Balance.”

75 Zajonc and Burnstein, “Structural Balance.”

76 Wiest, William M., “A Quantitative Extension of Heider's Theory of Cognitive Balance Applied to Interpersonal Perception and Self-Esteem,” Psychological Monographs, 79, No. 1 (Whole No. 607) (December, 1965)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

77 Rodriguez, Aroldo, “Effects of Balance, Positivity and Agreement in Triadic Social Relations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5 (April, 1967), 472476 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

78 Zajonc, R. B., “The Concepts of Balance, Congruity, and Dissonance,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 24 (Summer, 1960), 280296 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

79 Zajonc, Robert B. and Sherman, Steven J., “Structural Balance and the Induction of Relations,” Journal of Personality, 35 (December, 1967), 635650 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

80 Zajonc and Burnstein, “Tlie Learning of Balanced and Unbalanced Social Structures.” The authors explain this result by saying that one issue was less important than the other, but an alternative explanation is that the balance schema is not accessible for a case type involving readership of a general purpose magazine.

81 Axelrod, “Psycho-Algebra.”

82 Feather, N. T., “Organization and Discrepancy in Cognitive Structures,” Psychological Review, 78 (September, 1971), 355379 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

83 William J. McGuire, “Cognitive Consistency and Attitude Change”; McGuire, William J., “A Syllogistic Analysis of Cognitive Relationships,” in Attitude Organization and Change, Rosenberg, Milton J. et al. (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1960), pp. 65111 Google Scholar; Dillehay, R. C., Insko, C. A., and Smith, M. B., “Logical Consistency and Attitude Change,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 (June, 1966), 646654 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

84 Rosenberg, Milton J. and Gardner, C. W., “Case Report: Some Dynamic Aspects of Posthypnotic Compliance,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57 (November, 1958), 351366 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

85 Scott, W. A., “Cognitive Consistency, Response Reinforcement, and Attitude Change,” Sociometry, 22 (September, 1959), 219229 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

86 Henle, Mary, “On Error in Deductive Reasoning,” Psychological Reports, 7 (August, 1960), 80 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

87 Whorf, Benjamin L., Language, Thought and Reality, ed. Carroll, J. B. (Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1956 Google Scholar).

88 For a review of linguistic development in children see Brown, Roger, Social Psychology (Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, 1956)Google ScholarPubMed. For a review of Piaget's work on cognitive development see Flavell, John H., The Developmental Psychology of Jean Piaget (Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1963)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

89 A recent formulation of the claim for linguistic universals which uses this example is Chomsky, Noam, Problems of Knowledge and Freedom (New York: Pantheon Books, 1971)Google Scholar. For a review of psycholinguistic research see Miller and McNeill, “Psycholinguistics.”

90 Attneave, “Multistability in Perception.”

91 Concept formation experiments provide further indirect evidence. See, for example, Bruner, Jerome S., Goodnow, Jacqueline J. and Austin, George A., A Study of Thinking (New York: Wiley, 1956)Google Scholar.

92 For example, DeSoto and Kuethe, “Perception of Mathematical Properties”; DeSoto and Kuethe, “Subective Probabilities”; and DeSoto, “Learning a Social Structure.”

93 For example, DeSoto and Kuethe, “Perception of Mathematical Properties”; DeSoto and Kuethe, “Subective Probabilities;” and DeSoto, “Learning a Social Structure.”

94 Henley, Horsfall and DeSoto, “Goodness of Figure,” Axelrod, “Psycho-Algebra.”

95 Henley, Horsfall and DeSoto, “Goodness of Figure”; DeSoto, Clinton and Albrecht, Frank, “Conceptual Good Figures,” in Theories of Cognitive Consistency, Abelson, et al., pp. 504511 Google Scholar; and DeSoto, Clinton and Albrecht, Frank, “Cognition and Social Orderings,” in Theories of Cognitive Consistency, Abelson, et al., pp. 531538 Google Scholar; Poitou, Jean Pierre and Van Kreveld, David, “Is There a Position Bias in Learning of Influence Strucures?European Journal of Social Psychology, 2, No. 1 (1972), 7585 CrossRefGoogle Scholar. For a related schema called proximity, see Peay, Edmund R., “Extensions of Clusterability to Quantitative Data with an Application to the Cognition of Political Attitudes” (Ph.D. diss., University of Michigan, 1970)Google Scholar.

96 DeSoto, Clinton B. and Bosley, John J., “The Cognitive Structure of a Social Structure,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64 (April, 1962), 303307 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; DeSoto and Albrecht. “Conceptual Good Figure,” and DeSoto and Albrecht, “Cognition and Social Orderngs.”

97 Alexander, C. Norman Jr., and Epstein, Joyce, “Problems of Dispositional Inference in Person Perception Research,” Sociometry, 32 (December, 1969), 381395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

98 Abelson, Robert P. and Kanouse, David E., “Subjective Acceptance of Verbal Generalization,” in Cognitive Consistency, ed. Feldman, Shel (New York: Academic Press, 1966), pp. 171197 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

99 Bieri, James and Blacker, Edward, “The Generality of Cognitive Complexity in the Perception of People and Inkblots,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53 (July, 1956), 112117 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

100 Brown, Social Psychology.

101 Frenkel-Brunswik, Else, “Intolerance of Ambiguity as an Emotional and Perceptual Personality Variable,” Journal of Personality, 18 (September, 1949), 103143 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

102 Newcomb, Theodore M., “Stabilities Underlying Changes in Interpersonal Attraction,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66 (April, 1963), 376386, esp. p. 385CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.

103 Steiner, Ivan D. and Johnson, Homer H., “Authoritarianism and ‘Tolerance of Trait Inconsistency’,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67 (October 1963), 388391 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

104 Festinger, Leon, A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957)Google Scholar; Brehm, Jack W. and Cohen, A. R., Explorations in Cognitive Dissonance (New York: Wiley, 1962)CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Zajonc, “Cognitive Theories in Social Psychology.”

105 Cantril, Hadley, “The Prediction of Social Events,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33 (1938), 364389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; McGreor, D., “The Major Determinants of the Prediction of Social Events,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33 (1938), 179204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and McGuire, “Cognitive Consistency and Attitude Change.”

106 For a formal model with a similar tradeoff, see Amari, Shun-ichi, “Theory of Learning Decision Systems,” in Unifying Study of Basic Problems in Engineering and Physical Sciences by Means of Geometry, ed. Kondo, Kazoo (Division 1), (Tokyo: Gakujutsu Bunken Fukyu-kai, 1969), pp. 3342 (which is RAAG Memoirs, 4, [1969], 567–576)Google Scholar.

107 With the assumption that each instance of a schema has equal subjective probability, size determines redundancy. The assumption is based on the principle that if some instances have a higher subjective probability than others, they should be treated by the model as belonging to different schemata. For applications of redundancy to psychology see Garner, Uncertainty and Structure; and Garner, Wendell R. and Clement, David E., “Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2 (December, 1963), 446452 CrossRefGoogle Scholar. For a recent critical review see Corcoran, D. W. J., Pattern Recognition (Harmondsworth, England: Penguin Books, 1971)Google Scholar.

108 For schemata of infinite size (e.g., with continuous rather than discrete parameters), the appropriate measure of size uses degrees of freedom. For example, a linear equation has two degrees of freedom (namely a and b in y = ax + b), and a quadratic equation has three degrees of freedom (namely a, b and c in y = ax3 + bx + c).

109 Kogan and Tagiuri, “Interpersonal Preference and Cognitive Organization.”

110 To carry the analysis further one could do the type of indifference curve analysis used in microeconomics. Negatively sloped indifference curves could be drawn in Figure 4 to represent the tradeoffs between smallness and veridicality. Then the schemata should be given accessibility in order of their intersection with the ordered set of indifference curves. If there are enough alternative schemata to allow for a continuous approximation of the frontier of knowledge, then the schema which should be given the highest accessibility is represented by the point at which one of the indifference curves is tangent to the frontier of knowledge.

111 A metric function has the following properties: d(x,y) = d(y,x); d(x,y) ≥ 0; d(x,y) = 0 if and only if x = y, and d(x,y) + d(y,z) ≥ d(x,z).

112 Although the distance between points is a metric function as defined in the previous footnote, the distance between sets is not.

113 Abelson and Rosenberg, “Symbolic Psycho-logic.”

114 Wiest, “A Quantitative Extension.”

115 Signal detectability theory treats the situation in which there is only one accessible schema. See Coombs, Dawes, and Tversky, Mathematical Psychology.

116 Geometric analysis of this section can be extended to yield a number of suggestive theorems about some of the more subtle consequences of the present information processing model. Here are four such results.

(1) Potential Completeness. If Q is a continuous space, then under a weak set of assumptions the modified and extended specification of a case will be a complete specification of the case. This corresponds to the situation in which the person can make an estimate about every feature of the case (e.g., every friendship relationship). For example, in Figure 5 the set of points in S2 which are minimally distant from X is a single point, x′, and is therefore a complete specification.

(2) Potential Enlargement. Under certain circumstances the modified and extended specification can actually be larger than the initial partial specification. This occurs when there are few points in the partial specification, but more points in the selected schema which are at the minimum distance from the initial partial specification than there were in the initial partial specification itself. In such a situation the person has a schema which he can use to interpret the case, but he does not end up with a single unique specification of the case (even if he started with one). He believes something is wrong with the initial information, but he can not pin down what it is, even though he has selected a schema with which to interpret the case.

(3) Cancellation Effect. Under a wide range of conditions, an initial partial specification with continuous parameters will be modified to achieve a fit to the selected schema by leaving most of the parameters unchanged and by giving the rest neutral values. This can be called the cancellation effect since the only changes in the modification are neutralizations. The cancellation effect is a consequence of the principle that the modification is made in such a way as to move the least distance to the selected schema.

(4) Seriatim Effect. Under a wide range of circumstances, the sequential presentation of the same information can reduce the amount of attitude change compared to a simultaneous presentation. The seriatim effect is due to the principle that a message is first evaluated for how well it fits into a previous interpretation of the case. Presenting discrepant information a little at a time can result in each message being blamed as it is received, whereas if the discrepant information were presented all at once, the blame might be affixed to the old interpretation.

117 Allison, Graham T., “Conceptual Models and the Cuban Missile Crisis,” American Political Science Review, 63 (September, 1969), 689718 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

118 Holsti, Ole R., “The Belief System and National Images: A Case Study,” Journal of Conflict Resolution, 6 (September, 1962), 244252 CrossRefGoogle Scholar.

119 Axelrod, Framework for a General Theory of Cognition and Choice.

120 Masterman, J. C., The Double-Cross System in the War of 1939 to 1945 (New Haven and London: Yale University Press, 1972)Google Scholar.

121 Masterman, The Double-Cross System.

122 Ben-David, Joseph, The Scientist's Role in Society (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1971), pp. 25f.Google Scholar

123 Solomon, Kamm and Wynne, “Traumatic Avoidance Learning.”

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.