Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-l82ql Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-26T04:03:48.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Genetic influence on growth: family and race comparisons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2010

Phyllis B. Eveleth
Affiliation:
National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Maryland
James M. Tanner
Affiliation:
University of London
Get access

Summary

The ultimate size and shape that a child attains as an adult is the result of a continous interaction between genetical and environmental influences during the whole period of growth. Such interaction may be complex. Two genotypes which produce the same adult height under optimal environmental circumstances may produce different heights under circumstances of privation. Thus two children who would be the same height in a well-off community may not only both be smaller under poor economic conditions, but one may be significantly smaller than the other. This type of interaction, called non-additivity of genotype and environment, may be quite detailed and specific in its effects. If a particular environmental stimulus is lacking at a time when it is essential for the child (times known as ‘sensitive periods’), then the child's development may be shunted, as it were, from one line to another. We know, as yet, little of the details of such interactions, but quite enough to make oversimplified models scientifically suspect.

Statements about the relative contributions of heredity and environment to adult size and shape must therefore always specify the circumstances with some exactness. A biologically permissible statement (provided all the evidence is really available), is, for example: ‘Eighty-five per cent of the variance of height in the population of young adults growing up in middle class homes in a London suburb in the 1980s is due to genetical factors’. In a population growing up under famine conditions the percentage of variance due to genetical factors would be expected to be less. A particularly lucid discussion of these points may be found in Thoday (1965).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×