Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
The Study of Behavior
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 2
  • Cited by
    This book has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Bowers, Robert Ian 2018. A common heritage of behaviour systems. Behaviour, Vol. 155, Issue. 5, p. 415.

    Varella, Marco Antonio Correa 2018. The Biology and Evolution of the Three Psychological Tendencies to Anthropomorphize Biology and Evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 9, Issue. ,

    ×

Book description

Behavior studies now span a variety of sub-disciplines, including behavioral ecology, neuroscience, cognitive psychology and evolutionary developmental biology. While the fields' rapid growth has led to startling new insights into animal behavior, it has brought increasingly fragmented approaches to the subject. Integrating ideas and findings from a range of disciplines, this book provides a common framework for understanding diverse issues in behavior studies. The framework is derived from classical ethology, incorporating concepts and data from research in experimental psychology, neurophysiology and evolutionary biology. Hogan outlines the origin and development of major ideas and issues in the field, drawing on examples throughout to highlight connections across sub-disciplines. Demonstrating how results in one area can directly inform work in others, the book ultimately proposes concepts to facilitate new discussions that will open the way for improved dialog between researchers across behavior studies.

Refine List
Actions for selected content:
Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Send to Kindle
  • Send to Dropbox
  • Send to Google Drive
  • Send content to

    To send content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about sending content to .

    To send content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

    Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

    Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

    Please be advised that item(s) you selected are not available.
    You are about to send
    ×

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×
Adams, C. D. & Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental Responding Following Reinforcer Devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 109122.
Adler, N. T & Hogan, J. A. (1963). Classical Conditioning and Punishment of an Instinctive Response in Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 11, 351354.
Adrian, E. D., Cattell, M. & Hoagland, H. (1931). Sensory Discharges in Single Cutaneous Nerve Fibres. Journal of Physiology, 72, 377391.
Agnvall, B., Katajamaa, R., Altimiras, J. & Jensen, P. (2015). Biology Letters, 11, 20150509.
Akers, K. G. et al. (2014). Hippocampal Neurogenesis Regulates Forgetting during Adulthood and Infancy. Science, 344, 598602.
Alcock, J. S. (1989). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvika, E. & Chittka, L. (2016). Associative Mechanisms Allow for Social Learning and Cultural Transmission of String Pulling in an Insect. PLoS Biology, 14(10).
Allport, G. (1937). Personality: A Psychological Interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Andrew, R. J. (1966). Precocious Adult Behaviour in the Young Chick. Animal Behaviour, 14, 485500.
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism – How to Carry out an Exaptationist Program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 489553.
Armitage, K. B. (1986). Individuality, Social Behavior, and Reproductive Success in Yellow-Bellied Marmots. Ecology, 67, 11861193.
Armstrong, E. A. (1954). Bird Display and Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S. & Rosenblatt, J. S. (Eds.). (1970). Development and Evolution of Behavior. San Francisco: Freeman.
Aschoff, J. (1960). Exogenous and Endogenous Components in Circadian Rhythms. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 25, 1128.
Aschoff, J. (1989). Temporal Organization: Circadian Clocks in Animals and Humans. Animal Behaviour, 37, 881896.
Ashton, M. C. (2013). Individual Differences and Personality, 2nd ed. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ashton, M. C. (2015). Hogan’s Framework for the Study of Behavior as Applied to Personality Psychology. Behavioural Processes, 117, 4851.
Aslin, R. N. (2014). Infant Learning: Historical, Conceptual, and Methodological Challenges. Infancy, 19, 227.
Avarguès-Weber, A., Dyer, A. G., Combe, M. & Giurfa, M. (2012). Simultaneous Mastering of Two Abstract Concepts by the Miniature Brain of Bees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 74817486.
Baerends, G. P. (1941). Fortpflanzungsverhalten und Orientierung der Grabwespe Ammophila campestris. Tijdschrift voor Entomologie, 84, 81275.
Baerends, G. P. (1958). Comparative Methods and the Concept of Homology in the Study of Behaviour. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 13, Suppl 1, pp. 401417.
Baerends, G. P. (1970). A Model of the Functional Organization of Incubation Behaviour. Behaviour, Suppl. 17, pp. 263312.
Baerends, G. P. (1975). An Evaluation of the Conflict Hypothesis as an Explanatory Principle for the Evolution of Displays. In Baerends et al., pp. 187227.
Baerends, G. P. (1976). The Functional Organization of Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 24, 726738.
Baerends, G. P. (1982). The Relative Effectiveness of Different Egg-features in Responses Other Than Egg-retrieval. Behaviour, 82, 225246.
Baerends, G. P. & Kruijt, J. P. (1973). Stimulus Selection. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 2349.
Baerends, G. P. & Drent, R. H. (Eds.). (1982). The Herring Gull and Its Eggs. Behaviour, 82, 1416.
Baerends, G. P., Beer, C. & Manning, A. (Eds.). (1975). Function and Evolution in Behaviour. London: Oxford University Press.
Baerends, G. P., Brouwer, R. & Waterbolk, H. T. (1955). Ethological Studies on Lebistes reticulatus: 1. An Analysis of the Male Courtship Pattern. Behaviour, 8, 249334.
Baerends-van Roon, J. M. & Baerends, G. P. (1979). The Morphogenesis of the Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, with a Special Emphasis on the Development of Prey-catching. Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Afd. Natuurkunde, Tweede Reeks (Proceedings of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences, Section Physics, Second Series), Part 72.
Baker, J. R. (1938). The Evolution of Breeding Seasons. In de Beer, G. R. (Ed.), Evolution: Essays on Aspects of Evolutionary Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 161171.
Balleine, B. W. & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-directed and Habitual Action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 4869.
Balsam, P. D & Silver, R. (1994). Behavioral Change as a Result of Experience: Toward Principles of Learning and Development. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 327357.
Balsam, P. D., Graf, J. S. & Silver, R. (1992). Operant and Pavlovian Contributions to the Ontogeny of Pecking in Ring Doves. Developmental Psychobiology, 25, 389410.
Baptista, L. F. & Gaunt, S. L. L. (1997). Social Interaction and Vocal Development in Birds. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 2340.
Barbour, M. A. & Clark, R. W. (2012). Ground Squirrel Tail-flag Displays Alter Both Predatory Strike and Ambush Site Selection Behaviours of Rattlesnakes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 38273833.
Barlow, G. W. (1968). Ethological Units of Behavior. In Ingle, D. (Ed.), Central Nervous System and Fish Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 217232.
Barnard, C. J. (2004). Animal Behaviour: Mechanism, Development, Function and Evolution. London: Pearson Education Ltd./Prentice-Hall.
Barnard, C. J. & Sibly, R. M. (1981). Producers and Scroungers: A General Model and Its Application to Captive Flocks of House Sparrows. Animal Behaviour, 29, 543550.
Barrett, H. C. & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in Cognition: Framing the Debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628647.
Barrett, L. F. (2013). Psychological Construction: The Darwinian Approach to the Science of Emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 379389.
Barrett, L. F. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.). (2015). The Psychological Construction of Emotion. New York: Guilford Press.
Bateson, P. P. G. (1979). How Do Sensitive Periods Arise and What Are They For? Animal Behaviour, 27, 470486.
Bateson, P. P. G. (1987). Imprinting as a Process of Competitive Exclusion. In Rauschecker, J. P. & Marler, P. (Eds.), Imprinting and Cortical Plasticity. New York: Wiley, pp. 151168.
Bateson, P. P. G. (1999). Foreword. In Bolhuis, & Hogan, , pp. ixxi.
Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (1987). Developmental Changes in Sensitivity to Experience. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Sensitive Periods in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1934. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)
Bateson, P. P. G. & Laland, K. N. (2013). Tinbergen’s Four Questions: An Appreciation and an Update. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 28, 712718.
Battley, P. F. et al. (2012). Contrasting Extreme Long-distance Migration Patterns in Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa Lapponica. Journal of Avian Biology, 43, 112.
Baylis, J. R. (1982). Avian Vocal Mimicry: Its Function and Evolution. In Kroodsma, D. E., Miller, E. H. & Ouellet, H. (Eds.), Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, pp. 5183.
Beach, F. A. (1942). Analysis of Factors Involved in the Arousal, Maintenance, and Manifestation of Sexual Excitement in Male Animals. Psychosomatic Medicine, 4, 173198.
Beach, F. A. (1948). Hormones and Behavior. New York: Hoeber.
Beach, F. A. (1955). The Descent of Instinct. Psychological Review, 62, 401410.
Beach, F. A. (1956). Characteristics of the Masculine “Sex Drive”. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), The Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 132.
Beer, C. G. (1975). Multiple Functions and Gull Displays. In Baerends, et al., pp. 1654.
Bergerud, A. T. & Mossop, D. H. (1984). The Pair Bond in Ptarmigan. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 62, 21292141.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Berridge, K. C. (1990). Comparative Fine Structure of Action: Rules of Form and Sequence in the Grooming Patterns of Six Rodent Species. Behaviour, 113, 2156.
Berridge, K. C. (1994). The Development of Action Patterns. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 147180.
Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation Concepts in Behavioral Neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior, 81, 179209.
Berridge, K. C. (2012). From Prediction Error to Incentive Salience: Mesolimbic Computation of Reward Motivation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, pp. 11241143.
Berridge, K. C., Fentress, J. C. & Parr, H. (1987). Natural Syntax Rules Control Action Sequence of Rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 5968.
Berridge, K. C. & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing Reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 507513.
Berry, C. S. (1908). An Experimental Study of Imitation in Cats. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Physiology, 18, 112.
Berthold, P. (1973). Relationships between Migratory Restlessness and Migration Distance in Six Sylvia Species. Ibis, 115, 594599.
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, Brain, and the Nature of Language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 8998.
Bijleveld, A. I., Folmer, E. O. & Piersma, T. (2012). Experimental Evidence for Cryptic Interference among Socially Foraging Shorebirds. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 806814.
Bijleveld, A. I., van Gils, J. A., Jouta, J. & Piersma, T. (2015). Benefits of Foraging in Small Groups: An Experimental Study on Public Information Use in Red Knots Calidris canutus. Behavioural Processes, 117, 7481.
Bischof, H.-J. (1994). Sexual Imprinting as a Two-stage Process. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 8297.
Bissonette, G. B., Bryden, D. W. & Roesch, M. R. (2014). You Won’t Regret Reading This. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 892893.
Blaisdell, A., Sawa, K., Leising, K. J. & Waldmann, M. R. (2006). Causal Reasoning in Rats. Science, 311, 10201022.
Blakemore, C. (1973). Environmental Constraints on Development in the Visual System. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 5173.
Blankenship, A. G. & Feller, M. B. (2010). Mechanisms Underlying Spontaneous Patterned Activity in Developing Neural Circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 1829.
Blass, E. M. (1999). The Ontogeny of Human Infant Face Recognition: Orogustatory, Visual, and Social Influences. In Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early Social Cognition. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 3565.
Blass, E. M. (2015). Energy Conservation in Infants. Behavioural Processes, 117, 3541.
Blass, E. M., Ganchrow, J. R. & Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical Conditioning in Newborn Humans 2–48 Hours of Age. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 125134.
Blass, E. M., Hall, W. G. & Teicher, M. H. (1979). The Ontogeny of Suckling and Ingestive Behaviors. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 8, 243299.
Blest, A. D. (1957). The Function of Eyespot Patterns in the Lepidoptera. Behaviour, 11, 209256.
Bloch, G., Barnes, B. M., Gerkema, M. P. & Helm, B. (2013). Animal Activity around the Clock with No Overt Circadian Rhythms: Patterns, Mechanisms and Adaptive Value. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 30130019.
Blüm, V. & Fiedler, K. (1965). Hormonal Control of Reproductive Behavior in Some Cichlid Fish. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 5, 186196.
Blumstein, D. T. (2007). The Evolution, Function, and Meaning of Marmot Alarm Communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 371401.
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviourism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bock, W. J. & von Wahlert, G. (1965). Adaptation and the Form-Function Complex. Evolution, 19, 269299.
Bohacek, J. & Mansuy, I. M. (2015). Molecular Insights into Transgenerational Non-genetic Inheritance of Acquired Behaviours. Nature Reviews Genetics, 16, 641652.
Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of Avian Imprinting: A Review. Biological Reviews, 66, 303345.
Bolhuis, J. J. (1994). Neurobiological Analyses of Behavioural Mechanisms in Development. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 1646.
Bolhuis, J. J. (1996). Development of Perceptual Mechanisms in Birds: Predispositions and Imprinting. In Moss, C. F. & Shettleworth, S. J. (Eds.), Neuroethological Studies of Cognitive and Perceptual Processes. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, pp. 158184. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)
Bolhuis, J. J. (2009). Function and Mechanism in Neuroecology: Looking for Clues. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 163196.
Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Evolution Cannot Explain How Minds Work. Behavioural Processes, 117, 8291.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Eda-Fujiwara, H. (2003). Bird Brains and Songs: Neural Mechanisms of Birdsong Perception and Memory. Animal Biology, 53, 129145.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Everaert, M. (Eds.). (2013). Birdsong, Speech, and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2005). The Behavior of Animals. New York: Wiley.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (in press). The Behavior of Animals, 2nd edn. New York: Wiley.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Hogan, J. A. (1999). The Development of Animal Behaviour: A Reader. Oxford: Blackwell.
Bolhuis, J. J. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2009). Can Evolution Explain How Minds Work? Nature, 458, 832833.
Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N. & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How Could Language Have Evolved? PLoS Biology, 12(8).
Bolhuis, J. J., Zijlstra, G. G. O., den Boer-Visser, A. M. & van der Zee, E. A. (2000). Localised Neuronal Activation in the Zebra Finch Brain Is Related to the Strength of Song Learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 22822285.
Bolhuis, J.J., Brown, G.R., Richardson, R.C. & Laland, K.N. (2011). Darwin in Mind: New Opportunities for Evolutionary Psychology. PLoS Biology, 9(7).
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of Motivation. New York: Harper & Row.
Bols, R. J. (1977). Display Reinforcement in the Siamese Fighting Fish, Betta splendens: Aggression Motivation or Curiosity? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 233244.
Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animals. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Borbély, A. A. (1982). A Two-process Model of Sleep. Human Neurobiology, 1, 195204.
Borbély, A. A., Dijk, D.-J., Achermann, P. & Tobler, I. (2001). Processes Underlying the Regulation of the Sleep-wake Cycle. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 458479.
Borchelt, P. L. (1975). The Organization of Dustbathing Components in Bobwhite Quail (Colinus virginianus). Behaviour, 53, 217237.
Bowlby, J. (1991). Ethological Light on Psychoanalytical Problems. In Bateson, P. (Ed.), The Development and Integration of Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 301313.
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Brainard, M. S. & Doupe, A. J. (2000). Auditory Feedback in Learning and Maintenance of Vocal Behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 3140.
Brembs, B. (2009). The Importance of Being Active. Journal of Neurogenetics, 23, 120126.
Bronstein, P. M. (1981). Commitments to Aggression and Nest Sites in Male Betta splendens. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 436449.
Brown, J. S. (1948). Gradients of Approach and Avoidance Responses and Their Relation to Level of Motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 450485.
Brown, M. C., Hopkins, W. G. & Keynes, R. J. (1991). Essentials of Neural Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bruce, R. H. (1935). A Further Study of the Effect of Reward and Drive upon the Maze Performance of Rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 20, 157182.
Buhusi, C. V. & Meck, W. H. (2005). What Makes Us Tick? Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 755765.
Buntin, J. D. (1996). Neural and Hormonal Control of Parental Behavior in Birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 25, 161213.
Burghagen, H. & Ewert, J.-P. (in press). Stimulus Perception. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , 2nd edn.
Burghardt, G. M. (2001). Play: Attributes and Neural Substrates. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13:7 Developmental Psychobiology, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 317356.
Buss, D. M. et al. (1998). Adaptations, Exaptations, and Spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533548.
Buss, R. R., Sun, W. & Oppenheim, R. W. (2006). Adaptive Roles of Programmed Cell Death during Nervous System Development. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 135.
Butler, R. A. & Harlow, H. F. (1954). Persistence of Visual Exploration in Monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 257263.
Caldwell., C. A. & Millen, A. E. (2008). Studying Cumulative Cultural Evolution in the Laboratory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 35293539.
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange Theory of Emotions: A Critical Examination and an Alternative Theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106124.
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Organization for Physiological Homeostasis. Physiological Reviews, 9, 399431.
Cannon, W. B. (1932). The Wisdom of the Body. New York: Norton.
Capranica, R. R. (1965). The Evoked Vocal Response of the Bullfrog: A Study of Communication by Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Caro, T. (2005). Antipredator Defences in Birds and Mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Carroll, S. B. (2008). Evo-Devo and an Expanding Evolutionary Synthesis: A Genetic Theory of Morphological Evolution. Cell, 134, 2536.
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G. & Heinsohn, R. (2013). Animal Personality: What Are Behavioural Ecologists Measuring? Biological Reviews, 88, 465475.
Cartwright, B. A. & Collett, T. S. (1982). How Honey Bees Use Landmarks to Guide Their Return to a Food Source. Nature, 295, 560564.
Cäsar, C., Zuberbühler, K., Young, R. J. & Byrne, R. W. (2013). Titi Monkey Call Sequences Vary with Predator Location and Type. Biology Letters, 9, 20130535.
Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. (2008). Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Chase, I. D. (1982). Dynamics of Hierarchy Formation: The Sequential Development of Dominance Relationships. Behaviour, 80, 218240.
Chittka, L. & Niven, J. (2009). Are Bigger Brains Better? Current Biology, 19, R995R1008.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2012). The Science of Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chow, A. & Hogan, J. A. (2005). The Development of Feather Pecking in Burmese Red Junglefowl: The Influence of Early Experience with Exploratory-rich Environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 93, 283294.
Clayton, N. S. (2015). Ways of Thinking: From Crows to Children and Back Again. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 209241.
Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origins of the Dog: Domestication and Early History. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 720.
Colgan, P. (1989). Animal Motivation. London: Chapman & Hall.
Collett, M. & Collett, T. S. (2000). How Do Insects Use Path Integration for Their Navigation? Biological Cybernetics, 83, 245259.
Collett, M., Chittka, L. & Collett, T. S. (2013). Spatial Memory in Insect Navigation. Current Biology, 23, R789R800.
Collett, T. S. & Collett, M. (2002). Memory Use in Insect Visual Navigation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 542552.
Colonnese, M. T., Stallman, E. L. & Berridge, K. C. (1996). Ontogeny of Action Syntax in Altricial and Precocial Rodents: Grooming Sequences of Rat and Guinea Pig Pups. Behaviour, 133, 11651195.
Colwill, R. C. & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning Devaluation of a Reinforcer Affects Instrumental Responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 120132.
Coppinger, R. & Schneider, R. (1995). Evolution of Working Dogs. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2147.
Coss, R. G. & Biardi, J. E. (1997). Individual Variation in the Antisnake Behavior of California Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Journal of Mammalogy, 73, 294310.
Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and Aversions as Constituents of Instincts. Biological Bulletin, 34, 91107.
Cramer, C. P. & Blass, E. M. (1983). Mechanisms of Control of Milk Intake in Suckling Rats. American Journal of Physiology, 245, R154R159.
Crews, D. & Groothuis, T. (2009). Tinbergen’s Fourth Question, Ontogeny: Sexual and Individual Differentiation. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 5481.
Cullen, E. (1957). Adaptations in the Kittiwake to Cliff-nesting. Ibis, 99, 275302.
Curio, E., Ernst, U. & Vieth, W. (1978). Cultural Transmission of Enemy Recognition: One Function of Mobbing. Science, 202, 899901.
Cuthill, I. (2009). The Study of Function in Behavioral Ecology. In Bolhuis, J J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 107126.
Czeisler, C. A. & Dijk, D.-J. (2001). Human Circadian Physiology and Sleep-Wake Regulation. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 4: Circadian Clocks. New York: Plenum, pp. 531569.
Daan, S. (2000). Learning and Circadian Behavior. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 15, 296299.
Daan, S., Dijkstra, C. & Tinbergen, J. M. (1990). Family Planning in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): The Ultimate Control of Covariance of Laying Date and Clutch Size. Behaviour, 114, 83116.
Daly, M. (1977). Some Experimental Tests of the Functional Significance of Scent-marking by Gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Journal of Compartive and Physiological Psychology, 91, 10821094.
Daly, M. (1979). Why Don’t Male Mammals Lactate? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 78, 325345.
Daly, M. (2015). On Function, Cause, and Being Jerry Hogan’s Student. Behavioural Processes, 117, 7073.
Daly, M. (2016). Killing the Competition. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Daly, M. & Daly, S. (1974). Spatial Distribution of a Leaf-eating Saharan Gerbil (Psammomys obesus) in Relation to Its Food. Mammalia, 38, 591603.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1978). Sex, Evolution, and Behavior. Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution, and Behavior, 2nd edn. Boston, MA: Willard Grant Press.
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1999). Human Evolutionary Psychology and Animal Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 57, 509519.
Darwin, C. (1845). Journal of Researches into the Natural History and Geology of the Countries Visited during the Voyage of H. M. S. Beagle Round the World, 2nd edn. London: Murray.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species. London: Murray.
Darwin, C. (1868). The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication. London: Murray.
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: Murray.
Darwin, C. (1873). Origin of Certain Instincts. Nature, 7, 417418.
Darwin, C. (1896). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, 2nd ed. New York: D. Appleton and Company.
Davies, N. B. (2011). Cuckoo Adaptations: Trickery and Tuning. Journal of Zoology (London), 284, 114.
Davies, N. B. Krebs, J. R. & West, S. A. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Davis, F. C. & Reppert, S. M. (2001). Development of Mammalian Rhythms. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 247290.
Dawkins, M. S. (2008). The Science of Animal Suffering. Ethology, 114, 937945.
Dawkins, M. S. (2015). Animal Welfare and the Paradox of Animal Consciousness. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 47, 537.
Dawkins, M. S. & Guilford, T. (1995). An Exaggerated Preference for Simple Neural Network Models of Signal Evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 261, 357360.
Dawkins, R. (1976a). Hierarchical Organization: A Candidate Principle for Ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 754.
Dawkins, R. (1976b). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal Signals: Information or Manipulation? In Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 282309.
De Bruyn, P. J. N., Tosh, C. A., Bester, M. N. et al. (2011). Sex at Sea: Alternative Mating System in an Extremely Polygynous Mammal. Animal Behaviour, 82, 445451.
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B. & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the Social Processes underlying Human Cumulative Culture. Science, 335, 11141118.
Dewsbury, D. A. (1992). On the Problems Studied in Ethology, Comparative Psychology, and Animal Behavior. Ethology, 92, 89107.
Dewsbury, D. A. (1994). On the Utility of the Proximate-Ultimate Distinction in the Study of Animal Behavior. Ethology, 96, 6368.
Dewsbury, D. A. (1999). The Proximate and the Ultimate: Past, Present, and Future. Behavioural Processes, 46, 189199.
Dezfouli, A. & Balleine, B. W. (2012). Habits, Action Sequences and Reinforcement Learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 10361051.
Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Dickinson, A. (1994). Instrumental Conditioning. In Mackintosh, , pp. 4579.
Dickinson, A. & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational Control of Goal-directed Action. Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 118.
Diedrichsen, J. & Kornysheva, K. (2015). Motor Skill Learning between Selection and Execution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 227233.
Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. (2010). The Memory Function of Sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 115126.
Dijk, D.-J. & von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and Consolidation of Human Sleep, Wakefulness, and Performance by a Symphony of Oscillators. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20, 279290.
Dijk, D.-J. & Archer, S. N. (2009). Light, Sleep, and Circadian Rhythms: Together Again. PLoS Biology, 7(6).
Dijk, D.-J., Duffy, J. F. & Czeisler, C. A. (1992). Circadian and Sleep/Wake Dependent Aspects of Subjective Alertness and Cognitive Performance. Journal of Sleep Research, 1, 112117.
Dijkstra, C., Bult, A. Bijlsma, S., Daan, S., Meijer, T & Zijlstra, M. (1990). Brood Size Manipulations in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): Effects on Offspring and Parent Survival. Journal of Animal Ecology, 59, 269285.
Dingemanse, N, J., Both, C., Drent, P. J. & Tinbergen, J. M. (2004). Fitness Consequences of Avian Personalities in a Fluctuating Environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, 847852.
Dingle, H. & Drake, V. A. (2007). What Is Migration? BioScience, 57, 113121.
Dollard, J. & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and Psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Domjan, M. & Holloway, K. S. (1998). Sexual Learning. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 602613.
Domjan, M., Cusato, B. & Krause, M. (2004). Learning with Arbitrary versus Ecological Conditioned Stimuli: Evidence from Sexual Conditioning. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 11, 232246.
Doty, R. W. (1976). The Concept of Neural Centers. In Fentress, J. C. (Ed.), Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 251265.
Doupe, A. J. & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and Human Speech: Common Themes and Mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567631.
Drachman, D. B. & Sokoloff, L. (1966). The role of Movement in Embryonic Joint Development. Developmental Biology, 14, 401420.
Duncan, I. J. H., Widowski, T. M., Malleau, A. E., Lindberg, A. C. & Petherick, J. C. (1988). External Factors and Causation of Dustbathing in Domestic Hens. Behavioural Processes, 43, 219228.
Durkin, J. M. & Aton, S. J. (2016). Sleep-dependent Potentiation in the Visual System Is at Odds with the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis. Sleep, 39, 155159.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1972). Similarities and Differences between Cultures in Expressive Movements. In Hinde, R. A. (Ed.), Non-verbal Communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 297311.
Eimas, P. D., Miller, J. L. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1987). On Infant Speech Perception and the Acquisition of Language. In Harnad, S. (Ed.), Categorical Perception. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 161195.
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, P., Jusczyk, P. & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech Perception in Infants. Science, 171, 303306.
Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the Human Face, 2nd edn. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Elliot, A. J., Eder, A. B. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2013). Avoidance Motivation and Emotion: Convergency and Divergence. Emotion Review, 5, 308311.
Emlen, S. T. (1967). Migratory Orientation in the Indigo Bunting, Passerina cyanea. Auk, 84, 309342, 463489.
Emlen, S. T. (1969). Bird Migration: Influence of Physiological State upon Celestial Orientation. Science, 165, 716718.
Enquist, M. & Arak, A. (1993). Selection of Exaggerated Male Traits by Female Aesthetic Senses. Nature, 361, 446448.
Enquist, M. & Ghirlanda, S. (2005). Neural Networks and Animal Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Enquist, M. & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Evolution of Social Learning Does Not Explain the Origin of Human Cumulative Culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 246, 129135,
Enquist, M. & Leimar, O. (1983). Evolution of Fighting Behaviour: Decision Rules and Assessment of Relative Strength. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 102, 387410.
Enquist, M. Leimar, O., Ljungberg, T., Mallner, Y. & Segerdahl, N. (1990). A Test of the Sequential Game: Fighting in the Cichlid Fish Nannacara anomala. Animal Behaviour, 40, 114.
Etienne, A. S. & Jeffery, K. J. (2004). Path Integration in Mammals. Hippocampus, 14, 180192.
Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R. & Séguinot, V. (1996). Path Integration in Mammals and Its Interaction with Visual Landmarks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 201209.
Evans, R. M. (1968). Early Aggressive Responses in Domestic Chicks. Animal Behaviour, 16, 2428.
Ewert, J.-P. (1997). Neural Correlates of Key Stimulus and Releasing Mechanism. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 332339.
Ewing, A. W. & Miyan, J. A. (1986). Sexual Selection, Sexual Isolation and the Evolution of Song in the Drosophila repleta Group of Species. Animal Behaviour, 34, 421429.
Falk, J. L. (1961). Production of Polydipsia in Normal Rats by an Intermittent Food Schedule. Science, 133, 195196.
Falk, J. L. (1966). Schedule-induced Polydipsia as a Function of Fixed Interval Length. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 3739.
Falk, J. L. (1971). The Nature and Determinants of Adjunctive Behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 577588.
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2014). Sexual Bias in Probe Tool Manufacture and Use by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 108, 117122.
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2016). The Manifold Use of Pounding Stone Tools by Wild Capuchin Monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil. Behaviour, 153, 421442.
Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural Organization of the Defensive Behavior System Responsible for Fear. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 429438.
Fanselow, M. S. & De Oca, B. M. (1988). Defensive Behaviors. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 653665.
Fantz, R. L. (1957). Form Preferences in Newly Hatched Chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 422430.
Farris, H. E. (1967). Classical Conditioning of Courting Behavior in the Japanese Quail (Coturnix c. japonica). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 213217.
Fawcett, T. W., Hamblin, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2013). Exposing the Behavioral Gambit: The Evolution of Learning and Decision Rules. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 211.
Feekes, F. (1972). “Irrelevant” Ground Pecking in Agonistic Situations in Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour, 43, 186326.
Fehér, O. & Tchernichovski, O. (2013). Vocal Culture in Songbirds: An Experimental Approach to Cultural Evolution. In Bolhuis, & Everaert, , pp. 143156.
Fentress, J. C. & Gadbois, S. (2001). The Development of Action Sequences. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13: Developmental Psychobiology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 393431.
Fentress, J. C. & Stilwell, F. P. (1973). Grammar of a Movement Sequence in Inbred Mice. Nature, 224, 5253.
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R. & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and Imitation of Facial Expressions by Neonates. Science, 218, 179181.
Finn, A. S. et al. (2016). Developmental Dissociation between the Maturation of Procedural Memory and Declarative Memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 212220.
Fleming, A. S. & Blass, E. M. (1994). Psychobiology of the Early Mother-Young Relationship. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 212241.
Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D., Krieger, H. & Wong, P. Y. (1997). Hormonal and Experiential Correlates of Maternal Responsiveness during Pregnancy and the Puerperium in Human Mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 145158.
Fraenkel, G. S. & Gunn, D. L. (1940/1961). The Orientation of Animals. New York: Dover.
Francis, R. C. (1990). Causes, Proximate and Ultimate. Biology and Philosophy, 5, 401415.
Frankland, P. W., Kōhler, S. & Josselyn, S. A. (2013). Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Forgetting. Trends in Neuroscience, 36, 497503.
Freedman, A. H. et al. (2014). Genome Sequencing Highlights the Dynamic Early History of Dogs. PLoS Genetics, 10(1).
Freud, S. (1905). Three Contributions to the Theory of Sexuality. London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1915). Instincts and Their Vicissitudes. London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1940/1949). An Outline of Psycho-analysis. New York: Norton.
Frishkopf, L. S. & Goldstein, M. H. Jr. (1963). Responses to Acoustic Stimuli from Single Units in the Eighth Nerve of the Bullfrog. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 12191228.
Fugazza, C, Pogány, Á. & Miklósi, Á. (2016). Recall of Others’ Actions after Incidental Encoding Reveals Episodic-like Memory in Dogs. Current Biology, 26, 15.
Gadbois, S., Sievert, O., Reeve, C., Harrington, F. H. & Fentress, J. C. (2015). Revisiting the Concept of Behavior Patterns in Animal Behavior with an Example from Food-caching Sequences in Wolves (Canis lupus), Coyotes (Canis latrans), and Red Foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Behavioural Processes, 110, 314.
Galef, B. G. Jr. (1988). Imitation in Animals: History, Definition and Interpretation of Data from the Psychological Laboratory. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (Eds.), Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 328.
Galef, B. G. Jr. (1996). Social Enhancement of Food Preferences in Norway Rats. In Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (Eds.), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. New York: Academic Press, pp. 4964.
Galef, B. G. Jr. (2013). Imitation and Local Enhancement: Detrimental Effects of Consensus Definitions on Analyses of Social Learning in Animals. Behavioural Processes, 100, 123130.
Gallistel, C. R. (1973). Self-stimulation: The Neurophysiology of Reward and Motivation. In Deutsch, J. A. (Ed.), The Physiological Basis of Memory. New York: Academic Press, pp. 175267.
Gallistel, C. R. (1980). The Organization of Action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gallistel, C. R. (1981). Précis of Gallistel’s The Organization of Action: A New Synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 609650.
Gallistel, C. R. (1990a). Representations in Animal Cognition: An Introduction. Cognition, 37, 122.
Gallistel, C. R. (1990b). The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gallistel, C. R. & Matzel, L. D. (2013). The Neuroscience of Learning: Beyond the Hebbian Synapse. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 169200.
Gendron, M. & Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the Past: A Century of Ideas about Emotion in Psychology. Emotion Review, 1, 316339.
Gerhardt, H. C. (2001). Acoustic Communication in Two Groups of Closely Related Treefrogs. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 30, 99167.
Ghirlanda, S. & Enquist, M. (2003). A Century of Generalization. Animal Behaviour, 66, 1536.
Ghirlanda, S., Jansson, L. & Enquist, M. (2002). Chickens Prefer Beautiful Humans. Human Nature, 13, 383389.
Gil, S. A. & Bierema, A. M. K. (2013). On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling. Ethology, 119, 449461.
Gilbert, S. F. (2012). Ecological Developmental Biology: Environmental Signals for Normal Animal Development. Evolution and Development, 14, 2028.
Giraldeau, L.-A. (2005). The Function of Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 199225.
Giraldeau, L.-A. & Dubois, F. (2008). Social Foraging and the Study of Exploitative Behaviour. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 59104.
Giraldeau, L.-A. & Kramer, D. L. (1982). The Marginal Value Theorem: A Quantitative Test Using Load Size Variation in a Central Place Forager the Eastern Chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Animal Behaviour, 30, 10361042.
Glickman, S. E. (1973). Responses and Reinforcement. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 207241.
Glickman, S. E. & Schiff, B. B. (1967). A Biological Theory of Reinforcement. Psychological Review, 74, 81109.
Glimcher, P. W. (2011). Understanding Dopamine and Reinforcement Learning: The Dopamine Reward Prediction Error Hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1564715654.
Gobes, M. H., Fritz, J. B. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Neural Mechanisms of Auditory Learning in Songbirds and Mammals. In Bolhuis, & Everaert, , pp. 295315.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). The Resilience of Language in Humans. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 293311.
Gosling, S. D. (2001). From Mice to Men: What Can We Learn about Personality from Animal Research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 4586.
Gottlieb, G. (1978). Development of Species Identification in Ducklings: IV. Change in Species-specific Perception Caused by Auditory Deprivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 375387.
Gottlieb, G. (1980). Development of Species Identification in Ducklings: VI. Specific Embryonic Experience Required to Maintain Species-typical Perception in Peking Ducklings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94, 579587. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)
Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual Development and Evolution: The Genesis of Novel Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing Nature-Nurture: Prenatal Roots of Instinctive Behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The Spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: A Critique of the Adaptationist Programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 205, 581598.
Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. S. (1988). Exaptation-A Missing Term in the Science of Form. Paleobiology, 8, 415.
Graf, J. S., Balsam, P. D. & Silver, R. (1985). Associative Factors and the Development of Pecking in the Ring Dove. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 447460.
Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. (2006). Evolution of Character Displacement in Darwin’s Finches. Science, 313, 224226.
Gray, R. D., Atkinson, Q. D. & Greenhill, S. J. (2011). Language Evolution and Human History: What a Difference a Day Makes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 10901100.
Graybiel, A. M. & Smith, K. S. (2014). Good Habits, Bad Habits. Scientific American, 310, 3843.
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E. & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and Brain Development. Child Development, 58, 539559.
Griesemer, J. (2000). Development, Culture, and the Units of Inheritance. Philosophy of Science, 67 (Suppl.), S348S368.
Groothuis, T. G. G. (1992). The Influence of Social Experience on the Development and Fixation of the Form of Displays in the Black-headed Gull. Animal Behaviour, 43, 114.
Groothuis, T. G. G. (1994). The Ontogeny of Social Displays: Interplay between Motor Development, Development of Motivational Systems and Social Experience. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 183211.
Groothuis, T. G. G. & Meeuwissen, G. (1992). The Influence of Testosterone on the Development and Fixation of the Form of Displays in Two Age Classes of Young Black-headed Gulls. Animal Behaviour, 43, 189208.
Gross, C. T. & Canteras, N S. (2012). The Many Paths to Fear. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 651658.
Grossman, S. P. (1967). A Textbook of Physiological Psychology. New York: Wiley.
Gwinner, E. (1986). Circannual Rhythms in the Control of Avian Migrations. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 16, 191228.
Gwinner, E. (1996). Circadian and Circannual Programmes in Avian Migration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 3948.
Gwinner, E. & Brandstätter, R. (2001). Complex Bird Clocks. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 356, 18011810.
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S. Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a Spatial Map in the Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 436, 801806.
Hale, C. & Green, L. (1979). Effect of Initial Pecking Consequences on Subsequent Pecking in Young Chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 730735.
Hall, C. S. (1934). Emotional Behavior in the Rat: 1. Defecation and Urination as Measures of Individual Differences in Emotionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 385403.
Hall, W. G. & Browde, J. A. (1986). The Ontogeny of Independent Ingestion in Mice: Or, Why Won’t Infant Mice Feed? Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 211222.
Hall, W. G. & Williams, C. L. (1983). Suckling Isn’t Feeding, or Is It? A Search for Developmental Continuities. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 13, 219254.
Hamburger, V. (1973). Anatomical and Physiological Basis of Embryonic Motility in Birds and Mammals. In Gottlieb, G. (Ed.), Behavioral Embryology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 5276.
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The Genetical Evolution of Social Behaviour. I & II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.
Hamilton, W. D. (1971). Geometry for the Selfish Herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 31, 295311.
Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and Satiation of Response in Intrinsically Motivated Complex Puzzle Performance by Monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 289294.
Harlow, H. F. & Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social Deprivation in Monkeys. Scientific American, Nov. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Harlow, H. F. & Harlow, M. K. (1965). The Affectional Systems. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F. & Stollnitz, F. (Eds.), Behavior of Nonhuman Primates, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.
Harlow, H. F. & Suomi, S. J. (1971). Social Recovery by Isolation-reared Monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 68, 15341538.
Hart, B. L. (1995). Analysing Breed and Gender Differences in Behaviour. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 6577.
Haslam, M., Luncz, L. V. & Falótico, T. (2016). Pre-Columbian Monkey Tools. Current Biology, 26, R515R522.
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hauser, M.D., Yang, C., Berwick, R. C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M. J., Watumull, J., Chomsky, N. & Lewontin, R. C. (2014). The Mystery of Language Evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, May, article 401.
Hebb, D. O. (1946). On the Nature of Fear. Psychological Review, 53, 259276.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.
Heiligenberg, W. (1974). Processes Governing Behavioral States of Readiness. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 173200.
Heinroth, O. (1911). Beiträge zur Biologie: namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Verhandlungen des 5. Internationalen Ornitologen-Kongresses in Berlin, 1910, pp. 389702.
Henning, F. & Meyer, A. (2014). The Evolutionary Genomics of Cichlid Fishes: Explosive Speciation and Adaptation in the Postgenomic Era. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 15, 417441.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1977). The Evolution of Behaviorism. American Psychologist, 32, 593603.
Hess, E. H. (1956). Natural Preferences of Chicks and Ducklings for Objects of Different Colors. Psychological Reports, 2, 477483.
Heyes, C. M. (2012). New Thinking: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 20912096.
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social Learning in Animals: Categories and Mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207231.
Heyes, C. M. (2012). What’s Social about Social Learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 109113.
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., Lazer, D. et al. (2015). Exploration versus Exploitation in Space, Mind, and Society. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 4654.
Hinde, R. A. (1959). Unitary Drives. Animal Behaviour, 7, 130141.
Hinde, R. A. (1960). Energy Models of Motivation. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 14, 199213.
Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behavior, 2nd edn. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hinde, R. A. (1975). The Concept of Function. In Baerends, et al., pp. 215.
Hinde, R. A. (1977). Mother-Infant Separation and the Nature of Inter-individual Relationships: Experiments with Rhesus Monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 196, 2950. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. G. (Eds.). (1973). Constraints on Learning. London: Academic Press.
Hodos, W. & Campbell, C. B. G. (1969). Scala Naturae: Why There Is No Theory in Comparative Psychology. Psychological Review, 76, 337350.
Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early Social Relationships: A Psychobiologist’s View. Child Development, 58, 633647.
Hofer, M. A. (1996). On the Nature and Consequences of Early Loss. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 570581.
Hogan, J. A. (1961). Motivational Aspects of Instinctive Behavior in Betta splendens. Ph.D. thesis, Harvard University.
Hogan, J. A. (1964). Operant Control of Preening in Pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 351354.
Hogan, J. A. (1965). An Experimental Study of Conflict and Fear: An Analysis of Behavior of Young Chicks to a Mealworm. Part I: The Behavior of Chicks Which Do Not Eat the Mealworm. Behaviour, 25, 4597.
Hogan, J. A. (1966). An Experimental Study of Conflict and Fear: An Analysis of Behavior of Young Chicks to a Mealworm. Part II: The Behavior of Chicks Which Eat the Mealworm. Behaviour, 27, 273289.
Hogan, J. A. (1971). The Development of a Hunger System in Young Chicks. Behaviour, 39, 128201.
Hogan, J. A. (1973). How Young Chicks Learn to Recognize Food. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 119139.
Hogan, J. A. (1974). Responses in Pavlovian Conditioning Studies. Science, 186, 156157.
Hogan, J. A. (1977). The Ontogeny of Food Preferences in Chicks and Other Animals. In Barker, L. M., Best, M. & Domjan, M. (Eds.), Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Waco, TX: Baylor University Press.
Hogan, J. A. (1978). An Eccentric View of Development: A Review of The Dynamics of Behavior Development by Zing-Yang Kuo. Contemporary Psychology, 23, 690691.
Hogan, J. A. (1980). Homeostasis and Behaviour. In Toates, F. M. & Halliday, T. R. (Eds.), Analysis of Motivational Processes. London: Academic Press, pp. 321.
Hogan, J. A. (1981). Hierarchy and Behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 625.
Hogan, J. A. (1984a). Cause, Function, and the Analysis of Behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 6571.
Hogan, J. A. (1984b). Pecking and Feeding in Chicks. Learning and Motivation, 15, 360376.
Hogan, J. A. (1988). Cause and Function in the Development of Behavior Systems. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 9. New York: Plenum, pp. 63105.
Hogan, J. A. (1989). The Interaction of Incubation and Feeding in Broody Junglefowl Hens. Animal Behaviour, 38, 121128.
Hogan, J. A. (1994a). The Concept of Cause in the Study of Behavior. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 315.
Hogan, J. A. (1994b). Development of Behavior Systems. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 242264.
Hogan, J. A. (1994c). Structure and Development of Behavior Systems. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 439450.
Hogan, J. A. (1997). Energy Models of Motivation: A Reconsideration. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 53, 89105.
Hogan, J. A. (1998). Motivation. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook, pp. 164175. New York: Garland.
Hogan, J. A. (2001). Development of Behavior Systems. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13: Developmental Psychobiology, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 229279.
Hogan, J. A. (2005). Motivation. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 4170.
Hogan, J. A. (2015). A Framework for the Study of Behavior. Behavioural Processes, 117, 105113.
Hogan, J. A. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2009). Tinbergen’s Four Questions and Contemporary Behavioral Biology. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2534.
Hogan, J. A. & Bolhuis, J. J. (Eds.). (1994). Causal Mechanisms of Behavioural Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hogan, J. A. & Bols, R. J. (1980). Priming of Aggressive Motivation in Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 28, 135142.
Hogan, J. A. & Roper, T. J. (1978). A Comparison of the Properties of Different Reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 8, 155255.
Hogan, J. A. & van Boxel, F. (1993). Causal Factors Controlling Dustbathing in Burmese Red Junglefowl: Some Results and a Model. Animal Behaviour, 46, 627635.
Hogan, J. A., Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Panning, L. & Moffatt, C. A. (1998). Causal Factors Controlling the Brooding Cycle of Broody Junglefowl Hens with Chicks. Behaviour, 135, 957980.
Hogan, J. A., Honrado, G. I. & Vestergaard, K. S. (1991). Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus): II. Internal Factors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 195, 269273.
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. (1966). Social Behavior of the Ruff, Philomachus pugnax (L.). Ardea, 54, 109229.
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. (1992). Female Choice and the Evolution of Mating Strategies in the Ruff Philomachus pugnax (L.). Ardea, 80, 395403.
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. & Hogan, J. A. (1981). Feeding Strategies in the Development of Food Recognition in Young Chicks. Animal Behaviour, 29, 143154.
Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Hogan, J. A. & Ashton, M. C. (1995). Locomotion and Grooming in Crickets: Competition or Time Sharing? Animal Behaviour, 49, 531533.
Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Panning, L. & Hogan, J. A. (1993). Analysis of the Brooding Cycle of Broody Junglefowl Hens with Chicks. Behaviour, 125, 2137.
Hollis, K. L. (1984). The Biological Function of Pavlovian Conditioning: The Best Defence Is a Good Offence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 413425.
Hollis, K. L., ten Cate, C. & Bateson, P. (1991). Stimulus Representation: A Subprocess of Imprinting and Conditioning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 307317.
Hopkins, B. & Butterworth, G. (1990). Concepts of Causality in Explanations of Development. In Butterworth, G. & Bryant, P. (Eds.), Causes of Development, London: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 333.
Horn, G. (1985). Memory, Imprinting, and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hughes, G. M. (Ed.). (1964). Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology: Homeostasis and Feedback Mechanisms, vol.18. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hull, C. L. (1933). Differential Habituation to Internal Stimuli in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 255273.
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Hultsch, H. (1993). Tracing the Memory Mechanisms in the Song Acquisition of Nightingales. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 155171.
Huxley, J. S. (1923). Courtship Activities in the Red-throated Diver (Colymbus stellatus Pontopp.); Together with a Discussion of the Evolution of Courtship in Birds. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society of London, 35, 253292.
Huxley, J. S. (1942). Evolution: The Modern Synthesis. London: Allen & Unwin.
Huxley, T. H. (1893). Collected Essays, Vol. 1, Method and Results. London: Macmillan.
Ibbotson, P. & Tomasello, M. (2016). Language in a New Key. Scientific American, 315(5), 7075.
Immelmann, K. (1972). The Influence of Early Experience upon the Development of Social Behaviour in Estrildine Finches. In Voous, K. H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the XV International Ornithological Congress, Den Haag, 1970. Leiden: Brill, pp. 316338.
Immelmann, K., Lassek, R., Pröve, R. & Bischof, H.-J. (1991). Influence of Adult Courtship Experience on the Development of Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males. Animal Behaviour, 42, 8389.
Ince, S. A., Slater, P. J. B. & Weismann, C. (1980). Changes with Time in the Songs of a Population of Chaffinches. Condor, 82, 285290.
Ingold, T. (2004). Beyond Biology and Culture. The Meaning of Evolution in a Relational World. Social Anthropology, 12, 209221.
Insel, N. & Frankland, P. W. (2015). Mechanism, Function, and Computation in Neural Systems. Behavioural Processes, 117, 411.
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. J. (2014). Evolution in Four Dimensions, revised edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackson, R. R. & Cross, F. R. (2011). Spider Cognition. Advances in Insect Physiology, 41, 115174.
Jackson, R. R. & Wilcox, R. S. (1998). Spider-eating Spiders. American Scientist, 86, 350357.
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology, vol. 2. New York: Holt.
Japyassú, H. F. & Malange, J. (2014). Plasticity, Stereotypy, Intro-individual Variability and Personality: Handle with Care. Behavioural Processes, 109, 4047.
Jarvis, E. D. & Nottebohm, F. (1997). Motor-driven Gene Expression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 40974102.
Johanson, I. B. & Terry, L. M. (1988). Learning in Infancy: A Mechanism of Behavioral Change in Development. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 9. New York: Plenum, pp. 245281.
Johnson, A. & Redish, A. D. (2007). Neural Ensembles in CA3 Transiently Encode Paths Forward of the Animal at a Decision Point. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1217612189.
Johnson, K. P. (2000). The Evolution of Courtship Display Repertoire Size in the Dabbling Ducks (Anatini). Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 13, 634644.
Johnson, M. H. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2000). Predispositions in Perceptual and Cognitive Development. In Bolhuis, J. J. (Ed.), Brain, Perception, Memory: Advances in Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 6984.
Johnson, M. H., Bolhuis, J. J. & Horn, G. (1985). Interaction between Acquired Preferences and Developing Predispositions during Imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 33, 10001006.
Johnson, M. H., Davies, D. C. & Horn, G. (1989). A Critical Period for the Development of a Predisposition in the Chick. Animal Behaviour, 37, 10441046.
Johnson, R. N. & Johnson, L. D. (1973). Intra- and Interspecific Social and Aggressive Behaviour in the Siamese Fighting Fish, Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 21, 665672.
Josselyn, S. A., Köhler, S. & Frankland, P. W. (2015). Finding the Engram. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 521534.
Josselyn, S. A., Kōhler, S. & Frankland, P. W. (2017). Heroes of the Engram. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 46474657.
Jukema, J. & Piersma, T. (2006). Permanent Female Mimics in a Lekking Shorebird. Biology Letters, 2, 161164.
Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). Finding and Remembering Words: Some Beginnings by English-learning Infants. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 170174.
Kacelnik, A. (1984). Central-place Foraging in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch Residence Time. Journal of Animal Ecology, 53, 283299.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. Canada: Doubleday.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development Itself Is the Key to Understanding Developmental Disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389398.
Kellman, P. J. (2002). Perceptual Learning. In Gallistel, C. R. (Ed.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, vol. 3: Learning, Motivation, and Emotion. New York: Wiley, pp. 259299.
Kenny, J. T., Stoloff, M. L., Bruno, J. P. & Blass, E. M. (1979). The Ontogeny of Preferences for Nutritive over Nonnutritive Suckling in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93,752759.
King, A. P. & West, M. J. (1977). Species Identification in the North American Cowbird: Appropriate Responses to Abnormal Song. Science, 195, 10021004.
Kirkpatrick, K. & Hall, G. (2005). Learning and Memory. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 146169.
Kish, G. B. (1966). Studies of Sensory Reinforcement. In Honig, W. K. (Ed.), Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application. New York: Appleton, pp. 109159.
Kissileff, H. R. (1969). Food-associated drinking in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 284300.
Konishi, M. (1965). The Role of Auditory Feedback in the Control of Vocalizations in the White-crowned Sparrow. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 22, 770783.
Kortlandt, A. (1940). Wechselwirkung zwischen Instinkten. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 4, 443520.
Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A Psychobiological Theory of Attachment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 493511.
Kramer, D. L. & Bonenfant, M. (1997). Direction of Predator Approach and the Decision to Flee to a Refuge. Animal Behaviour, 54, 289295.
Kramer, G. (1952). Experiments on bird orientation. Ibis, 94, 265285.
Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, J. T., Webber, M. I. & Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal Prey-selection by the Great Tit (Parus major). Animal Behaviour, 25, 3038.
Kristiansen., M. & Ham, J. (2014). Programmed Cell Death during Neuronal Development: The Sympathetic Neuron Model. Cell Death and Differentiation, 21, 10251035.
Kruijt, J. P. (1962a). On the Evolutionary Derivation of Wing Display in Burmese Red Junglefowl and Other Gallinaceous Birds. Symposia of the Zoological Society, London, 8, 2535.
Kruijt, J. P. (1962b). Imprinting in Relation to Drive Interactions in Burmese Red Junglefowl. Symposia of the Zoological Society, London, 8, 219226.
Kruijt, J. P. (1964). Ontogeny of Social Behaviour in Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour (Suppl. 9).
Kruijt, J. P. (1985). On the Development of Social Attachments in Birds. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 35, 4562.
Kruijt, J. P. & Hogan, J. A. (1967). Social Behavior on the Lek in Black Grouse, Lyrurus tetrix tetrix (L.). Ardea, 55, 203240.
Kruijt, J. P. & Meeuwissen, G. B. (1991). Sexual Preferences of Male Zebra Finches: Effects of Early Adult Experience. Animal Behaviour, 42, 91102.
Kruijt, J. P. & Meeuwissen, G. B. (1993). Consolidation and Modification of Sexual Preferences in Adult Male Zebra Finches. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 6879.
Kruuk, H. (1964). Predators and Anti-predator Behaviour of the Black-headed Gull, Larus ridibundus. Behaviour (Suppl. 11), 1129.
Kuhl, P. K. (1994). Learning and Representation in Speech and Language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 812822.
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early Language Acquisition: Cracking the Speech Code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831843.
Kuhl, P. K. (2015). How Babies Learn Language. Scientific American, 313(5).
Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, R., Stevens, K. N. & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic Experience Alters Phonetic Perception in Infants by 6 Months of Age. Science, 255, 606608.
Kühn, A. (1919). Die Orientierung der Tiere im Raum. Jena.
Kuo, Z. Y. (1921). Giving up Instincts in Psychology. Journal of Philosophy, 18, 645664.
Kuo, Z. Y. (1967). The Dynamics of Behavioral Development. New York: Random House. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Küpper, C., Stocks, M., Risse, J. E. et al. (2016). A Supergene Determines Highly Divergent Male Reproductive Morphs in the Ruff. Nature Genetics, 48, 7983.
Lack, D. (1954). The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lack., D. (1947). Darwin’s Finches. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Laland, K. N. (2015). On Evolutionary Causes and Evolutionary Processes. Behavioural Processes, 117, 97104.
Laland, K. N. & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Laland, K. N. & Galef, B. G. Jr (Eds.). (2009). The Question of Animal Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Laland, K. N. et al. (2014). Does Evolutionary Theory Need a Rethink? Yes, Urgently. Nature, 514, 161164.
Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W. & Uller, T. (2011). Cause and Effect in Biology Revisited: Is Mayr’s Proximate-Ultimate Dichotomy Still Useful? Science, 334, 15121516.
Land, M. F. (2014). Do We Have an Internal Model of the Outside World? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 (doi: 20130045).
Langtimm, C. A. & Dewsbury, D. A. (1991). Phylogeny and Evolution of Rodent Copulatory Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 41, 217225.
Lank, D. B., Smith, C. M., Hanotte, O., Burke, T. & Cooke, F. (1995). Genetic Polymorphism for Alternative Mating Behaviour in Lekking Male Ruff Philomachus pugnax. Nature, 378, 5962.
Larsen, B. H., Hogan, J. A. & Vestergaard, K. S. (2000). Development of Dustbathing Behavior Sequences in the Domestic Fowl: The Significance of Functional Experience. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 512.
Larson, G. & Fuller, D. Q. (2014). The Evolution of Animal Domestication. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 45, 115136.
Lashley, K. S. (1938). Experimental Analysis of Instinctive Behavior. Psychological Review, 45, 445471.
Lashley, K. S. (1950). In Search of the Engram. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 454482.
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The Problem of Serial Order in Behavior. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. New York: Wiley, pp. 112136.
Leadbeater, E. (2015). What Evolves in the Evolution of Social Learning. Journal of Zoology, 295, 411.
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2007). Social Learning in Insects – From Miniature Brains to Consensus Building. Current Biology, 17, R703R713.
LeBeouf, B. J. (1974). Male-male Competition and Reproductive Success in Elephant Seals. American Zoologist, 14, 163176.
LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the Emotional Brain. Neuron, 73, 653676.
Lefebvre, L. (2015). Should Neuroecologists Separate Tinbergen’s Four Questions? Behavioural Processes, 117, 9296.
Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A Critique of Konrad Lorenz’s Theory of Instinctive Behaviour. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 337363.
Lehrman, D. S. (1955). The Physiological Basis of Parental Feeding Behaviour in the Ring Dove (Streptopelia risoria). Behaviour, 7, 241286.
Lehrman, D. S. (1965). Interaction between Internal and External Environments in the Regulation of the Reproductive Cycle of the Ring Dove. In Beach, F. A. (Ed.), Sex and Behavior. New York: Wiley, pp. 355380.
Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and Conceptual Issues in the Nature-Nurture Problem. In Aronson, et al., pp. 1752. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. H. (1959). What the Frog’s Eye Tells the Frog’s Brain. Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers, 47, 19401951.
Levinson, S. C. & Gray, R. D. (2012). Tools from Evolutionary Biology Shed New Light on the Diversification of Languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 167173.
Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The Units of Selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 118.
Lieberman, P. (2016). The Evolution of Language and Thought. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 94, 127146.
Locke, J. L. (1993). The Child’s Path to Spoken Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Locke, J. L. (1994). The Biological Building Blocks of Spoken Language. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 300324.
Locke, J. L. & Snow, C. (1997). Social Influences on Vocal Learning in Human and Nonhuman Primates. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 274292.
Loeb, J. (1918). Forced Movements, Tropisms and Animal Conduct. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Logan, C. A. (1983). Biological Diversity in Avian Vocal Learning. In Zeiler, M. D. & Harzem, P. (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior, vol. 3: Biological Factors in Learning. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 137213, 289413. [Tr. as: Companions as Factors in the Bird’s Environment. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 101–258 – excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.]
Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 289300, 307318, 324331. [Tr. as: The Establishment of the Instinct Concept. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 259–315.]
Lorenz, K. (1941). Vergleichende Bewegungstudien an Anatinen. Journal für Ornithologie, 89, Sonderheft. (Tr. by Martin, R. D. as: Comparative Studies of the Motor Patterns of Anatinae. In Lorenz, (1971). Studies in Animal and Human Behaviour, vol. 2. London: Methuen, pp. 14114.)
Lorenz, K. (1950). The Comparative Method in Studying Innate Behaviour Patterns. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 221268.
Lorenz, K. (1956). Plays and Vacuum Activities. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l‘homme. Paris: Masson, pp. 633637.
Lorenz, K. (1961). Phylogenetische Anpassung und adaptive Modifikation des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 18, 139187.
Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and Modification of Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Lorenz, K. (1966). On Aggression. London: Methuen.
Lorenz, K. (1970). Studies in Animal and Human Behaviour, vol. 1. (Tr. by Martin, R.). London: Methuen.
Lorenz, K. (1981). The Foundations of Ethology. New York: Springer.
Lorenz, K. & Tinbergen, N. (1939). Taxis und Instinkthandlung in der Eirollbewegung der Graugans. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 2, 129. [Tr. as: Taxis and Instinctive Behavior Pattern in Egg-rolling by the Greylag Goose. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 316–350.]
Losos, J. B. (2010). Adaptive Radiation, Ecological Opportunity, and Evolutionary Determinism. American Naturalist, 175, 623639.
Lotem, A. (1993). Learning to Recognize Nestlings Is Maladaptive for Cuckoo Cuculus canorus Hosts. Nature, 362, 743745.
Lovic, V. & Fleming, A. S. (2015). Propagation of Maternal Behavior across Generations Is Associated with Changes in Non-maternal Cognitive and Behavioral Processes. Behavioural Processes, 117, 4247.
Lowe, P. R. (1936). The Finches of the Galapagos in Relation to Darwin’s Conception of Species. Ibis, 78, 310321.
Macedonia, J. M. & Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among Mammalian Alarm Call Systems and the Problem of Meaning in Animal Signals. Ethology, 93, 177197.
Macintosh, N. J. (1994). Animal Learning and Cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Marler, P. (1955). Characteristics of Some Animal Calls. Nature, 176, 68.
Marler, P. (1970a). A Comparative Approach to Vocal Learning: Song Development in White-crowned Sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology (monograph suppl.), 71, 125.
Marler, P. (1970b). Birdsong and Speech Development: Could There Be Parallels? American Scientist, 58, 669673.
Marler, P. (1976). Sensory Templates in Species-specific Behavior. In Fentress, J. C. (Ed.), Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 314329. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Marler, P. (1984). Song Learning: Innate Species Differences in the Learning Process. In Marler, P. & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.), The Biology of Learning. Dahlem Workshop Reports-Life Sciences Research Report 29. Berlin: Springer.
Marler, P. (1991). Song Learning Behavior: The Interface with Neuroethology. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 199206.
Marler, P. & Evans, C. (1996). Bird Calls: Just Emotional Displays or Something More? Ibis, 138, 2633.
Marler, P. & Peters, S. (1982). Subsong and Plastic Song: Their Role in the Vocal Learning Process. In Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H. (Eds.), Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, pp. 2550.
Marler, P. & Tamura, M. (1964). Culturally Transmitted Patterns of Vocal Behavior in Sparrows. Science, 146, 14831486.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: Freeman
Martin, P. & Caro, T. M. (1985). On the Functions of Play and Its Role in Behavioral Development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 59103.
Mason, G. J. (2010). Species Differences in Responses to Captivity: Stress, Welfare and the Comparative Method. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25, 713721.
Maye, J., Werker, J. F. & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant Sensitivity to Distributional Information Can Affect Phonetic Discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101B111.
Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group Selection and Kin Selection. Nature, 201, 11451147.
Maynard Smith, J. & Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of Animal Conflict. Nature, 246, 1518.
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and Effect in Biology. Science, 134, 15011506.
Mayr, E. (1982). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mayr, E. (1988). The Multiple Meanings of Teleological. Toward a New Philosophy of Biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 3866.
Mayr, E. (1993). Proximate and Ultimate Causations. Biology and Philosophy, 8, 9394.
McAleer, K. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2006). Testing Central Place Foraging in Eastern Chipmunks, Tamias Striatus, by Altering Loading Functions. Animal Behaviour, 71, 14471453.
McDougall, W. (1923). An Outline of Psychology. London: Methuen.
McFarland, D. J. (1970). Behavioural Aspects of Homeostasis. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 3, 126.
McFarland, D. J. (1974). Time-sharing as a Behavioral Phenomenon. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 201225.
McFarland, D. J. & Houston, A. (1981). Quantitative Ethology: The State Space Approach. London: Pitman.
McKeon, R. (Ed.). (1947). Introduction to Aristotle. New York: Random House.
McKinney, F. (1975). The Evolution of Duck Displays. In Baerends, et al., pp. 331357.
McLaren, I. P. L. (1994). Representation Development in Associative Systems. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 377402.
McNamara, J. M. & Houston, A. I. (2009). Integrating Function and Mechanism. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 24, 670675.
McNamara, J. M., Houston, A. I. & Collins, E. J. (2001). Optimality Models in Behavioral Biology. SIAM Review, 42, 413466.
Meijer, T., Daan, S. & Hall, M. (1990). Family Planning in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): The Proximate Control of Covariation of Laying Date and Clutch Size. Behaviour, 114, 117160.
Mendl, M. T., Burman, O. H. P. & Paul, E. S. (2010). An Integrative and Functional Framework for the Study of Animal Emotion and Mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 28952904.
Mendl, M. T. & Paul, E. S. (2016). Bee Happy. Science, 353, 14991500.
Menzel, R. & Fischer, J. (2011). Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Meyer, A. (1993). Phylogenetic Relationships and Evolutionary Processes in East African Cichlid Fishes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 8, 279284.
Meyer, A. (2015). Extreme Evolution. Scientific American, 313(4), 7075.
Miller, N. E. (1957). Experiments on Motivation. Science, 126, 12711278.
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of Basic S-R Concepts: Extensions to Conflict Behavior, Motivation, and Social Learning. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science, vol. 2, pp. 196292. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ming, G. & Song, H. (2011). Adult Neurogenesis in the Mammalian Brain: Significant Answers and Significant Questions. Neuron, 70, 687702.
Mirmiran, M., Maas, Y. G. H. & Ariagno, R. L. (2003). Development of Fetal and Neonatal Sleep and Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 7, 321334.
Mistleberger, R. E. (2011). Neurobiology of Food Anticipatory Circadian Rhythms. Physiology and Behavior, 104, 535545.
Mistleberger, R. E. & Rusak, B. (2005). Biological Rhythms and Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 7196.
Mittelstaedt, M.-L. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1980). Homing by Path Integration in a Mammal. Naturwissenschaften, 67, 566567.
Moczek, A. P. et al. (2015). The Significance and Scope of Evolutionary Developmental Biology: A Vision for the 21st Century. Evolution and Development, 17, 198219.
Moffatt, C. A. & Hogan, J. A. (1992). Ontogeny of Chick Responses to Maternal Food Calls in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 9296.
Møller, A. P. (1988). Female Choice Selects for Male Sexual Tail Ornaments in a Swallow. Nature, 332, 640642.
Møller, A. P. & de Lope, F. (1994). Differential Costs of a Secondary Sexual Character: An Experimental Test of the Handicap Principle. Evolution, 48, 16761683.
Montgomerie, R., Lyon, B. & Holder, K. (2001). Dirty Ptarmigan: Behavioral Modification of Conspicuous Male Plumage. Behavioral Ecology, 12, 429438.
Mook, D. G. (1987). Motivation: The Organization of Action. New York: Norton.
Moore, B. R. (1973). The Role of Directed Pavlovian Reactions in Simple Instrumental Learning in the Pigeon. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 159188.
Moran, T. H. (1986). Environmental and Neural Determinants of Behavior in Development. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 2: Neuropsychology. New York: Plenum, pp. 99128.
Morand-Ferron, J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2010). Behavioral Ecology, 21, 343348.
Morgan, C. L. (1896). Habit and Instinct. London: Arnold.
Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. (2012). The Evolutionary Basis of Human Social Learning. Procedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 653662.
Moro, A. (2014). On the Similarity between Syntax and Actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 109110.
Moro, A. (2014). Response to Pulvermüller: The Syntax of Actions and Other Metaphors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 221.
Morris, D. (1956). [Discussion following Lorenz] In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de I’homme. Paris: Masson, pp. 642643.
Morris, W. (Ed.). (1969). The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Moser, M-B. & Moser, E. I. (2016). Where Am I? Where Am I Going? Scientific American, 314(1), pp. 2633.
Mulder, C. K., Gerkema, M. P. & Van der Zee, E. A. (2013). Circadian Clocks and Memory: Time-Place Learning. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 6, article 8.
Neill, S. R. & Cullen, J. M. (1974). Experiments on Whether Schooling by Their Prey Affects the Hunting Behaviour of Cephalopods and Fish Predators. Journal of Zoology, 172, 549569.
Nelson, D. A. (1997). Social Interaction and Sensitive Phases for Song Learning: A Critical Review. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 722.
Nelson, K. (1964). The Temporal Patterning of Courtship Behaviour in the Glandulocaudine Fishes. Behaviour, 24, 90146.
Nelson, R. J. (2016). An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology, 4th edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Fernandez, N., Newport, E. L. & Bavelier, D. (2015). Neural Systems Supporting Linguistic Structure, Linguistic Experience, and Symbolic Communication in Sign Language and Gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1168411689.
Novak, M. & Harlow, H. F. (1975). Social Recovery of Monkeys Isolated for the First Year of Life: I. Rehabilitation and Therapy. Developmental Psychology, 11, 453465.
O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon.
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N. & Feldman, M. W. (1996). Niche Construction. American Naturalist, 147, 641648.
Olkowicz, S, Kocourek, M., Lučan, R. K. et al. (2016). Birds have Primate-like Numbers of Neurons in the Forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 72557260.
Olds, J. (1958). Self-stimulation of the Brain. Science, 127, 315324.
Oppenheim, R. W. (1974). The Ontogeny of Behavior in the Chick Embryo. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 133172.
Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic Adaptations and Retrogressive Processes in the Development of the Nervous System and Behaviour: A Neuroembryological Perspective. In Connolly, K. J. & Prechtl, H. F. R. (Eds.), Maturation and Development: Biological and Psychological Perspectives. Philadelphia: Lippincott, pp. 73109. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Oppenheim, R. W. (1991). Cell Death during Development of the Nervous System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 14, 453501.
Orr, H. A. (2009). Fitness and Its Role in Evolutionary Genetics. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, 531539.
Oster, H. (1978). Facial Expression and Affect Development. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), The Development of Affect. New York: Plenum, pp. 4376.
Ottoni, E. B. & Izar, P. (2008). Capuchin Monkey Tool Use: Overview and Implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17, 171178.
Oyama, S. (1985). The Ontogeny of Information: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E. & Gray, R. D. (2001). Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pagel, M., Atkinson, Q. D. & Meade, A. (2007). Frequency of Word-use Predicts Rates of Lexical Evolution throughout European History. Nature, 449, 717720.
Pagnotta, M. (2014). On the Controversy over Non-human Culture: The Reasons for Disagreement and Possible Directions toward Consensus. Behavioural Processes 109, 95100.
Palameta, B. & Lefebvre, L. (1985). The social Transmission of a Food-finding Technique in Pigeons: What Is Learned? Animal Behaviour, 33, 892896.
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective Consciousness: Core Emotional Feelings in Animals and Humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 3080.
Panksepp, J. (2010). Affective Consciousness in Animals: Perspectives on Dimensional and Primary Process Emotion Approaches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 29052907.
Paul, E. S., Harding, E. J. & Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring Emotional Processes in Animals: The Utility of a Cognitive Approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 469491.
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Payne, R. B., Thompson, W. L., Fiala, K. L. & Sweany, L. L. (1981). Local Song Traditions in Indigo Buntings: Cultural Transmission of Behaviour Patterns across Generations. Behaviour, 77, 199221.
Pennisi, E. (2016a). Tracking how Humans Evolve in Real Time. Science, 352, 876877.
Pennisi, E. (2016b). Fossil Fishes Challenge “Urban Legend” of Evolution. Science, 353, 14831484.
Petherick, J. C., Seawright, E., Waddington, D., Duncan, I. J. H. & Murphy, L. B. (1995). The Role of Perception in the Causation of Dustbathing Behaviour in Domestic Fowl. Animal Behaviour, 49, 15211530.
Petitto, L. A. & Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the Manual Mode: Evidence for the Ontogeny of lLnguage. Science, 251, 14931496.
Pfaffmann, C. (1960). The Pleasures of Sensation. Psychological Review, 67, 253268.
Phelps, S. M., Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. (2001). Vestigial Preference Functions in Neural Networks and Túngara Frogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 13161134166.
Piersma, T. & van Gils, J. A. (2011). The Flexible Phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pievani, T. & Serrelli, E. (2011). Exaptation in Human Evolution: How to Test Adaptive vs Exaptive Evolutionary Hypotheses. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 89, 923.
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.
Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought. New York: Viking Penguin.
Pittendrigh, C. S. (1958). Adaptation, Natural Selection, and Behavior. In Roe, A. & Simpson, G. G. (Eds.), Behavior and Evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, pp. 390416.
Polsky, R. H. (1975). Hunger, Prey Feeding, and Predatory Aggression. Behavioral Biology, 13, 8193.
Pulvermüller, F. & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active Perception: Sensorimotor Circuits as a Cortical Basis for Language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 351360.
Raby, C. R., Alexisi, D. M., Dickinson, A. & Clayton, N. S. (2007). Planning for the Future by Western Scrub-Jays. Nature, 445, 919921.
Raff, R. A. (2000). Evo-Devo: The Evolution of a New Discipline. Nature Reviews Genetics, 1, 7479.
Rasa, O. A. E. (1971). Appetence for Aggression in Juvenile Damsel Fish. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, Beiheft, 7.
Réale, D, Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating Animal Temperament within Ecology and Evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291318.
Redish, A. D. (2001). The Hippocampal Debate: Are We Asking the Right Questions? Behavioral Brain Research, 127, 8198.
Reisbick, S. H. (1973). Development of Food Preferences in Newborn Guinea Pigs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 427442.
Rendell, L. et al. (2011). Cognitive Culture: Theoretical and Empirical Insights into Social Learning Strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 6876.
Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian Conditioning and Its Proper Control Procedures. Psychological Review, 74, 7180.
Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Behavioral Studies of Pavlovian Conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 329352.
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W.F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 6499.
Resende, B. D., Nagy-Reis, M. B., Lacerda, F. N., Pagnotta, M. & Savalli, C. (2014). Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus sp.) Learning how to Crack Nuts: Does Variability Decline throughout Development? Behavioural Processes, 109, 8994.
Rhoad, K. D., Kalat, J. W. & Klopfer, P. (1975). Aggression and Avoidance by Betta splendens toward Natural and Artificial Stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior, 3, 271276.
Rial, R. V. et al. (2010). Evolution of Wakefulness, Sleep and Hibernation: From Reptiles to Mammals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 11441160.
Rice, J. C. (1978). Effects of Learning Constraints and Behavioural Organization on the Association of Vocalizations and Hunger in Burmese Red Junglefowl Chicks. Behaviour, 67, 259298.
Richards, R. J. (1992). The Meaning of Evolution. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Richter, C. P. (1942). Total Self Regulatory Functions in Animals and Human Beings. The Harvey Lectures Series, 38, 63103.
Riebel, K. (2003). The “Mute” Sex Revisited; Vocal Production and Perception Learning in Female Songbirds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 33, 4986.
Rivkees, S. A. (2003). Developing Circadian Rhythmicity in Infants. Pediatrics, 112, 373381.
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The Mirror-neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.
Roberts, E. P. & Weigl, P. D. (1984). Habitat Preference in the Dark-eyed Junco (Junco hyemalis): The Role of Photoperiod and Dominance. Animal Behaviour, 32, 709714.
Rodenburg, T. B. et al. (2004). Feather Pecking in Laying Hens: New Insights and Directions for Research? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 86, 291298.
Roeder, K. D. (1967). Nerve Cells and Insect Behavior, revised edn. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Roesch, M. R., Esber, G. R., Li, J., Daw, N. D. & Schoenbaum, G. (2013). Surprise! Neural Correlates of Pearce-Hall and Rescorla-Wagner Coexist within the Brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, pp. 11901200.
Romanes, G. J. R. (1883). Mental Evolution in Animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.
Roper, T. J. (1973). Nesting Material as a Reinforcer for Female Mice. Animal Behaviour, 21, 733740.
Roper, T. J. (1975). Nest Material and Food as Reinforcers for Fixed-ratio Responding in Mice. Learning and Motivation, 6, 327343.
Rozin, P. (1976). The Selection of Foods by Rats, Humans, and other Animals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 6, 2176.
Rozin, P. (1984). The Acquisition of Food Habits and Preferences. In Matarazzo, J. D., Weiss, S. M., Herd, J. A., Miller, N. E. & Weiss, S. M. (Eds.), Behavioral Health: A Handbook of Health Enhancement and Disease Prevention. New York: Wiley.
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core Affect and the Psychological Construction of Emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145172.
Rutter, M. (1991). A Fresh Look at “Maternal Deprivation.” In Bateson, P. (Ed.), The Development and Integration of Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 331374.
Rutter, M. (2002). Nature, Nurture, and Development: From Evangelism through Science toward Policy and Practice. Child Development, 73, 121.
Ryan, M. J. (2009). The Evolution of Behavior, and Integrating It towards a Complete and Correct Understanding of Behavioral Biology. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 127146.
Ryan, M. J. (2005). Evolution of Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 294314.
Ryan, M. J. & Cummings, M. E. (2003). Perceptual Biases and Mate Choice. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 44, 437459.
Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. (1999). Phylogenetic Influences on Mating Call Preferences in Female Túngara Frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus). Animal Behaviour, 57, 945956.
Sachs, B. D. & Barfield, R, J. (1976). Functional Analysis of Masculine Copulatory Behavior in the Rat. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 7, 92154.
Sackett, G. P. (1970). Unlearned Responses, Differential Rearing Experiences, and the Development of Social Attachments by Rhesus Monkeys. In Rosenblum, L. A. (Ed.), Primate Behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on Statistical Language Learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172196.
Sahay, A., Wilson, D. A. & Hen, R. (2011). Pattern Separation: A Common Function for New Neurons in Hippocampus and Olfactory Bulb. Neuron, 70, 582588.
Sanotra, G. S., Vestergaard, K. S., Agger, J. F. & Lawson, L. G. (1995). The Relative Preferences for Feathers, Straw, Wood-shavings and Sand for Dustbathing, Pecking and Scratching in Domestic Chicks. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 43, 263277.
Savory, C. J. (1979). Changes in Food Intake and Body Weight of Bantam Hens during Breeding. Applied Animal Ethology, 5, 283288.
Schachter, S. & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, Social, and Physiological Determinants of Emotional State. Psychological Review, 69, 379399.
Schacter, D. L. (2009). Memory Systems, Neural Basis Of. In Bayne, T., Clemens, A. & Wilken, P. (Eds.), The Oxford Companion to Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 432436.
Schacter, D. L. & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Schiller, P. H. (1957). Manipulative Patterns in the Chimpanzee (1949). In Schiller, C. H. (Ed.), Instinctive Behavior. New York: International Universities Press, pp. 264287.
Schjelderup-Ebbe, Th. (1922). Beiträge zur Social-psychologie des Haushuhns. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 92, 225252.
Schluter, D. (1996). Ecological Causes of Adaptive Radiation. American Naturalist, 148 (Suppl.), S40S64.
Schmid-Hempel, P., Kacelnik, A. & Houston, A. I. (1985). Honeybees Maximise Efficiency by Not Filling Their Crop. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17, 6166.
Schneirla, T. C. (1949). Levels in the Psychological Capacities of Animals. In Sellars, R. W., McGill, V. J. & Farber, M. (Eds.), Philosophy for the Future: The Quest for Modern Materialism. New York: Macmillan.
Schneirla, T. C. (1956). Interrelationships of the “Innate” and the “Acquired” in Instinctive Behavior. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de 1’homme. Paris: Masson.
Schneirla, T. C. (1965). Aspects of Stimulation and Organization in Approach/Withdrawal Processes Underlying Vertebrate Behavioral Development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1, 174.
Schneirla, T. C. & Rosenblatt, J. S. (1961). Behavioral Organization and Genesis of the Social Bond in Insects and Mammals. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 31, 223253.
Schneirla, T. C., Rosenblatt, J. S. & Tobach, E. (1963). Maternal Behavior in the Cat. In Rheingold, H. (Ed.), Maternal Behavior in Mammals. New York: Wiley.
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral Theories and the Neurophysiology of Reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87115.
Sevenster, P. (1961). A Causal Analysis of a Displacement Activity (Fanning in Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Behaviour, Suppl. 9, 1170.
Sevenster, P. (1968). Motivation and Learning in Sticklebacks. In Ingle, D. (Ed.), Central Nervous System and Fish Behaviour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 233245.
Sevenster, P. (1973). Incompatibility of Response and Reward. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 265283.
Sevenster, P. & van Roosmalen, M. E. (1985). Cognition in Sticklebacks: Some Experiments on Operant Conditioning. Behaviour93, 170183.
Sevenster-Bol, A. C. A. (1962). On the Causation of Drive Reduction after a Consummatory Act. Archive Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 15, 175236.
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic Communication in a Free-ranging Primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 10701094.
Shatz, C. J. (1992). The Developing Brain. Scientific American, 267, 6067. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Sheffield, F. D. (1966). A Drive Induction Theory of Reinforcement. In Haber, R. N. (Ed.), Current Research in Motivation. New York: Holt, pp. 98111.
Shelton, J. R. & Caramazza, A. (1999). Deficits in Lexical and Semantic Processing. Implications for Models of Normal Language. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 527.
Sherman, P. W. (1988). The Levels of Analysis. Animal Behaviour, 36, 616619.
Sherry, D. F. (1977). Parental Food-calling and the Role of the Young in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus g. spadiceus). Animal Behaviour, 25, 594601.
Sherry, D. F. (1981). Parental Care and Development of Thermoregulation in Red Junglefowl. Behaviour, 76, 250279.
Sherry, D. F. (1985). Food Storage by Birds and Mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 153188.
Sherry, D. F. (2009). Do Ideas about Function Help in the Study of Causation? In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 147162.
Sherry, D. F. & Duff, S. J. (1996). Behavioural and Neural Bases of Orientation in Food-storing Birds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 165171.
Sherry, D. F. & Schacter, D. L. (1987). The Evolution of Multiple Memory Systems. Psychological Review, 94, 439454.
Sherry, D. F. & Strang, C. G. (2015). Contrasting Styles in Cognition and Behaviour in Bumblebees and Honeybees. Behavioural Processes, 117, 5969.
Sherry, D. F. & Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus and Memory for Food Caches in Black-capped Chickadees. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 308318.
Sherry, D. F., Mrosovsky, N. & Hogan, J. A. (1980). Weight Loss and Anorexia during Incubation in Birds. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94, 8998.
Shettleworth, S. J. (1972). Constraints on Learning. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 4, 168.
Shettleworth, S. J. (1975). Reinforcement and the Organization of Behavior in Golden Hamsters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 5687.
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
Shubin, N., Tabin, C. & Carroll, S. (2009). Deep Homology and the Origins of Evolutionary Novelty. Nature, 457, 818823.
Sih, A., Bell, A. & Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioral Syndromes: An Ecological and Evolutionary Overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19, 372378.
Silver, R. (1990). Biological Timing Mechanisms with Special Emphasis on the Parental Behavior of Doves. In Dewsbury, D. A. (Ed.), Contemporary Issues in Comparative Psychology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 252277.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are Theories of Learning Necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193216.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan.
Slack, J. M. W. (1991). From Egg to Embryo. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Slater, P. J. B. (1981). Individual Differences in Animal Behavior. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H. (Eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, vol. 4. New York: Plenum, pp. 3549.
Slater, P. J. B. (1986). The Cultural Transmission of Bird Song. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 9497.
Smith, K. S. & Graybiel, A. M. (2013). A Dual Operator View of Habitual Behavior Reflecting Cortical and Striatal Dynamics. Neuron, 79, 361374.
Snell-Rood, E. C. (2013). An Overview of the Evolutionary Causes and Consequences of Behavioural Plasticity. Animal Behaviour, 85, 10441011.
Snowdon, C. T. & Hausberger, M. (Eds.). (1997). Social Influences on Vocal Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Sober, E. (2001). The Two Faces of Fitness. In Singh, R. S. et al. (Eds.), Thinking about Evolution, vol. 2. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 309321.
Sokolowski, M. B. (2001). Drosophila: Genetics Meets Behaviour. Nature Reviews Genetics, 2, 879890.
Sokolowski, M. B. (2010). Social Interactions in “Simple” Model Systems. Neuron, 65, 780794.
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M. & Lefebvre, L. (2008). Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced into Novel Environments. American Naturalist, 172, S63S71.
Spalding, D. A. (1873/1954). Instinct with Original Observations on Young Animals. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2, 211.
Speirs, A. L. (1962). Thalidomide and Congenital Abnormalities. Lancet, 279, 303305.
Spencer, H. (1864). Principles of Biology. London.
Staddon, J. E. R. & Ayres, S. L. (1976). Sequential and Temporal Properties of Behavior Induced by a Schedule of Periodic Food Delivery. Behaviour, 54, 2649.
Staddon, J. E. R. & Cerutti, D. T. (2003). Operant Conditioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 115144.
Staddon, J. E. R. & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The “Superstition” Experiment: A Reexamination of Its Implication for the Principles of Adaptive Behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 343.
Stamps, J. A. (2016). Individual Differences in Behavioural Plasticities. Biological Reviews, 91, 534567.
Stamps, J. A. & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). Developmental Perspectives on Personality: Implications for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies of Individual Differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 40294041.
Steiner, A. P. & Redish, A. (2014). Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Regret in Rat Decision-making on a Neuroeconomic Task. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 9951002.
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human Facial Expressions in Response to Taste and Smell Stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 13, 257295.
Stellar, E. (1960). Drive and Motivation. In Field, J., Magoun, H. W. & Hall, V. E. (Eds.), Handbook of Physiology (Section 1, Vol. 3). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 15011528.
Stephan, F. K. (2001). Food-entrainable Oscillators in Mammals. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 223246.
Stevens, M. (2005). The Role of Eyespots as Anti-predator Mechanisms, Principally Demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573588.
Stevens, M. (2013). Sensory Ecology, Behaviour, and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stevens, M., Hardman, C. J. & Stubbins, C. L. (2008). Conspicuousness, Not Eye Mimicry, Makes ‘Eyespots’ Effective Antipredator Signals. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 525531.
Stevenson, J. G. (1967). Reinforcing Effects of Chaffinch Song. Animal Behaviour, 15, 427432.
Struhsaker, T. T. (1967). Social Structure among Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Behaviour, 29, 6121.
Suboski, M. D. & Bartashunas, C. (1984). Mechanisms for the Social Transmission of Pecking Preferences in Neonatal Chicks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 182194.
Sutherland, N. S. (1964). The Learning of Discriminations by Animals. Endeavour, 23, 148152.
Tarsitano, M. & Andrew, R. (1999). Scanning and Route Selection in the Jumping Spider Portia Labiata. Animal Behaviour, 58, 255265.
ten Cate, C. (1984). The Influence of Social Relations on the Development of Species Recognition in Zebra Finch Males. Behaviour, 91, 263285.
ten Cate, C. (1986). Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males Exposed to Two Species. I. A Case of Double Imprinting. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 248252.
ten Cate, C. (1987). Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males Exposed to Two Species. II. The Internal Representation Resulting from Double Imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 35, 321330.
ten Cate, C. (1994). Perceptual Mechanisms in Imprinting and Song Learning. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 116146.
ten Cate, C. (1991). Behaviour-contingent Exposure to Song and Zebra Finch Song Learning. Animal Behaviour, 42, 857859.
Terman, M. (1983). Behavioral Analysis and Circadian Rhythms. In Zeiler, M. D. & Harzen, P. (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior, vol. 3: Biological Factors in Learning. New York: Wiley, pp. 103141.
Thelen, E. (1985). Expression as Action: A Motor Perspective on the Transition from Spontaneous to Instrumental Behaviors. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The Development of Expressive Behavior. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, pp. 221267.
Thelen, E. (1995). Motor Development: A New Synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 7995.
Thelen, E. & Fisher, C. M. (1992). Newborn Stepping: An Explanation for a “Disappearing Reflex.” Developmental Psychology, 18, 760775.
Thompson, T. I. (1963). Visual Reinforcement in Siamese Fighting Fish. Science, 141, 5557.
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal Intelligence. Psychological Review, Monograph suppl. 2, 1113.
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal Intelligence. New York: Macmillan.
Thorndike, E. L. (1931). Human Learning. New York: Century.
Thorpe, W. H. (1956). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London: Methuen.
Thorpe, W. H. (1958). The Learning of Song Patterns by Birds, with Especial Reference to the Song of the Chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Ibis, 100, 535570.
Thorpe, W. H. (1961). Bird Song. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Thorpe, W. H. & Jones, F. G. W. (1937). Olfactory Conditioning and Its Relation to the Problem of Host Selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 124, 5681.
Timberlake, W. (1994). Behavior Systems, Associationism, and Pavlovian Conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 405420.
Timberlake, W. (2004). Is Operant Contingency Enough for a Science of Purposive Behavior? Behavior and Philosophy, 32, 197229.
Tinbergen, J. M. (1981). Foraging Decisions in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Ardea, 69, 167.
Tinbergen, J. M. & Daan, S. (1990). Family Planning in the Great Tit (Parus major): Optimal Clutch Size as Integration of Parent and Offspring Fitness. Behaviour, 114, 161190.
Tinbergen, N. (1932/1972). On the Orientation of the Digger Wasp Philanthus triangulum Fabr. In Tinbergen, N. (Ed.), The Animal in Its World, vol. 1. London: George Allen & Unwin, pp. 103127.
Tinbergen, N. (1940). Die Übersprungbewegung. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 4, 140.
Tinbergen, N. (1942). An Objectivistic Study of the Innate Behaviour of Animals. Bibliotheca Biotheoretica, 1, 3998.
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived Activities: Their Causation, Biological Significance, Origin and Emancipation during Evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27, 132.
Tinbergen, N. (1959). Comparative Studies of the Behaviour of Gulls (Laridae): A Progress Report. Behaviour, 15, 170.
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On Aims and Methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410433.
Tinbergen, N. (1965). Animal Behavior. New York: Time, Inc.
Tinbergen, N. (1972). Ethology (1969). In The Animal in Its World, Vol. 2. London: Allen and Unwin, pp. 130160.
Tinbergen, N., Broekhuysen, G. J., Feekes, F., Houghton, J. C. W., Kruuk, H. & Szulc, E. (1963). Egg Shell Removal by the Black-headed Gull, Larus ridibundus L.; A Behaviour Component of Camouflage. Behaviour, 19, 74117.
Toates, F. M. (1983). Animal Behaviour – A Systems Approach. Chichester: Wiley.
Toates, F. M. & Jensen, P. (1991). Ethological and Psychological Models of Motivation: Towards a Synthesis. In Meyer, J.-A. & Wilson, S. (Eds.), From Animals to Animats, pp. 194205. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Language Is Not an Instinct. Cognitive Development, 10, 131156.
Tracy, J. L. & Randles, D. (2011). Four Models of Basic Emotions: A Review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3, 397405.
Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-Offspring Conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249264.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental Investment and Sexual Selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 139179.
Trut, L. N. (1999). Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment. American Scientist, 87, 160169.
Trut, L. N., Oskins, I. & Khariamova, A. (2009). Animal Evolution during Domestication: The Domesticated Fox as a Model. BioEssays, 31, 349360.
Underwood, H. (2001). Circadian Organization in Nonmammalian Vertebrates. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 247290.
Van der Kooy, D. & Hogan, J. A. (1978). Priming Effects with Food and Water Reinforcers in Hamsters. Learning and Motivation, 9, 332346.
van der Meer, M. A. A. & Redish, A. D. (2010). Expectancies in Decision Making, Reinforcement Learning, and Ventral Striatum. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 2937.
van der Zee, E. A., et al. (2008). Circadian Time-Place Learning in Mice Depends on Cry Genes. Current Biology, 18, 844848.
van Gils, J. A., Schenk, I. W., Bos, O. & Piersma, T. (2003). Incompletely Informed Shorebirds that Face a Digestive Constraint Maximize Net Energy Gain when Exploiting Patches. American Naturalist, 161, 777793.
van Iersel, J. J. A. (1953). An Analysis of the Parental Behaviour of the Three-spined Stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Behaviour, Suppl. 3, 1159.
van Iersel, J. J. A. & Bol, A. C. A. (1958). Preening of Two Tern Species. A study on Displacement Activities. Behaviour, 13, 188.
van Kampen, H. S. (1996). A Framework for the Study of Filial Imprinting and the Development of Attachment. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 320.
van Kampen, H. S. (1997). Courtship Food-calling in Burmese Red Junglefowl: II. Sexual Conditioning and the Role of the Female. Behaviour, 134, 775787.
van Kampen, H. S. (2015). Violated Expectancies: Cause and Function of Exploration, Fear, and Aggression. Behavioural Processes, 117, 1228.
van Kampen, H. S. & Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Auditory Learning and Filial Imprinting in the Chick. Behaviour, 117, 303339.
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1970). Tool Using in Primates and Other Vertebrates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 3, 195249.
van Liere, D. W. (1992). The Significance of Fowls’ Bathing in Dust. Animal Welfare, 1, 187202.
van Liere, D. W. & Bokma, S. (1987). Short Term Feather Maintenance as a Function of Dustbathing in Laying Hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 18, 197204.
van Rhijn, J. G. (1991). The Ruff. London: Poyser.
van Schaik, C. P. et al. (2003). Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture. Science, 299, 102105.
Verkuil, Y. I. (2010). The Ephemeral Shorebird: Population History of Ruffs. Ph.D. thesis, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.
Verkuil, Y. I. et al. (2012). Losing a Staging Area: Eastward Redistribution of Afro-Eurasian Ruffs Is Associated with Deteriorating Fuelling Conditions along the Western Flyway. Biological Conservation, 149, 5159.
Vestergaard, K. S. (1982). Dust-bathing in the Domestic Fowl: Diurnal Rhythm and Dust Deprivation. Applied Animal Ethology, 8, 487495.
Vestergaard, K. S. & Baranyiova, E. (1996). Pecking and Scratching in the Development of Dust Perception in Young Chicks. Acta Veterinaria Brno, 65, 133142.
Vestergaard, K. S. & Hogan, J. A. (1992). The Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl. III. Effects of Experience on Stimulus Preference. Behaviour, 121, 215230.
Vestergaard, K. S., Damm, B. I., Abbot, U. K. & Bildsøe, M. (1999). Regulation of Dustbathing in Feathered and Featherless Domestic Chicks: The Lorenzian Model Revisited. Animal Behaviour, 58, 10171025.
Vestergaard, K. S., Hogan, J. A. & Kruijt, J. P. (1990). The Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl: I. The Influence of the Rearing Environment on the Organization of Dustbathing. Behaviour, 112, 99116.
Vestergaard, K. S., Kruijt, J. P. & Hogan, J. A. (1993). Feather Pecking and Chronic Fear in Groups of Red Junglefowl: Its Relations to Dustbathing, Rearing Environment and Social Status. Animal Behaviour, 45, 11271140.
von Frisch, K. (1955). The Dancing Bees. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
von Helmholtz, H. (1878). Lecture: The Facts of Perception. Selected Writings of Hermann Helmholtz. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.
von Holst, E. (1935). Über den Prozess der zentralnervösen Koordination. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 236, 149158.
von Holst, E. (1937). Vom Wesen der Ordnung im Zentralnervensystem. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 625631, 641647. [Tr. as: On the Nature of Order in the Central Nervous System. In von Holst, , 1973, pp. 3–32.]
von Holst, E. (1954). Relations between the Central Nervous System and the Peripheral Organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2, 8994.
von Holst, E. (1973). The Selected Papers of Erich von Holst, vol. 1. (Tr. by Martin, R.). London: Methuen.
von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464476. [Tr. as: The Reafference Principle. In von Holst, , 1973, pp. 139–173.]
von Holst, E. & Saint Paul, U. von. (1960). Vom Wirkungsgefüge der Triebe. Naturwissenschaften, 47, 409422. [Tr. as: On the Functional Organisation of Drives. Animal Behaviour, 1963, 11, 1–20; also in von Holst, , 1973, pp. 220–258.]
von Uexküll, J. (1921). Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere. Berlin.
von Uexküll, J. (1934). Streifzüge durch die Umwelten von Tieren und Menschen. Berlin: Springer. [Tr. as: A Stroll through the World of Animals and Men. In Schiller, C. H. (Ed.), Instinctive Behavior. New York: International Universities Press, 1957, pp. 580.]
Vygotsky, L. (1934/1988). Thought and Language. (Tr. by Kozulin, A.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of Development and the Inheritance of Acquired Characters. Nature, 150, pp. 563565.
Waddington, C. H. (1953). Genetic Assimilation of an Acquired Character. Evolution, 7, 118126.
Waddington, C. H. (1959a). Evolutionary Systems – Animal and Human. Nature, 183, 16341638.
Waddington, C. H. (1959b). Canalization of Development and Genetic Assimilation of Acquired Characters. Nature, 183, 16541655.
Waddington, C. H. (1966). Principles of Development and Differentiation. New York: Macmillan. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)
Walker, S. (1983). Animal Thought. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Warden, C. J., Warner, H. C. & Jenkins, T. N. (1931). Animal Motivation: Experimental Studies on the Albino Rat. New York: Columbia University Press.
Wasserman, E. A. (1973). Pavlovian Conditioning with Heat Reinforcement Produces Stimulus-directed Pecking in Chicks. Science, 181, 875877.
Watkins, C. (1969). The Indo-European Origin of English. In Morris, W. (Ed.), The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin, pp. xixxx.
Watson, A. (1973). Moults of Wild Scottish Ptarmigan, Lagopus mutus, in Relation to Sex, Climate and Status. Journal of Zoology (London) 171, 207223.
Watson, J. B. (1908). The behavior of Noddy and Sooty Terns. Papers from the Tortugas Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, 2, 187255.
Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Wauters, A. M., Richard-Yris, M. A. & Talec, N. (2002). Maternal Influences on Feeding and General Activity in Domestic Chicks. Ethology, 108, 529540.
Wayne, R. K. & vonHoldt, B. (2012). Evolutionary Genomics of Dog Domestication. Mammalian Genome, 23, 318.
Webb, W. B. (1998). Sleep. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 327331.
Wehner, R. & Menzel, R. (1990). Do Insects Have Cognitive Maps? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 403414.
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1992). The Organization and Reorganization of Human Speech Perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 377402.
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Development Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
West, M. J. & King, A. P. (1988). Female Visual Display Affects the Development of Males Song in the Cowbird. Nature, 334, 244246.
Whiten, A. et al. (2001). Charting Cultural Variation in Chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 14811516.
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in Chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.
Whitman, C. O. (1911). The Behavior of Pigeons. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute, Publication, 257, pp. 1161.
Wiesel, T. N. (1982). Postnatal Development of the Visual Cortex and the Influence of the Environment. Nature, 299, 583591.
Wilczynski, W. Rand, A. S. & Ryan, M. J. (2001). Evolution of Calls and Auditory Tuning in the Physalaemus pustulosus Species Group. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 58, 137151.
Williams, C. L. (1991). Development of a Sexually Dimorphic Behavior: Hormonal and Neural Controls. In Shair, H. N., Barr, G. A. & Hofer, M. A. (Eds.), Developmental Psychobiology: New Methods and Changing Concepts. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 206222.
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Wilson, D. M. (1966). Central Nervous Mechanisms for the Generation of Rhythmic Behaviour in Arthropods. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 20, 199228.
Wilson, D. S. & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the Theoretical Foundation of Sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 327348.
Wilson, E. O. (1971). The Insect Societies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian Navigation: From Historical to Modern Concepts. Animal Behaviour, 65, 257272.
Wise, R.A. (2004). Dopamine, Learning and Motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 483494.
Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J. & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of Sensorimotor Learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 739751.
Wood-Gush, D. G. M. (1971). The Behaviour of the Domestic Fowl. London: Heinemann.
Wouters, A. G. (2003). Four Notions of Biological Function. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 34, 633668.
Wray, G. A. et al. (2014). Does Evolutionary Theory Need a Rethink? No, All Is Well. Nature, 514, 161164.
Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). Do Animals Think? Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Xie, L. et al. (2013). Sleep Drives Metabolite Clearance from the Adult Brain. Science, 342, 373377.
Young, P. T. (1961). Motivation and Emotion. New York: Wiley.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate Selection – A Selection for a Handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.
Zeder, M. A. (2012). The Domestication of Animals. Journal of Anthropological Research, 68, 161190.
Zeiler, M. D. (1992). On Immediate Function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 417427.
Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Referential Labeling in Diana Monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59, 917927.
Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Survivor Signals: The Biology and Psychology of Animal Alarm Calling. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 40, 277322.