Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T02:12:19.405Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2017

Jerry A. Hogan
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Study of Behavior
Organization, Methods, and Principles
, pp. 327 - 362
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, C. D. & Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental Responding Following Reinforcer Devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 109122.Google Scholar
Adler, N. T & Hogan, J. A. (1963). Classical Conditioning and Punishment of an Instinctive Response in Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 11, 351354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adrian, E. D., Cattell, M. & Hoagland, H. (1931). Sensory Discharges in Single Cutaneous Nerve Fibres. Journal of Physiology, 72, 377391.Google Scholar
Agnvall, B., Katajamaa, R., Altimiras, J. & Jensen, P. (2015). Biology Letters, 11, 20150509.Google Scholar
Akers, K. G. et al. (2014). Hippocampal Neurogenesis Regulates Forgetting during Adulthood and Infancy. Science, 344, 598602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcock, J. S. (1989). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvika, E. & Chittka, L. (2016). Associative Mechanisms Allow for Social Learning and Cultural Transmission of String Pulling in an Insect. PLoS Biology, 14(10).Google ScholarPubMed
Allport, G. (1937). Personality: A Psychological Interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Andrew, R. J. (1966). Precocious Adult Behaviour in the Young Chick. Animal Behaviour, 14, 485500.Google Scholar
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism – How to Carry out an Exaptationist Program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 489553.Google Scholar
Armitage, K. B. (1986). Individuality, Social Behavior, and Reproductive Success in Yellow-Bellied Marmots. Ecology, 67, 11861193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, E. A. (1954). Bird Display and Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S. & Rosenblatt, J. S. (Eds.). (1970). Development and Evolution of Behavior. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Aschoff, J. (1960). Exogenous and Endogenous Components in Circadian Rhythms. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 25, 1128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aschoff, J. (1989). Temporal Organization: Circadian Clocks in Animals and Humans. Animal Behaviour, 37, 881896.Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C. (2013). Individual Differences and Personality, 2nd ed. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C. (2015). Hogan’s Framework for the Study of Behavior as Applied to Personality Psychology. Behavioural Processes, 117, 4851.Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N. (2014). Infant Learning: Historical, Conceptual, and Methodological Challenges. Infancy, 19, 227.Google Scholar
Avarguès-Weber, A., Dyer, A. G., Combe, M. & Giurfa, M. (2012). Simultaneous Mastering of Two Abstract Concepts by the Miniature Brain of Bees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 74817486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baerends, G. P. (1941). Fortpflanzungsverhalten und Orientierung der Grabwespe Ammophila campestris. Tijdschrift voor Entomologie, 84, 81275.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. (1958). Comparative Methods and the Concept of Homology in the Study of Behaviour. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 13, Suppl 1, pp. 401417.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. (1970). A Model of the Functional Organization of Incubation Behaviour. Behaviour, Suppl. 17, pp. 263312.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. (1975). An Evaluation of the Conflict Hypothesis as an Explanatory Principle for the Evolution of Displays. In Baerends et al., pp. 187227.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. (1976). The Functional Organization of Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 24, 726738.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. (1982). The Relative Effectiveness of Different Egg-features in Responses Other Than Egg-retrieval. Behaviour, 82, 225246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baerends, G. P. & Kruijt, J. P. (1973). Stimulus Selection. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 2349.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P. & Drent, R. H. (Eds.). (1982). The Herring Gull and Its Eggs. Behaviour, 82, 1416.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P., Beer, C. & Manning, A. (Eds.). (1975). Function and Evolution in Behaviour. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baerends, G. P., Brouwer, R. & Waterbolk, H. T. (1955). Ethological Studies on Lebistes reticulatus: 1. An Analysis of the Male Courtship Pattern. Behaviour, 8, 249334.Google Scholar
Baerends-van Roon, J. M. & Baerends, G. P. (1979). The Morphogenesis of the Behaviour of the Domestic Cat, with a Special Emphasis on the Development of Prey-catching. Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Afd. Natuurkunde, Tweede Reeks (Proceedings of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences, Section Physics, Second Series), Part 72.Google Scholar
Baker, J. R. (1938). The Evolution of Breeding Seasons. In de Beer, G. R. (Ed.), Evolution: Essays on Aspects of Evolutionary Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 161171.Google Scholar
Balleine, B. W. & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-directed and Habitual Action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 4869.Google Scholar
Balsam, P. D & Silver, R. (1994). Behavioral Change as a Result of Experience: Toward Principles of Learning and Development. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 327357.Google Scholar
Balsam, P. D., Graf, J. S. & Silver, R. (1992). Operant and Pavlovian Contributions to the Ontogeny of Pecking in Ring Doves. Developmental Psychobiology, 25, 389410.Google Scholar
Baptista, L. F. & Gaunt, S. L. L. (1997). Social Interaction and Vocal Development in Birds. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 2340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbour, M. A. & Clark, R. W. (2012). Ground Squirrel Tail-flag Displays Alter Both Predatory Strike and Ambush Site Selection Behaviours of Rattlesnakes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 38273833.Google Scholar
Barlow, G. W. (1968). Ethological Units of Behavior. In Ingle, D. (Ed.), Central Nervous System and Fish Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 217232.Google Scholar
Barnard, C. J. (2004). Animal Behaviour: Mechanism, Development, Function and Evolution. London: Pearson Education Ltd./Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Barnard, C. J. & Sibly, R. M. (1981). Producers and Scroungers: A General Model and Its Application to Captive Flocks of House Sparrows. Animal Behaviour, 29, 543550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. C. & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in Cognition: Framing the Debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628647.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2013). Psychological Construction: The Darwinian Approach to the Science of Emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 379389.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.). (2015). The Psychological Construction of Emotion. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. (1979). How Do Sensitive Periods Arise and What Are They For? Animal Behaviour, 27, 470486.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. (1987). Imprinting as a Process of Competitive Exclusion. In Rauschecker, J. P. & Marler, P. (Eds.), Imprinting and Cortical Plasticity. New York: Wiley, pp. 151168.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. (1999). Foreword. In Bolhuis, & Hogan, , pp. ixxi.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (1987). Developmental Changes in Sensitivity to Experience. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Sensitive Periods in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1934. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. & Laland, K. N. (2013). Tinbergen’s Four Questions: An Appreciation and an Update. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 28, 712718.Google Scholar
Battley, P. F. et al. (2012). Contrasting Extreme Long-distance Migration Patterns in Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa Lapponica. Journal of Avian Biology, 43, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baylis, J. R. (1982). Avian Vocal Mimicry: Its Function and Evolution. In Kroodsma, D. E., Miller, E. H. & Ouellet, H. (Eds.), Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, pp. 5183.Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1942). Analysis of Factors Involved in the Arousal, Maintenance, and Manifestation of Sexual Excitement in Male Animals. Psychosomatic Medicine, 4, 173198.Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1948). Hormones and Behavior. New York: Hoeber.Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1955). The Descent of Instinct. Psychological Review, 62, 401410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beach, F. A. (1956). Characteristics of the Masculine “Sex Drive”. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), The Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 132.Google Scholar
Beer, C. G. (1975). Multiple Functions and Gull Displays. In Baerends, et al., pp. 1654.Google Scholar
Bergerud, A. T. & Mossop, D. H. (1984). The Pair Bond in Ptarmigan. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 62, 21292141.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1990). Comparative Fine Structure of Action: Rules of Form and Sequence in the Grooming Patterns of Six Rodent Species. Behaviour, 113, 2156.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1994). The Development of Action Patterns. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 147180.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation Concepts in Behavioral Neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior, 81, 179209.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2012). From Prediction Error to Incentive Salience: Mesolimbic Computation of Reward Motivation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, pp. 11241143.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., Fentress, J. C. & Parr, H. (1987). Natural Syntax Rules Control Action Sequence of Rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 5968.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing Reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 507513.Google Scholar
Berry, C. S. (1908). An Experimental Study of Imitation in Cats. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Physiology, 18, 112.Google Scholar
Berthold, P. (1973). Relationships between Migratory Restlessness and Migration Distance in Six Sylvia Species. Ibis, 115, 594599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, Brain, and the Nature of Language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 8998.Google Scholar
Bijleveld, A. I., Folmer, E. O. & Piersma, T. (2012). Experimental Evidence for Cryptic Interference among Socially Foraging Shorebirds. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 806814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijleveld, A. I., van Gils, J. A., Jouta, J. & Piersma, T. (2015). Benefits of Foraging in Small Groups: An Experimental Study on Public Information Use in Red Knots Calidris canutus. Behavioural Processes, 117, 7481.Google Scholar
Bischof, H.-J. (1994). Sexual Imprinting as a Two-stage Process. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 8297.Google Scholar
Bissonette, G. B., Bryden, D. W. & Roesch, M. R. (2014). You Won’t Regret Reading This. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 892893.Google Scholar
Blaisdell, A., Sawa, K., Leising, K. J. & Waldmann, M. R. (2006). Causal Reasoning in Rats. Science, 311, 10201022.Google Scholar
Blakemore, C. (1973). Environmental Constraints on Development in the Visual System. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 5173.Google Scholar
Blankenship, A. G. & Feller, M. B. (2010). Mechanisms Underlying Spontaneous Patterned Activity in Developing Neural Circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 1829.Google Scholar
Blass, E. M. (1999). The Ontogeny of Human Infant Face Recognition: Orogustatory, Visual, and Social Influences. In Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early Social Cognition. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 3565.Google Scholar
Blass, E. M. (2015). Energy Conservation in Infants. Behavioural Processes, 117, 3541.Google Scholar
Blass, E. M., Ganchrow, J. R. & Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical Conditioning in Newborn Humans 2–48 Hours of Age. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 125134.Google Scholar
Blass, E. M., Hall, W. G. & Teicher, M. H. (1979). The Ontogeny of Suckling and Ingestive Behaviors. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 8, 243299.Google Scholar
Blest, A. D. (1957). The Function of Eyespot Patterns in the Lepidoptera. Behaviour, 11, 209256.Google Scholar
Bloch, G., Barnes, B. M., Gerkema, M. P. & Helm, B. (2013). Animal Activity around the Clock with No Overt Circadian Rhythms: Patterns, Mechanisms and Adaptive Value. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 30130019.Google Scholar
Blüm, V. & Fiedler, K. (1965). Hormonal Control of Reproductive Behavior in Some Cichlid Fish. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 5, 186196.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T. (2007). The Evolution, Function, and Meaning of Marmot Alarm Communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 371401.Google Scholar
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviourism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bock, W. J. & von Wahlert, G. (1965). Adaptation and the Form-Function Complex. Evolution, 19, 269299.Google Scholar
Bohacek, J. & Mansuy, I. M. (2015). Molecular Insights into Transgenerational Non-genetic Inheritance of Acquired Behaviours. Nature Reviews Genetics, 16, 641652.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of Avian Imprinting: A Review. Biological Reviews, 66, 303345.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. (1994). Neurobiological Analyses of Behavioural Mechanisms in Development. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 1646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. (1996). Development of Perceptual Mechanisms in Birds: Predispositions and Imprinting. In Moss, C. F. & Shettleworth, S. J. (Eds.), Neuroethological Studies of Cognitive and Perceptual Processes. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, pp. 158184. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. (2009). Function and Mechanism in Neuroecology: Looking for Clues. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 163196.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Evolution Cannot Explain How Minds Work. Behavioural Processes, 117, 8291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, J. J. & Eda-Fujiwara, H. (2003). Bird Brains and Songs: Neural Mechanisms of Birdsong Perception and Memory. Animal Biology, 53, 129145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Everaert, M. (Eds.). (2013). Birdsong, Speech, and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2005). The Behavior of Animals. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (in press). The Behavior of Animals, 2nd edn. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Hogan, J. A. (1999). The Development of Animal Behaviour: A Reader. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2009). Can Evolution Explain How Minds Work? Nature, 458, 832833.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N. & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How Could Language Have Evolved? PLoS Biology, 12(8).Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Zijlstra, G. G. O., den Boer-Visser, A. M. & van der Zee, E. A. (2000). Localised Neuronal Activation in the Zebra Finch Brain Is Related to the Strength of Song Learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 22822285.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J.J., Brown, G.R., Richardson, R.C. & Laland, K.N. (2011). Darwin in Mind: New Opportunities for Evolutionary Psychology. PLoS Biology, 9(7).Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of Motivation. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bols, R. J. (1977). Display Reinforcement in the Siamese Fighting Fish, Betta splendens: Aggression Motivation or Curiosity? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 233244.Google Scholar
Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animals. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Borbély, A. A. (1982). A Two-process Model of Sleep. Human Neurobiology, 1, 195204.Google ScholarPubMed
Borbély, A. A., Dijk, D.-J., Achermann, P. & Tobler, I. (2001). Processes Underlying the Regulation of the Sleep-wake Cycle. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 458479.Google Scholar
Borchelt, P. L. (1975). The Organization of Dustbathing Components in Bobwhite Quail (Colinus virginianus). Behaviour, 53, 217237.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1991). Ethological Light on Psychoanalytical Problems. In Bateson, P. (Ed.), The Development and Integration of Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 301313.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Brainard, M. S. & Doupe, A. J. (2000). Auditory Feedback in Learning and Maintenance of Vocal Behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 3140.Google Scholar
Brembs, B. (2009). The Importance of Being Active. Journal of Neurogenetics, 23, 120126.Google Scholar
Bronstein, P. M. (1981). Commitments to Aggression and Nest Sites in Male Betta splendens. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 436449.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1948). Gradients of Approach and Avoidance Responses and Their Relation to Level of Motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 450485.Google Scholar
Brown, M. C., Hopkins, W. G. & Keynes, R. J. (1991). Essentials of Neural Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bruce, R. H. (1935). A Further Study of the Effect of Reward and Drive upon the Maze Performance of Rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 20, 157182.Google Scholar
Buhusi, C. V. & Meck, W. H. (2005). What Makes Us Tick? Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 755765.Google Scholar
Buntin, J. D. (1996). Neural and Hormonal Control of Parental Behavior in Birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 25, 161213.Google Scholar
Burghagen, H. & Ewert, J.-P. (in press). Stimulus Perception. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , 2nd edn.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. (2001). Play: Attributes and Neural Substrates. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13:7 Developmental Psychobiology, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 317356.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. et al. (1998). Adaptations, Exaptations, and Spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533548.Google Scholar
Buss, R. R., Sun, W. & Oppenheim, R. W. (2006). Adaptive Roles of Programmed Cell Death during Nervous System Development. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 135.Google Scholar
Butler, R. A. & Harlow, H. F. (1954). Persistence of Visual Exploration in Monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 257263.Google Scholar
Caldwell., C. A. & Millen, A. E. (2008). Studying Cumulative Cultural Evolution in the Laboratory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 35293539.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange Theory of Emotions: A Critical Examination and an Alternative Theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106124.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Organization for Physiological Homeostasis. Physiological Reviews, 9, 399431.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1932). The Wisdom of the Body. New York: Norton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capranica, R. R. (1965). The Evoked Vocal Response of the Bullfrog: A Study of Communication by Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Caro, T. (2005). Antipredator Defences in Birds and Mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. B. (2008). Evo-Devo and an Expanding Evolutionary Synthesis: A Genetic Theory of Morphological Evolution. Cell, 134, 2536.Google Scholar
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G. & Heinsohn, R. (2013). Animal Personality: What Are Behavioural Ecologists Measuring? Biological Reviews, 88, 465475.Google Scholar
Cartwright, B. A. & Collett, T. S. (1982). How Honey Bees Use Landmarks to Guide Their Return to a Food Source. Nature, 295, 560564.Google Scholar
Cäsar, C., Zuberbühler, K., Young, R. J. & Byrne, R. W. (2013). Titi Monkey Call Sequences Vary with Predator Location and Type. Biology Letters, 9, 20130535.Google Scholar
Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. (2008). Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chase, I. D. (1982). Dynamics of Hierarchy Formation: The Sequential Development of Dominance Relationships. Behaviour, 80, 218240.Google Scholar
Chittka, L. & Niven, J. (2009). Are Bigger Brains Better? Current Biology, 19, R995R1008.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2012). The Science of Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chow, A. & Hogan, J. A. (2005). The Development of Feather Pecking in Burmese Red Junglefowl: The Influence of Early Experience with Exploratory-rich Environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 93, 283294.Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S. (2015). Ways of Thinking: From Crows to Children and Back Again. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 209241.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origins of the Dog: Domestication and Early History. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 720.Google Scholar
Colgan, P. (1989). Animal Motivation. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Collett, M. & Collett, T. S. (2000). How Do Insects Use Path Integration for Their Navigation? Biological Cybernetics, 83, 245259.Google Scholar
Collett, M., Chittka, L. & Collett, T. S. (2013). Spatial Memory in Insect Navigation. Current Biology, 23, R789R800.Google Scholar
Collett, T. S. & Collett, M. (2002). Memory Use in Insect Visual Navigation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 542552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colonnese, M. T., Stallman, E. L. & Berridge, K. C. (1996). Ontogeny of Action Syntax in Altricial and Precocial Rodents: Grooming Sequences of Rat and Guinea Pig Pups. Behaviour, 133, 11651195.Google Scholar
Colwill, R. C. & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning Devaluation of a Reinforcer Affects Instrumental Responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 120132.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. & Schneider, R. (1995). Evolution of Working Dogs. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2147.Google Scholar
Coss, R. G. & Biardi, J. E. (1997). Individual Variation in the Antisnake Behavior of California Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Journal of Mammalogy, 73, 294310.Google Scholar
Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and Aversions as Constituents of Instincts. Biological Bulletin, 34, 91107.Google Scholar
Cramer, C. P. & Blass, E. M. (1983). Mechanisms of Control of Milk Intake in Suckling Rats. American Journal of Physiology, 245, R154R159.Google Scholar
Crews, D. & Groothuis, T. (2009). Tinbergen’s Fourth Question, Ontogeny: Sexual and Individual Differentiation. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 5481.Google Scholar
Cullen, E. (1957). Adaptations in the Kittiwake to Cliff-nesting. Ibis, 99, 275302.Google Scholar
Curio, E., Ernst, U. & Vieth, W. (1978). Cultural Transmission of Enemy Recognition: One Function of Mobbing. Science, 202, 899901.Google Scholar
Cuthill, I. (2009). The Study of Function in Behavioral Ecology. In Bolhuis, J J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 107126.Google Scholar
Czeisler, C. A. & Dijk, D.-J. (2001). Human Circadian Physiology and Sleep-Wake Regulation. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 4: Circadian Clocks. New York: Plenum, pp. 531569.Google Scholar
Daan, S. (2000). Learning and Circadian Behavior. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 15, 296299.Google Scholar
Daan, S., Dijkstra, C. & Tinbergen, J. M. (1990). Family Planning in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): The Ultimate Control of Covariance of Laying Date and Clutch Size. Behaviour, 114, 83116.Google Scholar
Daly, M. (1977). Some Experimental Tests of the Functional Significance of Scent-marking by Gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Journal of Compartive and Physiological Psychology, 91, 10821094.Google Scholar
Daly, M. (1979). Why Don’t Male Mammals Lactate? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 78, 325345.Google Scholar
Daly, M. (2015). On Function, Cause, and Being Jerry Hogan’s Student. Behavioural Processes, 117, 7073.Google Scholar
Daly, M. (2016). Killing the Competition. New Brunswick, NJ: TransactionGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. & Daly, S. (1974). Spatial Distribution of a Leaf-eating Saharan Gerbil (Psammomys obesus) in Relation to Its Food. Mammalia, 38, 591603.Google Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1978). Sex, Evolution, and Behavior. Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution, and Behavior, 2nd edn. Boston, MA: Willard Grant Press.Google Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1999). Human Evolutionary Psychology and Animal Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 57, 509519.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1845). Journal of Researches into the Natural History and Geology of the Countries Visited during the Voyage of H. M. S. Beagle Round the World, 2nd edn. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1868). The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1873). Origin of Certain Instincts. Nature, 7, 417418.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1896). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, 2nd ed. New York: D. Appleton and Company.Google Scholar
Davies, N. B. (2011). Cuckoo Adaptations: Trickery and Tuning. Journal of Zoology (London), 284, 114.Google Scholar
Davies, N. B. Krebs, J. R. & West, S. A. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Davis, F. C. & Reppert, S. M. (2001). Development of Mammalian Rhythms. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 247290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, M. S. (2008). The Science of Animal Suffering. Ethology, 114, 937945.Google Scholar
Dawkins, M. S. (2015). Animal Welfare and the Paradox of Animal Consciousness. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 47, 537.Google Scholar
Dawkins, M. S. & Guilford, T. (1995). An Exaggerated Preference for Simple Neural Network Models of Signal Evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 261, 357360.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976a). Hierarchical Organization: A Candidate Principle for Ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 754.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976b). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal Signals: Information or Manipulation? In Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 282309.Google Scholar
De Bruyn, P. J. N., Tosh, C. A., Bester, M. N. et al. (2011). Sex at Sea: Alternative Mating System in an Extremely Polygynous Mammal. Animal Behaviour, 82, 445451.Google Scholar
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B. & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the Social Processes underlying Human Cumulative Culture. Science, 335, 11141118.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (1992). On the Problems Studied in Ethology, Comparative Psychology, and Animal Behavior. Ethology, 92, 89107.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (1994). On the Utility of the Proximate-Ultimate Distinction in the Study of Animal Behavior. Ethology, 96, 6368.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (1999). The Proximate and the Ultimate: Past, Present, and Future. Behavioural Processes, 46, 189199.Google Scholar
Dezfouli, A. & Balleine, B. W. (2012). Habits, Action Sequences and Reinforcement Learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 10361051.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1994). Instrumental Conditioning. In Mackintosh, , pp. 4579.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational Control of Goal-directed Action. Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 118.Google Scholar
Diedrichsen, J. & Kornysheva, K. (2015). Motor Skill Learning between Selection and Execution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 227233.Google Scholar
Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. (2010). The Memory Function of Sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 115126.Google Scholar
Dijk, D.-J. & von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and Consolidation of Human Sleep, Wakefulness, and Performance by a Symphony of Oscillators. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20, 279290.Google Scholar
Dijk, D.-J. & Archer, S. N. (2009). Light, Sleep, and Circadian Rhythms: Together Again. PLoS Biology, 7(6).Google Scholar
Dijk, D.-J., Duffy, J. F. & Czeisler, C. A. (1992). Circadian and Sleep/Wake Dependent Aspects of Subjective Alertness and Cognitive Performance. Journal of Sleep Research, 1, 112117.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, C., Bult, A. Bijlsma, S., Daan, S., Meijer, T & Zijlstra, M. (1990). Brood Size Manipulations in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): Effects on Offspring and Parent Survival. Journal of Animal Ecology, 59, 269285.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, N, J., Both, C., Drent, P. J. & Tinbergen, J. M. (2004). Fitness Consequences of Avian Personalities in a Fluctuating Environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, 847852.Google Scholar
Dingle, H. & Drake, V. A. (2007). What Is Migration? BioScience, 57, 113121.Google Scholar
Dollard, J. & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and Psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Domjan, M. & Holloway, K. S. (1998). Sexual Learning. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 602613.Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Cusato, B. & Krause, M. (2004). Learning with Arbitrary versus Ecological Conditioned Stimuli: Evidence from Sexual Conditioning. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 11, 232246.Google Scholar
Doty, R. W. (1976). The Concept of Neural Centers. In Fentress, J. C. (Ed.), Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 251265.Google Scholar
Doupe, A. J. & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and Human Speech: Common Themes and Mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567631.Google Scholar
Drachman, D. B. & Sokoloff, L. (1966). The role of Movement in Embryonic Joint Development. Developmental Biology, 14, 401420.Google Scholar
Duncan, I. J. H., Widowski, T. M., Malleau, A. E., Lindberg, A. C. & Petherick, J. C. (1988). External Factors and Causation of Dustbathing in Domestic Hens. Behavioural Processes, 43, 219228.Google Scholar
Durkin, J. M. & Aton, S. J. (2016). Sleep-dependent Potentiation in the Visual System Is at Odds with the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis. Sleep, 39, 155159.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1972). Similarities and Differences between Cultures in Expressive Movements. In Hinde, R. A. (Ed.), Non-verbal Communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 297311.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Miller, J. L. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1987). On Infant Speech Perception and the Acquisition of Language. In Harnad, S. (Ed.), Categorical Perception. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 161195.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, P., Jusczyk, P. & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech Perception in Infants. Science, 171, 303306.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the Human Face, 2nd edn. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J., Eder, A. B. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2013). Avoidance Motivation and Emotion: Convergency and Divergence. Emotion Review, 5, 308311.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1967). Migratory Orientation in the Indigo Bunting, Passerina cyanea. Auk, 84, 309342, 463489.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1969). Bird Migration: Influence of Physiological State upon Celestial Orientation. Science, 165, 716718.Google Scholar
Enquist, M. & Arak, A. (1993). Selection of Exaggerated Male Traits by Female Aesthetic Senses. Nature, 361, 446448.Google Scholar
Enquist, M. & Ghirlanda, S. (2005). Neural Networks and Animal Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Enquist, M. & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Evolution of Social Learning Does Not Explain the Origin of Human Cumulative Culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 246, 129135,Google Scholar
Enquist, M. & Leimar, O. (1983). Evolution of Fighting Behaviour: Decision Rules and Assessment of Relative Strength. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 102, 387410.Google Scholar
Enquist, M. Leimar, O., Ljungberg, T., Mallner, Y. & Segerdahl, N. (1990). A Test of the Sequential Game: Fighting in the Cichlid Fish Nannacara anomala. Animal Behaviour, 40, 114.Google Scholar
Etienne, A. S. & Jeffery, K. J. (2004). Path Integration in Mammals. Hippocampus, 14, 180192.Google Scholar
Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R. & Séguinot, V. (1996). Path Integration in Mammals and Its Interaction with Visual Landmarks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 201209.Google Scholar
Evans, R. M. (1968). Early Aggressive Responses in Domestic Chicks. Animal Behaviour, 16, 2428.Google Scholar
Ewert, J.-P. (1997). Neural Correlates of Key Stimulus and Releasing Mechanism. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 332339.Google Scholar
Ewing, A. W. & Miyan, J. A. (1986). Sexual Selection, Sexual Isolation and the Evolution of Song in the Drosophila repleta Group of Species. Animal Behaviour, 34, 421429.Google Scholar
Falk, J. L. (1961). Production of Polydipsia in Normal Rats by an Intermittent Food Schedule. Science, 133, 195196.Google Scholar
Falk, J. L. (1966). Schedule-induced Polydipsia as a Function of Fixed Interval Length. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 3739.Google Scholar
Falk, J. L. (1971). The Nature and Determinants of Adjunctive Behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 577588.Google Scholar
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2014). Sexual Bias in Probe Tool Manufacture and Use by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 108, 117122.Google Scholar
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2016). The Manifold Use of Pounding Stone Tools by Wild Capuchin Monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil. Behaviour, 153, 421442.Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural Organization of the Defensive Behavior System Responsible for Fear. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 429438.Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. & De Oca, B. M. (1988). Defensive Behaviors. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 653665.Google Scholar
Fantz, R. L. (1957). Form Preferences in Newly Hatched Chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 422430.Google Scholar
Farris, H. E. (1967). Classical Conditioning of Courting Behavior in the Japanese Quail (Coturnix c. japonica). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 213217.Google Scholar
Fawcett, T. W., Hamblin, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2013). Exposing the Behavioral Gambit: The Evolution of Learning and Decision Rules. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 211.Google Scholar
Feekes, F. (1972). “Irrelevant” Ground Pecking in Agonistic Situations in Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour, 43, 186326.Google Scholar
Fehér, O. & Tchernichovski, O. (2013). Vocal Culture in Songbirds: An Experimental Approach to Cultural Evolution. In Bolhuis, & Everaert, , pp. 143156.Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. & Gadbois, S. (2001). The Development of Action Sequences. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13: Developmental Psychobiology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 393431.Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. & Stilwell, F. P. (1973). Grammar of a Movement Sequence in Inbred Mice. Nature, 224, 5253.Google Scholar
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R. & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and Imitation of Facial Expressions by Neonates. Science, 218, 179181.Google Scholar
Finn, A. S. et al. (2016). Developmental Dissociation between the Maturation of Procedural Memory and Declarative Memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 212220.Google Scholar
Fleming, A. S. & Blass, E. M. (1994). Psychobiology of the Early Mother-Young Relationship. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 212241.Google Scholar
Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D., Krieger, H. & Wong, P. Y. (1997). Hormonal and Experiential Correlates of Maternal Responsiveness during Pregnancy and the Puerperium in Human Mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 145158.Google Scholar
Fraenkel, G. S. & Gunn, D. L. (1940/1961). The Orientation of Animals. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Francis, R. C. (1990). Causes, Proximate and Ultimate. Biology and Philosophy, 5, 401415.Google Scholar
Frankland, P. W., Kōhler, S. & Josselyn, S. A. (2013). Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Forgetting. Trends in Neuroscience, 36, 497503.Google Scholar
Freedman, A. H. et al. (2014). Genome Sequencing Highlights the Dynamic Early History of Dogs. PLoS Genetics, 10(1).Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1905). Three Contributions to the Theory of Sexuality. London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1915). Instincts and Their Vicissitudes. London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1940/1949). An Outline of Psycho-analysis. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Frishkopf, L. S. & Goldstein, M. H. Jr. (1963). Responses to Acoustic Stimuli from Single Units in the Eighth Nerve of the Bullfrog. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 12191228.Google Scholar
Fugazza, C, Pogány, Á. & Miklósi, Á. (2016). Recall of Others’ Actions after Incidental Encoding Reveals Episodic-like Memory in Dogs. Current Biology, 26, 15.Google Scholar
Gadbois, S., Sievert, O., Reeve, C., Harrington, F. H. & Fentress, J. C. (2015). Revisiting the Concept of Behavior Patterns in Animal Behavior with an Example from Food-caching Sequences in Wolves (Canis lupus), Coyotes (Canis latrans), and Red Foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Behavioural Processes, 110, 314.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. (1988). Imitation in Animals: History, Definition and Interpretation of Data from the Psychological Laboratory. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (Eds.), Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 328.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. (1996). Social Enhancement of Food Preferences in Norway Rats. In Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (Eds.), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. New York: Academic Press, pp. 4964.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. (2013). Imitation and Local Enhancement: Detrimental Effects of Consensus Definitions on Analyses of Social Learning in Animals. Behavioural Processes, 100, 123130.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1973). Self-stimulation: The Neurophysiology of Reward and Motivation. In Deutsch, J. A. (Ed.), The Physiological Basis of Memory. New York: Academic Press, pp. 175267.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1980). The Organization of Action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1981). Précis of Gallistel’s The Organization of Action: A New Synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 609650.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1990a). Representations in Animal Cognition: An Introduction. Cognition, 37, 122.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1990b). The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. & Matzel, L. D. (2013). The Neuroscience of Learning: Beyond the Hebbian Synapse. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 169200.Google Scholar
Gendron, M. & Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the Past: A Century of Ideas about Emotion in Psychology. Emotion Review, 1, 316339.Google Scholar
Gerhardt, H. C. (2001). Acoustic Communication in Two Groups of Closely Related Treefrogs. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 30, 99167.Google Scholar
Ghirlanda, S. & Enquist, M. (2003). A Century of Generalization. Animal Behaviour, 66, 1536.Google Scholar
Ghirlanda, S., Jansson, L. & Enquist, M. (2002). Chickens Prefer Beautiful Humans. Human Nature, 13, 383389.Google Scholar
Gil, S. A. & Bierema, A. M. K. (2013). On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling. Ethology, 119, 449461.Google Scholar
Gilbert, S. F. (2012). Ecological Developmental Biology: Environmental Signals for Normal Animal Development. Evolution and Development, 14, 2028.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L.-A. (2005). The Function of Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 199225.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L.-A. & Dubois, F. (2008). Social Foraging and the Study of Exploitative Behaviour. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 59104.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L.-A. & Kramer, D. L. (1982). The Marginal Value Theorem: A Quantitative Test Using Load Size Variation in a Central Place Forager the Eastern Chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Animal Behaviour, 30, 10361042.Google Scholar
Glickman, S. E. (1973). Responses and Reinforcement. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 207241.Google Scholar
Glickman, S. E. & Schiff, B. B. (1967). A Biological Theory of Reinforcement. Psychological Review, 74, 81109.Google Scholar
Glimcher, P. W. (2011). Understanding Dopamine and Reinforcement Learning: The Dopamine Reward Prediction Error Hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1564715654.Google Scholar
Gobes, M. H., Fritz, J. B. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Neural Mechanisms of Auditory Learning in Songbirds and Mammals. In Bolhuis, & Everaert, , pp. 295315.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). The Resilience of Language in Humans. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 293311.Google Scholar
Gosling, S. D. (2001). From Mice to Men: What Can We Learn about Personality from Animal Research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 4586.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1978). Development of Species Identification in Ducklings: IV. Change in Species-specific Perception Caused by Auditory Deprivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 375387.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1980). Development of Species Identification in Ducklings: VI. Specific Embryonic Experience Required to Maintain Species-typical Perception in Peking Ducklings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94, 579587. (Reprinted in Bolhuis & Hogan, 1999.)Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual Development and Evolution: The Genesis of Novel Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing Nature-Nurture: Prenatal Roots of Instinctive Behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The Spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: A Critique of the Adaptationist Programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 205, 581598.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. S. (1988). Exaptation-A Missing Term in the Science of Form. Paleobiology, 8, 415.Google Scholar
Graf, J. S., Balsam, P. D. & Silver, R. (1985). Associative Factors and the Development of Pecking in the Ring Dove. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 447460.Google Scholar
Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. (2006). Evolution of Character Displacement in Darwin’s Finches. Science, 313, 224226.Google Scholar
Gray, R. D., Atkinson, Q. D. & Greenhill, S. J. (2011). Language Evolution and Human History: What a Difference a Day Makes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 10901100.Google Scholar
Graybiel, A. M. & Smith, K. S. (2014). Good Habits, Bad Habits. Scientific American, 310, 3843.Google Scholar
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E. & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and Brain Development. Child Development, 58, 539559.Google Scholar
Griesemer, J. (2000). Development, Culture, and the Units of Inheritance. Philosophy of Science, 67 (Suppl.), S348S368.Google Scholar
Groothuis, T. G. G. (1992). The Influence of Social Experience on the Development and Fixation of the Form of Displays in the Black-headed Gull. Animal Behaviour, 43, 114.Google Scholar
Groothuis, T. G. G. (1994). The Ontogeny of Social Displays: Interplay between Motor Development, Development of Motivational Systems and Social Experience. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 183211.Google Scholar
Groothuis, T. G. G. & Meeuwissen, G. (1992). The Influence of Testosterone on the Development and Fixation of the Form of Displays in Two Age Classes of Young Black-headed Gulls. Animal Behaviour, 43, 189208.Google Scholar
Gross, C. T. & Canteras, N S. (2012). The Many Paths to Fear. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 651658.Google Scholar
Grossman, S. P. (1967). A Textbook of Physiological Psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gwinner, E. (1986). Circannual Rhythms in the Control of Avian Migrations. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 16, 191228.Google Scholar
Gwinner, E. (1996). Circadian and Circannual Programmes in Avian Migration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 3948.Google Scholar
Gwinner, E. & Brandstätter, R. (2001). Complex Bird Clocks. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 356, 18011810.Google Scholar
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S. Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a Spatial Map in the Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 436, 801806.Google Scholar
Hale, C. & Green, L. (1979). Effect of Initial Pecking Consequences on Subsequent Pecking in Young Chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 730735.Google Scholar
Hall, C. S. (1934). Emotional Behavior in the Rat: 1. Defecation and Urination as Measures of Individual Differences in Emotionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 385403.Google Scholar
Hall, W. G. & Browde, J. A. (1986). The Ontogeny of Independent Ingestion in Mice: Or, Why Won’t Infant Mice Feed? Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 211222.Google Scholar
Hall, W. G. & Williams, C. L. (1983). Suckling Isn’t Feeding, or Is It? A Search for Developmental Continuities. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 13, 219254.Google Scholar
Hamburger, V. (1973). Anatomical and Physiological Basis of Embryonic Motility in Birds and Mammals. In Gottlieb, G. (Ed.), Behavioral Embryology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 5276.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The Genetical Evolution of Social Behaviour. I & II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1971). Geometry for the Selfish Herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 31, 295311.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and Satiation of Response in Intrinsically Motivated Complex Puzzle Performance by Monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 289294.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. & Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social Deprivation in Monkeys. Scientific American, Nov. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. & Harlow, M. K. (1965). The Affectional Systems. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F. & Stollnitz, F. (Eds.), Behavior of Nonhuman Primates, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. & Suomi, S. J. (1971). Social Recovery by Isolation-reared Monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 68, 15341538.Google Scholar
Hart, B. L. (1995). Analysing Breed and Gender Differences in Behaviour. In Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 6577.Google Scholar
Haslam, M., Luncz, L. V. & Falótico, T. (2016). Pre-Columbian Monkey Tools. Current Biology, 26, R515R522.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, M.D., Yang, C., Berwick, R. C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M. J., Watumull, J., Chomsky, N. & Lewontin, R. C. (2014). The Mystery of Language Evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, May, article 401.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1946). On the Nature of Fear. Psychological Review, 53, 259276.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Heiligenberg, W. (1974). Processes Governing Behavioral States of Readiness. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 173200.Google Scholar
Heinroth, O. (1911). Beiträge zur Biologie: namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Verhandlungen des 5. Internationalen Ornitologen-Kongresses in Berlin, 1910, pp. 389702.Google Scholar
Henning, F. & Meyer, A. (2014). The Evolutionary Genomics of Cichlid Fishes: Explosive Speciation and Adaptation in the Postgenomic Era. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 15, 417441.Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1977). The Evolution of Behaviorism. American Psychologist, 32, 593603.Google Scholar
Hess, E. H. (1956). Natural Preferences of Chicks and Ducklings for Objects of Different Colors. Psychological Reports, 2, 477483.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012). New Thinking: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 20912096.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social Learning in Animals: Categories and Mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207231.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012). What’s Social about Social Learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 109113.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., Lazer, D. et al. (2015). Exploration versus Exploitation in Space, Mind, and Society. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 4654.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1959). Unitary Drives. Animal Behaviour, 7, 130141.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1960). Energy Models of Motivation. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 14, 199213.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behavior, 2nd edn. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1975). The Concept of Function. In Baerends, et al., pp. 215.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1977). Mother-Infant Separation and the Nature of Inter-individual Relationships: Experiments with Rhesus Monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 196, 2950. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. G. (Eds.). (1973). Constraints on Learning. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hodos, W. & Campbell, C. B. G. (1969). Scala Naturae: Why There Is No Theory in Comparative Psychology. Psychological Review, 76, 337350.Google Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early Social Relationships: A Psychobiologist’s View. Child Development, 58, 633647.Google Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1996). On the Nature and Consequences of Early Loss. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 570581.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1961). Motivational Aspects of Instinctive Behavior in Betta splendens. Ph.D. thesis, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1964). Operant Control of Preening in Pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 351354.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1965). An Experimental Study of Conflict and Fear: An Analysis of Behavior of Young Chicks to a Mealworm. Part I: The Behavior of Chicks Which Do Not Eat the Mealworm. Behaviour, 25, 4597.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1966). An Experimental Study of Conflict and Fear: An Analysis of Behavior of Young Chicks to a Mealworm. Part II: The Behavior of Chicks Which Eat the Mealworm. Behaviour, 27, 273289.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1971). The Development of a Hunger System in Young Chicks. Behaviour, 39, 128201.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1973). How Young Chicks Learn to Recognize Food. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 119139.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1974). Responses in Pavlovian Conditioning Studies. Science, 186, 156157.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1977). The Ontogeny of Food Preferences in Chicks and Other Animals. In Barker, L. M., Best, M. & Domjan, M. (Eds.), Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Waco, TX: Baylor University Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1978). An Eccentric View of Development: A Review of The Dynamics of Behavior Development by Zing-Yang Kuo. Contemporary Psychology, 23, 690691.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1980). Homeostasis and Behaviour. In Toates, F. M. & Halliday, T. R. (Eds.), Analysis of Motivational Processes. London: Academic Press, pp. 321.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1981). Hierarchy and Behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 625.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1984a). Cause, Function, and the Analysis of Behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 6571.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1984b). Pecking and Feeding in Chicks. Learning and Motivation, 15, 360376.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1988). Cause and Function in the Development of Behavior Systems. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 9. New York: Plenum, pp. 63105.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1989). The Interaction of Incubation and Feeding in Broody Junglefowl Hens. Animal Behaviour, 38, 121128.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1994a). The Concept of Cause in the Study of Behavior. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 315.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1994b). Development of Behavior Systems. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 242264.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1994c). Structure and Development of Behavior Systems. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 439450.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1997). Energy Models of Motivation: A Reconsideration. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 53, 89105.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1998). Motivation. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook, pp. 164175. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (2001). Development of Behavior Systems. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 13: Developmental Psychobiology, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 229279.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (2005). Motivation. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 4170.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (2015). A Framework for the Study of Behavior. Behavioural Processes, 117, 105113.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2009). Tinbergen’s Four Questions and Contemporary Behavioral Biology. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2534.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & Bolhuis, J. J. (Eds.). (1994). Causal Mechanisms of Behavioural Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & Bols, R. J. (1980). Priming of Aggressive Motivation in Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 28, 135142.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & Roper, T. J. (1978). A Comparison of the Properties of Different Reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 8, 155255.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & van Boxel, F. (1993). Causal Factors Controlling Dustbathing in Burmese Red Junglefowl: Some Results and a Model. Animal Behaviour, 46, 627635.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A., Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Panning, L. & Moffatt, C. A. (1998). Causal Factors Controlling the Brooding Cycle of Broody Junglefowl Hens with Chicks. Behaviour, 135, 957980.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A., Honrado, G. I. & Vestergaard, K. S. (1991). Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus): II. Internal Factors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 195, 269273.Google Scholar
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. (1966). Social Behavior of the Ruff, Philomachus pugnax (L.). Ardea, 54, 109229.Google Scholar
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. (1992). Female Choice and the Evolution of Mating Strategies in the Ruff Philomachus pugnax (L.). Ardea, 80, 395403.Google Scholar
Hogan-Warburg, A. J. & Hogan, J. A. (1981). Feeding Strategies in the Development of Food Recognition in Young Chicks. Animal Behaviour, 29, 143154.Google Scholar
Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Hogan, J. A. & Ashton, M. C. (1995). Locomotion and Grooming in Crickets: Competition or Time Sharing? Animal Behaviour, 49, 531533.Google Scholar
Hogan-Warburg, A. J., Panning, L. & Hogan, J. A. (1993). Analysis of the Brooding Cycle of Broody Junglefowl Hens with Chicks. Behaviour, 125, 2137.Google Scholar
Hollis, K. L. (1984). The Biological Function of Pavlovian Conditioning: The Best Defence Is a Good Offence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 413425.Google Scholar
Hollis, K. L., ten Cate, C. & Bateson, P. (1991). Stimulus Representation: A Subprocess of Imprinting and Conditioning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 307317.Google Scholar
Hopkins, B. & Butterworth, G. (1990). Concepts of Causality in Explanations of Development. In Butterworth, G. & Bryant, P. (Eds.), Causes of Development, London: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 333.Google Scholar
Horn, G. (1985). Memory, Imprinting, and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, G. M. (Ed.). (1964). Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology: Homeostasis and Feedback Mechanisms, vol.18. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1933). Differential Habituation to Internal Stimuli in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 255273.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Hultsch, H. (1993). Tracing the Memory Mechanisms in the Song Acquisition of Nightingales. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 155171.Google Scholar
Huxley, J. S. (1923). Courtship Activities in the Red-throated Diver (Colymbus stellatus Pontopp.); Together with a Discussion of the Evolution of Courtship in Birds. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society of London, 35, 253292.Google Scholar
Huxley, J. S. (1942). Evolution: The Modern Synthesis. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1893). Collected Essays, Vol. 1, Method and Results. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ibbotson, P. & Tomasello, M. (2016). Language in a New Key. Scientific American, 315(5), 7075.Google Scholar
Immelmann, K. (1972). The Influence of Early Experience upon the Development of Social Behaviour in Estrildine Finches. In Voous, K. H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the XV International Ornithological Congress, Den Haag, 1970. Leiden: Brill, pp. 316338.Google Scholar
Immelmann, K., Lassek, R., Pröve, R. & Bischof, H.-J. (1991). Influence of Adult Courtship Experience on the Development of Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males. Animal Behaviour, 42, 8389.Google Scholar
Ince, S. A., Slater, P. J. B. & Weismann, C. (1980). Changes with Time in the Songs of a Population of Chaffinches. Condor, 82, 285290.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2004). Beyond Biology and Culture. The Meaning of Evolution in a Relational World. Social Anthropology, 12, 209221.Google Scholar
Insel, N. & Frankland, P. W. (2015). Mechanism, Function, and Computation in Neural Systems. Behavioural Processes, 117, 411.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. J. (2014). Evolution in Four Dimensions, revised edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, R. R. & Cross, F. R. (2011). Spider Cognition. Advances in Insect Physiology, 41, 115174.Google Scholar
Jackson, R. R. & Wilcox, R. S. (1998). Spider-eating Spiders. American Scientist, 86, 350357.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology, vol. 2. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Japyassú, H. F. & Malange, J. (2014). Plasticity, Stereotypy, Intro-individual Variability and Personality: Handle with Care. Behavioural Processes, 109, 4047.Google Scholar
Jarvis, E. D. & Nottebohm, F. (1997). Motor-driven Gene Expression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 40974102.Google Scholar
Johanson, I. B. & Terry, L. M. (1988). Learning in Infancy: A Mechanism of Behavioral Change in Development. In Blass, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 9. New York: Plenum, pp. 245281.Google Scholar
Johnson, A. & Redish, A. D. (2007). Neural Ensembles in CA3 Transiently Encode Paths Forward of the Animal at a Decision Point. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1217612189.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. P. (2000). The Evolution of Courtship Display Repertoire Size in the Dabbling Ducks (Anatini). Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 13, 634644.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2000). Predispositions in Perceptual and Cognitive Development. In Bolhuis, J. J. (Ed.), Brain, Perception, Memory: Advances in Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 6984.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Bolhuis, J. J. & Horn, G. (1985). Interaction between Acquired Preferences and Developing Predispositions during Imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 33, 10001006.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Davies, D. C. & Horn, G. (1989). A Critical Period for the Development of a Predisposition in the Chick. Animal Behaviour, 37, 10441046.Google Scholar
Johnson, R. N. & Johnson, L. D. (1973). Intra- and Interspecific Social and Aggressive Behaviour in the Siamese Fighting Fish, Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 21, 665672.Google Scholar
Josselyn, S. A., Köhler, S. & Frankland, P. W. (2015). Finding the Engram. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 521534.Google Scholar
Josselyn, S. A., Kōhler, S. & Frankland, P. W. (2017). Heroes of the Engram. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 46474657.Google Scholar
Jukema, J. & Piersma, T. (2006). Permanent Female Mimics in a Lekking Shorebird. Biology Letters, 2, 161164.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). Finding and Remembering Words: Some Beginnings by English-learning Infants. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 170174.Google Scholar
Kacelnik, A. (1984). Central-place Foraging in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch Residence Time. Journal of Animal Ecology, 53, 283299.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. Canada: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development Itself Is the Key to Understanding Developmental Disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389398.Google Scholar
Kellman, P. J. (2002). Perceptual Learning. In Gallistel, C. R. (Ed.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, vol. 3: Learning, Motivation, and Emotion. New York: Wiley, pp. 259299.Google Scholar
Kenny, J. T., Stoloff, M. L., Bruno, J. P. & Blass, E. M. (1979). The Ontogeny of Preferences for Nutritive over Nonnutritive Suckling in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93,752759.Google Scholar
King, A. P. & West, M. J. (1977). Species Identification in the North American Cowbird: Appropriate Responses to Abnormal Song. Science, 195, 10021004.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, K. & Hall, G. (2005). Learning and Memory. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 146169.Google Scholar
Kish, G. B. (1966). Studies of Sensory Reinforcement. In Honig, W. K. (Ed.), Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application. New York: Appleton, pp. 109159.Google Scholar
Kissileff, H. R. (1969). Food-associated drinking in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 284300.Google Scholar
Konishi, M. (1965). The Role of Auditory Feedback in the Control of Vocalizations in the White-crowned Sparrow. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 22, 770783.Google Scholar
Kortlandt, A. (1940). Wechselwirkung zwischen Instinkten. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 4, 443520.Google Scholar
Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A Psychobiological Theory of Attachment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 493511.Google Scholar
Kramer, D. L. & Bonenfant, M. (1997). Direction of Predator Approach and the Decision to Flee to a Refuge. Animal Behaviour, 54, 289295.Google Scholar
Kramer, G. (1952). Experiments on bird orientation. Ibis, 94, 265285.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, J. T., Webber, M. I. & Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal Prey-selection by the Great Tit (Parus major). Animal Behaviour, 25, 3038.Google Scholar
Kristiansen., M. & Ham, J. (2014). Programmed Cell Death during Neuronal Development: The Sympathetic Neuron Model. Cell Death and Differentiation, 21, 10251035.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. (1962a). On the Evolutionary Derivation of Wing Display in Burmese Red Junglefowl and Other Gallinaceous Birds. Symposia of the Zoological Society, London, 8, 2535.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. (1962b). Imprinting in Relation to Drive Interactions in Burmese Red Junglefowl. Symposia of the Zoological Society, London, 8, 219226.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. (1964). Ontogeny of Social Behaviour in Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour (Suppl. 9).Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. (1985). On the Development of Social Attachments in Birds. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 35, 4562.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. & Hogan, J. A. (1967). Social Behavior on the Lek in Black Grouse, Lyrurus tetrix tetrix (L.). Ardea, 55, 203240.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. & Meeuwissen, G. B. (1991). Sexual Preferences of Male Zebra Finches: Effects of Early Adult Experience. Animal Behaviour, 42, 91102.Google Scholar
Kruijt, J. P. & Meeuwissen, G. B. (1993). Consolidation and Modification of Sexual Preferences in Adult Male Zebra Finches. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 6879.Google Scholar
Kruuk, H. (1964). Predators and Anti-predator Behaviour of the Black-headed Gull, Larus ridibundus. Behaviour (Suppl. 11), 1129.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (1994). Learning and Representation in Speech and Language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 812822.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early Language Acquisition: Cracking the Speech Code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831843.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2015). How Babies Learn Language. Scientific American, 313(5).Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, R., Stevens, K. N. & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic Experience Alters Phonetic Perception in Infants by 6 Months of Age. Science, 255, 606608.Google Scholar
Kühn, A. (1919). Die Orientierung der Tiere im Raum. Jena.Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1921). Giving up Instincts in Psychology. Journal of Philosophy, 18, 645664.Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1967). The Dynamics of Behavioral Development. New York: Random House. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Küpper, C., Stocks, M., Risse, J. E. et al. (2016). A Supergene Determines Highly Divergent Male Reproductive Morphs in the Ruff. Nature Genetics, 48, 7983.Google Scholar
Lack, D. (1954). The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lack., D. (1947). Darwin’s Finches. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2015). On Evolutionary Causes and Evolutionary Processes. Behavioural Processes, 117, 97104.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Galef, B. G. Jr (Eds.). (2009). The Question of Animal Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. et al. (2014). Does Evolutionary Theory Need a Rethink? Yes, Urgently. Nature, 514, 161164.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W. & Uller, T. (2011). Cause and Effect in Biology Revisited: Is Mayr’s Proximate-Ultimate Dichotomy Still Useful? Science, 334, 15121516.Google Scholar
Land, M. F. (2014). Do We Have an Internal Model of the Outside World? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 (doi: 20130045).Google Scholar
Langtimm, C. A. & Dewsbury, D. A. (1991). Phylogeny and Evolution of Rodent Copulatory Behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 41, 217225.Google Scholar
Lank, D. B., Smith, C. M., Hanotte, O., Burke, T. & Cooke, F. (1995). Genetic Polymorphism for Alternative Mating Behaviour in Lekking Male Ruff Philomachus pugnax. Nature, 378, 5962.Google Scholar
Larsen, B. H., Hogan, J. A. & Vestergaard, K. S. (2000). Development of Dustbathing Behavior Sequences in the Domestic Fowl: The Significance of Functional Experience. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 512.Google Scholar
Larson, G. & Fuller, D. Q. (2014). The Evolution of Animal Domestication. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 45, 115136.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1938). Experimental Analysis of Instinctive Behavior. Psychological Review, 45, 445471.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1950). In Search of the Engram. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 454482.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The Problem of Serial Order in Behavior. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. New York: Wiley, pp. 112136.Google Scholar
Leadbeater, E. (2015). What Evolves in the Evolution of Social Learning. Journal of Zoology, 295, 411.Google Scholar
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2007). Social Learning in Insects – From Miniature Brains to Consensus Building. Current Biology, 17, R703R713.Google Scholar
LeBeouf, B. J. (1974). Male-male Competition and Reproductive Success in Elephant Seals. American Zoologist, 14, 163176.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the Emotional Brain. Neuron, 73, 653676.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L. (2015). Should Neuroecologists Separate Tinbergen’s Four Questions? Behavioural Processes, 117, 9296.Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A Critique of Konrad Lorenz’s Theory of Instinctive Behaviour. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 337363.Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1955). The Physiological Basis of Parental Feeding Behaviour in the Ring Dove (Streptopelia risoria). Behaviour, 7, 241286.Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1965). Interaction between Internal and External Environments in the Regulation of the Reproductive Cycle of the Ring Dove. In Beach, F. A. (Ed.), Sex and Behavior. New York: Wiley, pp. 355380.Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and Conceptual Issues in the Nature-Nurture Problem. In Aronson, et al., pp. 1752. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. H. (1959). What the Frog’s Eye Tells the Frog’s Brain. Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers, 47, 19401951.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. & Gray, R. D. (2012). Tools from Evolutionary Biology Shed New Light on the Diversification of Languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 167173.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The Units of Selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 118.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2016). The Evolution of Language and Thought. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 94, 127146.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1993). The Child’s Path to Spoken Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1994). The Biological Building Blocks of Spoken Language. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 300324.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. & Snow, C. (1997). Social Influences on Vocal Learning in Human and Nonhuman Primates. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 274292.Google Scholar
Loeb, J. (1918). Forced Movements, Tropisms and Animal Conduct. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Logan, C. A. (1983). Biological Diversity in Avian Vocal Learning. In Zeiler, M. D. & Harzem, P. (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior, vol. 3: Biological Factors in Learning. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 137213, 289413. [Tr. as: Companions as Factors in the Bird’s Environment. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 101–258 – excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 289300, 307318, 324331. [Tr. as: The Establishment of the Instinct Concept. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 259–315.]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1941). Vergleichende Bewegungstudien an Anatinen. Journal für Ornithologie, 89, Sonderheft. (Tr. by Martin, R. D. as: Comparative Studies of the Motor Patterns of Anatinae. In Lorenz, (1971). Studies in Animal and Human Behaviour, vol. 2. London: Methuen, pp. 14114.)Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1950). The Comparative Method in Studying Innate Behaviour Patterns. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 221268.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1956). Plays and Vacuum Activities. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l‘homme. Paris: Masson, pp. 633637.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1961). Phylogenetische Anpassung und adaptive Modifikation des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 18, 139187.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and Modification of Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966). On Aggression. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1970). Studies in Animal and Human Behaviour, vol. 1. (Tr. by Martin, R.). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1981). The Foundations of Ethology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. & Tinbergen, N. (1939). Taxis und Instinkthandlung in der Eirollbewegung der Graugans. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 2, 129. [Tr. as: Taxis and Instinctive Behavior Pattern in Egg-rolling by the Greylag Goose. In Lorenz, , 1970, pp. 316–350.]Google Scholar
Losos, J. B. (2010). Adaptive Radiation, Ecological Opportunity, and Evolutionary Determinism. American Naturalist, 175, 623639.Google Scholar
Lotem, A. (1993). Learning to Recognize Nestlings Is Maladaptive for Cuckoo Cuculus canorus Hosts. Nature, 362, 743745.Google Scholar
Lovic, V. & Fleming, A. S. (2015). Propagation of Maternal Behavior across Generations Is Associated with Changes in Non-maternal Cognitive and Behavioral Processes. Behavioural Processes, 117, 4247.Google Scholar
Lowe, P. R. (1936). The Finches of the Galapagos in Relation to Darwin’s Conception of Species. Ibis, 78, 310321.Google Scholar
Macedonia, J. M. & Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among Mammalian Alarm Call Systems and the Problem of Meaning in Animal Signals. Ethology, 93, 177197.Google Scholar
Macintosh, N. J. (1994). Animal Learning and Cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1955). Characteristics of Some Animal Calls. Nature, 176, 68.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1970a). A Comparative Approach to Vocal Learning: Song Development in White-crowned Sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology (monograph suppl.), 71, 125.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1970b). Birdsong and Speech Development: Could There Be Parallels? American Scientist, 58, 669673.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1976). Sensory Templates in Species-specific Behavior. In Fentress, J. C. (Ed.), Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 314329. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1984). Song Learning: Innate Species Differences in the Learning Process. In Marler, P. & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.), The Biology of Learning. Dahlem Workshop Reports-Life Sciences Research Report 29. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1991). Song Learning Behavior: The Interface with Neuroethology. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 199206.Google Scholar
Marler, P. & Evans, C. (1996). Bird Calls: Just Emotional Displays or Something More? Ibis, 138, 2633.Google Scholar
Marler, P. & Peters, S. (1982). Subsong and Plastic Song: Their Role in the Vocal Learning Process. In Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H. (Eds.), Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, pp. 2550.Google Scholar
Marler, P. & Tamura, M. (1964). Culturally Transmitted Patterns of Vocal Behavior in Sparrows. Science, 146, 14831486.Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: FreemanGoogle Scholar
Martin, P. & Caro, T. M. (1985). On the Functions of Play and Its Role in Behavioral Development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 59103.Google Scholar
Mason, G. J. (2010). Species Differences in Responses to Captivity: Stress, Welfare and the Comparative Method. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25, 713721.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F. & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant Sensitivity to Distributional Information Can Affect Phonetic Discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101B111.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group Selection and Kin Selection. Nature, 201, 11451147.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. & Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of Animal Conflict. Nature, 246, 1518.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and Effect in Biology. Science, 134, 15011506.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1988). The Multiple Meanings of Teleological. Toward a New Philosophy of Biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 3866.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1993). Proximate and Ultimate Causations. Biology and Philosophy, 8, 9394.Google Scholar
McAleer, K. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2006). Testing Central Place Foraging in Eastern Chipmunks, Tamias Striatus, by Altering Loading Functions. Animal Behaviour, 71, 14471453.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1923). An Outline of Psychology. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. (1970). Behavioural Aspects of Homeostasis. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 3, 126.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. (1974). Time-sharing as a Behavioral Phenomenon. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 201225.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. & Houston, A. (1981). Quantitative Ethology: The State Space Approach. London: Pitman.Google Scholar
McKeon, R. (Ed.). (1947). Introduction to Aristotle. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
McKinney, F. (1975). The Evolution of Duck Displays. In Baerends, et al., pp. 331357.Google Scholar
McLaren, I. P. L. (1994). Representation Development in Associative Systems. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 377402.Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M. & Houston, A. I. (2009). Integrating Function and Mechanism. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 24, 670675.Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M., Houston, A. I. & Collins, E. J. (2001). Optimality Models in Behavioral Biology. SIAM Review, 42, 413466.Google Scholar
Meijer, T., Daan, S. & Hall, M. (1990). Family Planning in the Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): The Proximate Control of Covariation of Laying Date and Clutch Size. Behaviour, 114, 117160.Google Scholar
Mendl, M. T., Burman, O. H. P. & Paul, E. S. (2010). An Integrative and Functional Framework for the Study of Animal Emotion and Mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 28952904.Google Scholar
Mendl, M. T. & Paul, E. S. (2016). Bee Happy. Science, 353, 14991500.Google Scholar
Menzel, R. & Fischer, J. (2011). Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. (1993). Phylogenetic Relationships and Evolutionary Processes in East African Cichlid Fishes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 8, 279284.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. (2015). Extreme Evolution. Scientific American, 313(4), 7075.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1957). Experiments on Motivation. Science, 126, 12711278.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of Basic S-R Concepts: Extensions to Conflict Behavior, Motivation, and Social Learning. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science, vol. 2, pp. 196292. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Ming, G. & Song, H. (2011). Adult Neurogenesis in the Mammalian Brain: Significant Answers and Significant Questions. Neuron, 70, 687702.Google Scholar
Mirmiran, M., Maas, Y. G. H. & Ariagno, R. L. (2003). Development of Fetal and Neonatal Sleep and Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 7, 321334.Google Scholar
Mistleberger, R. E. (2011). Neurobiology of Food Anticipatory Circadian Rhythms. Physiology and Behavior, 104, 535545.Google Scholar
Mistleberger, R. E. & Rusak, B. (2005). Biological Rhythms and Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 7196.Google Scholar
Mittelstaedt, M.-L. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1980). Homing by Path Integration in a Mammal. Naturwissenschaften, 67, 566567.Google Scholar
Moczek, A. P. et al. (2015). The Significance and Scope of Evolutionary Developmental Biology: A Vision for the 21st Century. Evolution and Development, 17, 198219.Google Scholar
Moffatt, C. A. & Hogan, J. A. (1992). Ontogeny of Chick Responses to Maternal Food Calls in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 9296.Google Scholar
Møller, A. P. (1988). Female Choice Selects for Male Sexual Tail Ornaments in a Swallow. Nature, 332, 640642.Google Scholar
Møller, A. P. & de Lope, F. (1994). Differential Costs of a Secondary Sexual Character: An Experimental Test of the Handicap Principle. Evolution, 48, 16761683.Google Scholar
Montgomerie, R., Lyon, B. & Holder, K. (2001). Dirty Ptarmigan: Behavioral Modification of Conspicuous Male Plumage. Behavioral Ecology, 12, 429438.Google Scholar
Mook, D. G. (1987). Motivation: The Organization of Action. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Moore, B. R. (1973). The Role of Directed Pavlovian Reactions in Simple Instrumental Learning in the Pigeon. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 159188.Google Scholar
Moran, T. H. (1986). Environmental and Neural Determinants of Behavior in Development. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 2: Neuropsychology. New York: Plenum, pp. 99128.Google Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2010). Behavioral Ecology, 21, 343348.Google Scholar
Morgan, C. L. (1896). Habit and Instinct. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. (2012). The Evolutionary Basis of Human Social Learning. Procedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 653662.Google Scholar
Moro, A. (2014). On the Similarity between Syntax and Actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 109110.Google Scholar
Moro, A. (2014). Response to Pulvermüller: The Syntax of Actions and Other Metaphors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 221.Google Scholar
Morris, D. (1956). [Discussion following Lorenz] In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de I’homme. Paris: Masson, pp. 642643.Google Scholar
Morris, W. (Ed.). (1969). The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Moser, M-B. & Moser, E. I. (2016). Where Am I? Where Am I Going? Scientific American, 314(1), pp. 2633.Google Scholar
Mulder, C. K., Gerkema, M. P. & Van der Zee, E. A. (2013). Circadian Clocks and Memory: Time-Place Learning. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 6, article 8.Google Scholar
Neill, S. R. & Cullen, J. M. (1974). Experiments on Whether Schooling by Their Prey Affects the Hunting Behaviour of Cephalopods and Fish Predators. Journal of Zoology, 172, 549569.Google Scholar
Nelson, D. A. (1997). Social Interaction and Sensitive Phases for Song Learning: A Critical Review. In Snowdon, & Hausberger, , pp. 722.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1964). The Temporal Patterning of Courtship Behaviour in the Glandulocaudine Fishes. Behaviour, 24, 90146.Google Scholar
Nelson, R. J. (2016). An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology, 4th edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Fernandez, N., Newport, E. L. & Bavelier, D. (2015). Neural Systems Supporting Linguistic Structure, Linguistic Experience, and Symbolic Communication in Sign Language and Gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1168411689.Google Scholar
Novak, M. & Harlow, H. F. (1975). Social Recovery of Monkeys Isolated for the First Year of Life: I. Rehabilitation and Therapy. Developmental Psychology, 11, 453465.Google Scholar
O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N. & Feldman, M. W. (1996). Niche Construction. American Naturalist, 147, 641648.Google Scholar
Olkowicz, S, Kocourek, M., Lučan, R. K. et al. (2016). Birds have Primate-like Numbers of Neurons in the Forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 72557260.Google Scholar
Olds, J. (1958). Self-stimulation of the Brain. Science, 127, 315324.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. (1974). The Ontogeny of Behavior in the Chick Embryo. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 5, 133172.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic Adaptations and Retrogressive Processes in the Development of the Nervous System and Behaviour: A Neuroembryological Perspective. In Connolly, K. J. & Prechtl, H. F. R. (Eds.), Maturation and Development: Biological and Psychological Perspectives. Philadelphia: Lippincott, pp. 73109. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. (1991). Cell Death during Development of the Nervous System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 14, 453501.Google Scholar
Orr, H. A. (2009). Fitness and Its Role in Evolutionary Genetics. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, 531539.Google Scholar
Oster, H. (1978). Facial Expression and Affect Development. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), The Development of Affect. New York: Plenum, pp. 4376.Google Scholar
Ottoni, E. B. & Izar, P. (2008). Capuchin Monkey Tool Use: Overview and Implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17, 171178.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1985). The Ontogeny of Information: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E. & Gray, R. D. (2001). Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pagel, M., Atkinson, Q. D. & Meade, A. (2007). Frequency of Word-use Predicts Rates of Lexical Evolution throughout European History. Nature, 449, 717720.Google Scholar
Pagnotta, M. (2014). On the Controversy over Non-human Culture: The Reasons for Disagreement and Possible Directions toward Consensus. Behavioural Processes 109, 95100.Google Scholar
Palameta, B. & Lefebvre, L. (1985). The social Transmission of a Food-finding Technique in Pigeons: What Is Learned? Animal Behaviour, 33, 892896.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective Consciousness: Core Emotional Feelings in Animals and Humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 3080.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2010). Affective Consciousness in Animals: Perspectives on Dimensional and Primary Process Emotion Approaches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 29052907.Google Scholar
Paul, E. S., Harding, E. J. & Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring Emotional Processes in Animals: The Utility of a Cognitive Approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 469491.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Payne, R. B., Thompson, W. L., Fiala, K. L. & Sweany, L. L. (1981). Local Song Traditions in Indigo Buntings: Cultural Transmission of Behaviour Patterns across Generations. Behaviour, 77, 199221.Google Scholar
Pennisi, E. (2016a). Tracking how Humans Evolve in Real Time. Science, 352, 876877.Google Scholar
Pennisi, E. (2016b). Fossil Fishes Challenge “Urban Legend” of Evolution. Science, 353, 14831484.Google Scholar
Petherick, J. C., Seawright, E., Waddington, D., Duncan, I. J. H. & Murphy, L. B. (1995). The Role of Perception in the Causation of Dustbathing Behaviour in Domestic Fowl. Animal Behaviour, 49, 15211530.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. & Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the Manual Mode: Evidence for the Ontogeny of lLnguage. Science, 251, 14931496.Google Scholar
Pfaffmann, C. (1960). The Pleasures of Sensation. Psychological Review, 67, 253268.Google Scholar
Phelps, S. M., Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. (2001). Vestigial Preference Functions in Neural Networks and Túngara Frogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 13161134166.Google Scholar
Piersma, T. & van Gils, J. A. (2011). The Flexible Phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pievani, T. & Serrelli, E. (2011). Exaptation in Human Evolution: How to Test Adaptive vs Exaptive Evolutionary Hypotheses. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 89, 923.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought. New York: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
Pittendrigh, C. S. (1958). Adaptation, Natural Selection, and Behavior. In Roe, A. & Simpson, G. G. (Eds.), Behavior and Evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, pp. 390416.Google Scholar
Polsky, R. H. (1975). Hunger, Prey Feeding, and Predatory Aggression. Behavioral Biology, 13, 8193.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active Perception: Sensorimotor Circuits as a Cortical Basis for Language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 351360.Google Scholar
Raby, C. R., Alexisi, D. M., Dickinson, A. & Clayton, N. S. (2007). Planning for the Future by Western Scrub-Jays. Nature, 445, 919921.Google Scholar
Raff, R. A. (2000). Evo-Devo: The Evolution of a New Discipline. Nature Reviews Genetics, 1, 7479.Google Scholar
Rasa, O. A. E. (1971). Appetence for Aggression in Juvenile Damsel Fish. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, Beiheft, 7.Google Scholar
Réale, D, Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating Animal Temperament within Ecology and Evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291318.Google Scholar
Redish, A. D. (2001). The Hippocampal Debate: Are We Asking the Right Questions? Behavioral Brain Research, 127, 8198.Google Scholar
Reisbick, S. H. (1973). Development of Food Preferences in Newborn Guinea Pigs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 427442.Google Scholar
Rendell, L. et al. (2011). Cognitive Culture: Theoretical and Empirical Insights into Social Learning Strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 6876.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian Conditioning and Its Proper Control Procedures. Psychological Review, 74, 7180.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Behavioral Studies of Pavlovian Conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 329352.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W.F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 6499.Google Scholar
Resende, B. D., Nagy-Reis, M. B., Lacerda, F. N., Pagnotta, M. & Savalli, C. (2014). Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus sp.) Learning how to Crack Nuts: Does Variability Decline throughout Development? Behavioural Processes, 109, 8994.Google Scholar
Rhoad, K. D., Kalat, J. W. & Klopfer, P. (1975). Aggression and Avoidance by Betta splendens toward Natural and Artificial Stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior, 3, 271276.Google Scholar
Rial, R. V. et al. (2010). Evolution of Wakefulness, Sleep and Hibernation: From Reptiles to Mammals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 11441160.Google Scholar
Rice, J. C. (1978). Effects of Learning Constraints and Behavioural Organization on the Association of Vocalizations and Hunger in Burmese Red Junglefowl Chicks. Behaviour, 67, 259298.Google Scholar
Richards, R. J. (1992). The Meaning of Evolution. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richter, C. P. (1942). Total Self Regulatory Functions in Animals and Human Beings. The Harvey Lectures Series, 38, 63103.Google Scholar
Riebel, K. (2003). The “Mute” Sex Revisited; Vocal Production and Perception Learning in Female Songbirds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 33, 4986.Google Scholar
Rivkees, S. A. (2003). Developing Circadian Rhythmicity in Infants. Pediatrics, 112, 373381.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The Mirror-neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.Google Scholar
Roberts, E. P. & Weigl, P. D. (1984). Habitat Preference in the Dark-eyed Junco (Junco hyemalis): The Role of Photoperiod and Dominance. Animal Behaviour, 32, 709714.Google Scholar
Rodenburg, T. B. et al. (2004). Feather Pecking in Laying Hens: New Insights and Directions for Research? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 86, 291298.Google Scholar
Roeder, K. D. (1967). Nerve Cells and Insect Behavior, revised edn. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Roesch, M. R., Esber, G. R., Li, J., Daw, N. D. & Schoenbaum, G. (2013). Surprise! Neural Correlates of Pearce-Hall and Rescorla-Wagner Coexist within the Brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35, pp. 11901200.Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. R. (1883). Mental Evolution in Animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.Google Scholar
Roper, T. J. (1973). Nesting Material as a Reinforcer for Female Mice. Animal Behaviour, 21, 733740.Google Scholar
Roper, T. J. (1975). Nest Material and Food as Reinforcers for Fixed-ratio Responding in Mice. Learning and Motivation, 6, 327343.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1976). The Selection of Foods by Rats, Humans, and other Animals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 6, 2176.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1984). The Acquisition of Food Habits and Preferences. In Matarazzo, J. D., Weiss, S. M., Herd, J. A., Miller, N. E. & Weiss, S. M. (Eds.), Behavioral Health: A Handbook of Health Enhancement and Disease Prevention. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core Affect and the Psychological Construction of Emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145172.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1991). A Fresh Look at “Maternal Deprivation.” In Bateson, P. (Ed.), The Development and Integration of Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 331374.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2002). Nature, Nurture, and Development: From Evangelism through Science toward Policy and Practice. Child Development, 73, 121.Google Scholar
Ryan, M. J. (2009). The Evolution of Behavior, and Integrating It towards a Complete and Correct Understanding of Behavioral Biology. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 127146.Google Scholar
Ryan, M. J. (2005). Evolution of Behavior. In Bolhuis, & Giraldeau, , pp. 294314.Google Scholar
Ryan, M. J. & Cummings, M. E. (2003). Perceptual Biases and Mate Choice. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 44, 437459.Google Scholar
Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. (1999). Phylogenetic Influences on Mating Call Preferences in Female Túngara Frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus). Animal Behaviour, 57, 945956.Google Scholar
Sachs, B. D. & Barfield, R, J. (1976). Functional Analysis of Masculine Copulatory Behavior in the Rat. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 7, 92154.Google Scholar
Sackett, G. P. (1970). Unlearned Responses, Differential Rearing Experiences, and the Development of Social Attachments by Rhesus Monkeys. In Rosenblum, L. A. (Ed.), Primate Behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on Statistical Language Learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172196.Google Scholar
Sahay, A., Wilson, D. A. & Hen, R. (2011). Pattern Separation: A Common Function for New Neurons in Hippocampus and Olfactory Bulb. Neuron, 70, 582588.Google Scholar
Sanotra, G. S., Vestergaard, K. S., Agger, J. F. & Lawson, L. G. (1995). The Relative Preferences for Feathers, Straw, Wood-shavings and Sand for Dustbathing, Pecking and Scratching in Domestic Chicks. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 43, 263277.Google Scholar
Savory, C. J. (1979). Changes in Food Intake and Body Weight of Bantam Hens during Breeding. Applied Animal Ethology, 5, 283288.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, Social, and Physiological Determinants of Emotional State. Psychological Review, 69, 379399.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2009). Memory Systems, Neural Basis Of. In Bayne, T., Clemens, A. & Wilken, P. (Eds.), The Oxford Companion to Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 432436.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schiller, P. H. (1957). Manipulative Patterns in the Chimpanzee (1949). In Schiller, C. H. (Ed.), Instinctive Behavior. New York: International Universities Press, pp. 264287.Google Scholar
Schjelderup-Ebbe, Th. (1922). Beiträge zur Social-psychologie des Haushuhns. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 92, 225252.Google Scholar
Schluter, D. (1996). Ecological Causes of Adaptive Radiation. American Naturalist, 148 (Suppl.), S40S64.Google Scholar
Schmid-Hempel, P., Kacelnik, A. & Houston, A. I. (1985). Honeybees Maximise Efficiency by Not Filling Their Crop. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17, 6166.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C. (1949). Levels in the Psychological Capacities of Animals. In Sellars, R. W., McGill, V. J. & Farber, M. (Eds.), Philosophy for the Future: The Quest for Modern Materialism. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C. (1956). Interrelationships of the “Innate” and the “Acquired” in Instinctive Behavior. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de 1’homme. Paris: Masson.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C. (1965). Aspects of Stimulation and Organization in Approach/Withdrawal Processes Underlying Vertebrate Behavioral Development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1, 174.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C. & Rosenblatt, J. S. (1961). Behavioral Organization and Genesis of the Social Bond in Insects and Mammals. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 31, 223253.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C., Rosenblatt, J. S. & Tobach, E. (1963). Maternal Behavior in the Cat. In Rheingold, H. (Ed.), Maternal Behavior in Mammals. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral Theories and the Neurophysiology of Reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87115.Google Scholar
Sevenster, P. (1961). A Causal Analysis of a Displacement Activity (Fanning in Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Behaviour, Suppl. 9, 1170.Google Scholar
Sevenster, P. (1968). Motivation and Learning in Sticklebacks. In Ingle, D. (Ed.), Central Nervous System and Fish Behaviour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 233245.Google Scholar
Sevenster, P. (1973). Incompatibility of Response and Reward. In Hinde, & Stevenson-Hinde, , pp. 265283.Google Scholar
Sevenster, P. & van Roosmalen, M. E. (1985). Cognition in Sticklebacks: Some Experiments on Operant Conditioning. Behaviour93, 170183.Google Scholar
Sevenster-Bol, A. C. A. (1962). On the Causation of Drive Reduction after a Consummatory Act. Archive Néerlandaises de Zoologie, 15, 175236.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic Communication in a Free-ranging Primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 10701094.Google Scholar
Shatz, C. J. (1992). The Developing Brain. Scientific American, 267, 6067. (Reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Sheffield, F. D. (1966). A Drive Induction Theory of Reinforcement. In Haber, R. N. (Ed.), Current Research in Motivation. New York: Holt, pp. 98111.Google Scholar
Shelton, J. R. & Caramazza, A. (1999). Deficits in Lexical and Semantic Processing. Implications for Models of Normal Language. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 527.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W. (1988). The Levels of Analysis. Animal Behaviour, 36, 616619.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. (1977). Parental Food-calling and the Role of the Young in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus g. spadiceus). Animal Behaviour, 25, 594601.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. (1981). Parental Care and Development of Thermoregulation in Red Junglefowl. Behaviour, 76, 250279.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. (1985). Food Storage by Birds and Mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 153188.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. (2009). Do Ideas about Function Help in the Study of Causation? In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.), Tinbergen’s Legacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 147162.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Duff, S. J. (1996). Behavioural and Neural Bases of Orientation in Food-storing Birds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 165171.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Schacter, D. L. (1987). The Evolution of Multiple Memory Systems. Psychological Review, 94, 439454.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Strang, C. G. (2015). Contrasting Styles in Cognition and Behaviour in Bumblebees and Honeybees. Behavioural Processes, 117, 5969.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus and Memory for Food Caches in Black-capped Chickadees. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 308318.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F., Mrosovsky, N. & Hogan, J. A. (1980). Weight Loss and Anorexia during Incubation in Birds. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94, 8998.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1972). Constraints on Learning. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 4, 168.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1975). Reinforcement and the Organization of Behavior in Golden Hamsters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 5687.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shubin, N., Tabin, C. & Carroll, S. (2009). Deep Homology and the Origins of Evolutionary Novelty. Nature, 457, 818823.Google Scholar
Sih, A., Bell, A. & Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioral Syndromes: An Ecological and Evolutionary Overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19, 372378.Google Scholar
Silver, R. (1990). Biological Timing Mechanisms with Special Emphasis on the Parental Behavior of Doves. In Dewsbury, D. A. (Ed.), Contemporary Issues in Comparative Psychology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, pp. 252277.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are Theories of Learning Necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193216.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Slack, J. M. W. (1991). From Egg to Embryo. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slater, P. J. B. (1981). Individual Differences in Animal Behavior. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H. (Eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, vol. 4. New York: Plenum, pp. 3549.Google Scholar
Slater, P. J. B. (1986). The Cultural Transmission of Bird Song. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 9497.Google Scholar
Smith, K. S. & Graybiel, A. M. (2013). A Dual Operator View of Habitual Behavior Reflecting Cortical and Striatal Dynamics. Neuron, 79, 361374.Google Scholar
Snell-Rood, E. C. (2013). An Overview of the Evolutionary Causes and Consequences of Behavioural Plasticity. Animal Behaviour, 85, 10441011.Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. & Hausberger, M. (Eds.). (1997). Social Influences on Vocal Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sober, E. (2001). The Two Faces of Fitness. In Singh, R. S. et al. (Eds.), Thinking about Evolution, vol. 2. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 309321.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, M. B. (2001). Drosophila: Genetics Meets Behaviour. Nature Reviews Genetics, 2, 879890.Google Scholar
Sokolowski, M. B. (2010). Social Interactions in “Simple” Model Systems. Neuron, 65, 780794.Google Scholar
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M. & Lefebvre, L. (2008). Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced into Novel Environments. American Naturalist, 172, S63S71.Google Scholar
Spalding, D. A. (1873/1954). Instinct with Original Observations on Young Animals. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2, 211.Google Scholar
Speirs, A. L. (1962). Thalidomide and Congenital Abnormalities. Lancet, 279, 303305.Google Scholar
Spencer, H. (1864). Principles of Biology. London.Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. & Ayres, S. L. (1976). Sequential and Temporal Properties of Behavior Induced by a Schedule of Periodic Food Delivery. Behaviour, 54, 2649.Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. & Cerutti, D. T. (2003). Operant Conditioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 115144.Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The “Superstition” Experiment: A Reexamination of Its Implication for the Principles of Adaptive Behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 343.Google Scholar
Stamps, J. A. (2016). Individual Differences in Behavioural Plasticities. Biological Reviews, 91, 534567.Google Scholar
Stamps, J. A. & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). Developmental Perspectives on Personality: Implications for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies of Individual Differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 40294041.Google Scholar
Steiner, A. P. & Redish, A. (2014). Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Regret in Rat Decision-making on a Neuroeconomic Task. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 9951002.Google Scholar
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human Facial Expressions in Response to Taste and Smell Stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 13, 257295.Google Scholar
Stellar, E. (1960). Drive and Motivation. In Field, J., Magoun, H. W. & Hall, V. E. (Eds.), Handbook of Physiology (Section 1, Vol. 3). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 15011528.Google Scholar
Stephan, F. K. (2001). Food-entrainable Oscillators in Mammals. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 223246.Google Scholar
Stevens, M. (2005). The Role of Eyespots as Anti-predator Mechanisms, Principally Demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573588.Google Scholar
Stevens, M. (2013). Sensory Ecology, Behaviour, and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, M., Hardman, C. J. & Stubbins, C. L. (2008). Conspicuousness, Not Eye Mimicry, Makes ‘Eyespots’ Effective Antipredator Signals. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 525531.Google Scholar
Stevenson, J. G. (1967). Reinforcing Effects of Chaffinch Song. Animal Behaviour, 15, 427432.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1967). Social Structure among Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Behaviour, 29, 6121.Google Scholar
Suboski, M. D. & Bartashunas, C. (1984). Mechanisms for the Social Transmission of Pecking Preferences in Neonatal Chicks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 182194.Google Scholar
Sutherland, N. S. (1964). The Learning of Discriminations by Animals. Endeavour, 23, 148152.Google Scholar
Tarsitano, M. & Andrew, R. (1999). Scanning and Route Selection in the Jumping Spider Portia Labiata. Animal Behaviour, 58, 255265.Google Scholar
ten Cate, C. (1984). The Influence of Social Relations on the Development of Species Recognition in Zebra Finch Males. Behaviour, 91, 263285.Google Scholar
ten Cate, C. (1986). Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males Exposed to Two Species. I. A Case of Double Imprinting. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 248252.Google Scholar
ten Cate, C. (1987). Sexual Preferences in Zebra Finch Males Exposed to Two Species. II. The Internal Representation Resulting from Double Imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 35, 321330.Google Scholar
ten Cate, C. (1994). Perceptual Mechanisms in Imprinting and Song Learning. In Hogan, & Bolhuis, , pp. 116146.Google Scholar
ten Cate, C. (1991). Behaviour-contingent Exposure to Song and Zebra Finch Song Learning. Animal Behaviour, 42, 857859.Google Scholar
Terman, M. (1983). Behavioral Analysis and Circadian Rhythms. In Zeiler, M. D. & Harzen, P. (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior, vol. 3: Biological Factors in Learning. New York: Wiley, pp. 103141.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. (1985). Expression as Action: A Motor Perspective on the Transition from Spontaneous to Instrumental Behaviors. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The Development of Expressive Behavior. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, pp. 221267.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. (1995). Motor Development: A New Synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 7995.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. & Fisher, C. M. (1992). Newborn Stepping: An Explanation for a “Disappearing Reflex.” Developmental Psychology, 18, 760775.Google Scholar
Thompson, T. I. (1963). Visual Reinforcement in Siamese Fighting Fish. Science, 141, 5557.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal Intelligence. Psychological Review, Monograph suppl. 2, 1113.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal Intelligence. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1931). Human Learning. New York: Century.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. (1956). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. (1958). The Learning of Song Patterns by Birds, with Especial Reference to the Song of the Chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Ibis, 100, 535570.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. (1961). Bird Song. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. & Jones, F. G. W. (1937). Olfactory Conditioning and Its Relation to the Problem of Host Selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 124, 5681.Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1994). Behavior Systems, Associationism, and Pavlovian Conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 405420.Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (2004). Is Operant Contingency Enough for a Science of Purposive Behavior? Behavior and Philosophy, 32, 197229.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, J. M. (1981). Foraging Decisions in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Ardea, 69, 167.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, J. M. & Daan, S. (1990). Family Planning in the Great Tit (Parus major): Optimal Clutch Size as Integration of Parent and Offspring Fitness. Behaviour, 114, 161190.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1932/1972). On the Orientation of the Digger Wasp Philanthus triangulum Fabr. In Tinbergen, N. (Ed.), The Animal in Its World, vol. 1. London: George Allen & Unwin, pp. 103127.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1940). Die Übersprungbewegung. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 4, 140.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1942). An Objectivistic Study of the Innate Behaviour of Animals. Bibliotheca Biotheoretica, 1, 3998.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived Activities: Their Causation, Biological Significance, Origin and Emancipation during Evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27, 132.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1959). Comparative Studies of the Behaviour of Gulls (Laridae): A Progress Report. Behaviour, 15, 170.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On Aims and Methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410433.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1965). Animal Behavior. New York: Time, Inc.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1972). Ethology (1969). In The Animal in Its World, Vol. 2. London: Allen and Unwin, pp. 130160.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N., Broekhuysen, G. J., Feekes, F., Houghton, J. C. W., Kruuk, H. & Szulc, E. (1963). Egg Shell Removal by the Black-headed Gull, Larus ridibundus L.; A Behaviour Component of Camouflage. Behaviour, 19, 74117.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1983). Animal Behaviour – A Systems Approach. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. & Jensen, P. (1991). Ethological and Psychological Models of Motivation: Towards a Synthesis. In Meyer, J.-A. & Wilson, S. (Eds.), From Animals to Animats, pp. 194205. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Language Is Not an Instinct. Cognitive Development, 10, 131156.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L. & Randles, D. (2011). Four Models of Basic Emotions: A Review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3, 397405.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-Offspring Conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249264.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental Investment and Sexual Selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 139179.Google Scholar
Trut, L. N. (1999). Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment. American Scientist, 87, 160169.Google Scholar
Trut, L. N., Oskins, I. & Khariamova, A. (2009). Animal Evolution during Domestication: The Domesticated Fox as a Model. BioEssays, 31, 349360.Google Scholar
Underwood, H. (2001). Circadian Organization in Nonmammalian Vertebrates. In Takahashi, J. S., Turek, F. W. & Moore, R. Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 12: Circadian Clocks. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 247290.Google Scholar
Van der Kooy, D. & Hogan, J. A. (1978). Priming Effects with Food and Water Reinforcers in Hamsters. Learning and Motivation, 9, 332346.Google Scholar
van der Meer, M. A. A. & Redish, A. D. (2010). Expectancies in Decision Making, Reinforcement Learning, and Ventral Striatum. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 2937.Google Scholar
van der Zee, E. A., et al. (2008). Circadian Time-Place Learning in Mice Depends on Cry Genes. Current Biology, 18, 844848.Google Scholar
van Gils, J. A., Schenk, I. W., Bos, O. & Piersma, T. (2003). Incompletely Informed Shorebirds that Face a Digestive Constraint Maximize Net Energy Gain when Exploiting Patches. American Naturalist, 161, 777793.Google Scholar
van Iersel, J. J. A. (1953). An Analysis of the Parental Behaviour of the Three-spined Stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Behaviour, Suppl. 3, 1159.Google Scholar
van Iersel, J. J. A. & Bol, A. C. A. (1958). Preening of Two Tern Species. A study on Displacement Activities. Behaviour, 13, 188.Google Scholar
van Kampen, H. S. (1996). A Framework for the Study of Filial Imprinting and the Development of Attachment. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 320.Google Scholar
van Kampen, H. S. (1997). Courtship Food-calling in Burmese Red Junglefowl: II. Sexual Conditioning and the Role of the Female. Behaviour, 134, 775787.Google Scholar
van Kampen, H. S. (2015). Violated Expectancies: Cause and Function of Exploration, Fear, and Aggression. Behavioural Processes, 117, 1228.Google Scholar
van Kampen, H. S. & Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Auditory Learning and Filial Imprinting in the Chick. Behaviour, 117, 303339.Google Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1970). Tool Using in Primates and Other Vertebrates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 3, 195249.Google Scholar
van Liere, D. W. (1992). The Significance of Fowls’ Bathing in Dust. Animal Welfare, 1, 187202.Google Scholar
van Liere, D. W. & Bokma, S. (1987). Short Term Feather Maintenance as a Function of Dustbathing in Laying Hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 18, 197204.Google Scholar
van Rhijn, J. G. (1991). The Ruff. London: Poyser.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. et al. (2003). Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture. Science, 299, 102105.Google Scholar
Verkuil, Y. I. (2010). The Ephemeral Shorebird: Population History of Ruffs. Ph.D. thesis, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.Google Scholar
Verkuil, Y. I. et al. (2012). Losing a Staging Area: Eastward Redistribution of Afro-Eurasian Ruffs Is Associated with Deteriorating Fuelling Conditions along the Western Flyway. Biological Conservation, 149, 5159.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S. (1982). Dust-bathing in the Domestic Fowl: Diurnal Rhythm and Dust Deprivation. Applied Animal Ethology, 8, 487495.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S. & Baranyiova, E. (1996). Pecking and Scratching in the Development of Dust Perception in Young Chicks. Acta Veterinaria Brno, 65, 133142.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S. & Hogan, J. A. (1992). The Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl. III. Effects of Experience on Stimulus Preference. Behaviour, 121, 215230.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S., Damm, B. I., Abbot, U. K. & Bildsøe, M. (1999). Regulation of Dustbathing in Feathered and Featherless Domestic Chicks: The Lorenzian Model Revisited. Animal Behaviour, 58, 10171025.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S., Hogan, J. A. & Kruijt, J. P. (1990). The Development of a Behavior System: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl: I. The Influence of the Rearing Environment on the Organization of Dustbathing. Behaviour, 112, 99116.Google Scholar
Vestergaard, K. S., Kruijt, J. P. & Hogan, J. A. (1993). Feather Pecking and Chronic Fear in Groups of Red Junglefowl: Its Relations to Dustbathing, Rearing Environment and Social Status. Animal Behaviour, 45, 11271140.Google Scholar
von Frisch, K. (1955). The Dancing Bees. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1878). Lecture: The Facts of Perception. Selected Writings of Hermann Helmholtz. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
von Holst, E. (1935). Über den Prozess der zentralnervösen Koordination. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 236, 149158.Google Scholar
von Holst, E. (1937). Vom Wesen der Ordnung im Zentralnervensystem. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 625631, 641647. [Tr. as: On the Nature of Order in the Central Nervous System. In von Holst, , 1973, pp. 3–32.]Google Scholar
von Holst, E. (1954). Relations between the Central Nervous System and the Peripheral Organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2, 8994.Google Scholar
von Holst, E. (1973). The Selected Papers of Erich von Holst, vol. 1. (Tr. by Martin, R.). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464476. [Tr. as: The Reafference Principle. In von Holst, , 1973, pp. 139–173.]Google Scholar
von Holst, E. & Saint Paul, U. von. (1960). Vom Wirkungsgefüge der Triebe. Naturwissenschaften, 47, 409422. [Tr. as: On the Functional Organisation of Drives. Animal Behaviour, 1963, 11, 1–20; also in von Holst, , 1973, pp. 220–258.]Google Scholar
von Uexküll, J. (1921). Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere. Berlin.Google Scholar
von Uexküll, J. (1934). Streifzüge durch die Umwelten von Tieren und Menschen. Berlin: Springer. [Tr. as: A Stroll through the World of Animals and Men. In Schiller, C. H. (Ed.), Instinctive Behavior. New York: International Universities Press, 1957, pp. 580.]Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1934/1988). Thought and Language. (Tr. by Kozulin, A.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of Development and the Inheritance of Acquired Characters. Nature, 150, pp. 563565.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1953). Genetic Assimilation of an Acquired Character. Evolution, 7, 118126.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1959a). Evolutionary Systems – Animal and Human. Nature, 183, 16341638.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1959b). Canalization of Development and Genetic Assimilation of Acquired Characters. Nature, 183, 16541655.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1966). Principles of Development and Differentiation. New York: Macmillan. (Excerpt reprinted in Bolhuis, & Hogan, , 1999.)Google Scholar
Walker, S. (1983). Animal Thought. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Warden, C. J., Warner, H. C. & Jenkins, T. N. (1931). Animal Motivation: Experimental Studies on the Albino Rat. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Wasserman, E. A. (1973). Pavlovian Conditioning with Heat Reinforcement Produces Stimulus-directed Pecking in Chicks. Science, 181, 875877.Google Scholar
Watkins, C. (1969). The Indo-European Origin of English. In Morris, W. (Ed.), The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin, pp. xixxx.Google Scholar
Watson, A. (1973). Moults of Wild Scottish Ptarmigan, Lagopus mutus, in Relation to Sex, Climate and Status. Journal of Zoology (London) 171, 207223.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1908). The behavior of Noddy and Sooty Terns. Papers from the Tortugas Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, 2, 187255.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Wauters, A. M., Richard-Yris, M. A. & Talec, N. (2002). Maternal Influences on Feeding and General Activity in Domestic Chicks. Ethology, 108, 529540.Google Scholar
Wayne, R. K. & vonHoldt, B. (2012). Evolutionary Genomics of Dog Domestication. Mammalian Genome, 23, 318.Google Scholar
Webb, W. B. (1998). Sleep. In Greenberg, G. & Haraway, M. M. (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook. New York: Garland, pp. 327331.Google Scholar
Wehner, R. & Menzel, R. (1990). Do Insects Have Cognitive Maps? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 403414.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1992). The Organization and Reorganization of Human Speech Perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 377402.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Development Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
West, M. J. & King, A. P. (1988). Female Visual Display Affects the Development of Males Song in the Cowbird. Nature, 334, 244246.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. et al. (2001). Charting Cultural Variation in Chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 14811516.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in Chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.Google Scholar
Whitman, C. O. (1911). The Behavior of Pigeons. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute, Publication, 257, pp. 1161.Google Scholar
Wiesel, T. N. (1982). Postnatal Development of the Visual Cortex and the Influence of the Environment. Nature, 299, 583591.Google Scholar
Wilczynski, W. Rand, A. S. & Ryan, M. J. (2001). Evolution of Calls and Auditory Tuning in the Physalaemus pustulosus Species Group. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 58, 137151.Google Scholar
Williams, C. L. (1991). Development of a Sexually Dimorphic Behavior: Hormonal and Neural Controls. In Shair, H. N., Barr, G. A. & Hofer, M. A. (Eds.), Developmental Psychobiology: New Methods and Changing Concepts. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 206222.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. M. (1966). Central Nervous Mechanisms for the Generation of Rhythmic Behaviour in Arthropods. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 20, 199228.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the Theoretical Foundation of Sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 327348.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1971). The Insect Societies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian Navigation: From Historical to Modern Concepts. Animal Behaviour, 65, 257272.Google Scholar
Wise, R.A. (2004). Dopamine, Learning and Motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 483494.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J. & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of Sensorimotor Learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 739751.Google Scholar
Wood-Gush, D. G. M. (1971). The Behaviour of the Domestic Fowl. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Wouters, A. G. (2003). Four Notions of Biological Function. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 34, 633668.Google Scholar
Wray, G. A. et al. (2014). Does Evolutionary Theory Need a Rethink? No, All Is Well. Nature, 514, 161164.Google Scholar
Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). Do Animals Think? Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Xie, L. et al. (2013). Sleep Drives Metabolite Clearance from the Adult Brain. Science, 342, 373377.Google Scholar
Young, P. T. (1961). Motivation and Emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate Selection – A Selection for a Handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.Google Scholar
Zeder, M. A. (2012). The Domestication of Animals. Journal of Anthropological Research, 68, 161190.Google Scholar
Zeiler, M. D. (1992). On Immediate Function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 417427.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Referential Labeling in Diana Monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59, 917927.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Survivor Signals: The Biology and Psychology of Animal Alarm Calling. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 40, 277322.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Jerry A. Hogan, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Study of Behavior
  • Online publication: 26 October 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781108123792.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Jerry A. Hogan, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Study of Behavior
  • Online publication: 26 October 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781108123792.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Jerry A. Hogan, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Study of Behavior
  • Online publication: 26 October 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781108123792.013
Available formats
×