Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T23:16:11.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

51 - Human Development in Evolutionary-Biological Perspective

from Section B - Social/Personality Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

It was the best idea I would ever have, I realized, as soon as I had it – a true eureka experience – while teaching a graduate seminar early in my career. I had been struggling with an intellectual issue as I had never done before, seeking insight that would enable me to test a provocative – even radical – new theoretical perspective on the ways in which the experiences children have while growing up shape who they become later in life. My best idea would not only revolutionize my thinking, but would stimulate a second important insight.

My training in and early research on human development was rather traditional, founded on the commonsense idea that there are good ways of caring for children that promote their well-being – or “optimal” development – and bad ways that foster problematic functioning. Thus, sensitive-responsive parenting, provided by economically secure and happily married parents, fosters children's emotional security, intellectual competence, and social skills, resulting, eventually, in an adult able to love and to work in productive and satisfying ways. In contrast, marital discord, economic insufficiency, and detached, intrusive, insensitive, or harsh parenting foster the opposite. These views were founded on classic developmental perspectives, including psychoanalytic, attachment, and social-learning theories.

But my thinking began to change after reading a fascinating anthropology paper that a colleague, Pat Draper, had written and left with me many months earlier. Pat's work introduced me to life-history theory, a branch of evolutionary biology which stipulates that ALL living things have the same fundamental goal – and it is not to be rich, happy, or healthy, but, rather, to pass their genes on to future generations. And that the best way to get this job of life done depends on the conditions in which the living organism finds itself, humans included. Notably, then, there is no optimal development. So what my training had taught me regarding what qualified as “good” parenting, “healthy” family life, and “optimal” child development turned out, when examined from the perspective of life-history theory and evolutionary biology, to be best only under particular circumstances, but not others.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 240 - 243
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Belsky, J. (2012). The development of human reproductive strategies: Progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 310–316.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., & van Ijzendoorn, M. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 305–309.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013). Beyond risk, resilience and dysregulation: Phenotypic plasticity and human development. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 1243–1261.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×