Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T12:33:29.195Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - CONFIRMATORY QUESTIONNAIRES

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

M. Granger Morgan
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University, Pennsylvania
Baruch Fischhoff
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University, Pennsylvania
Ann Bostrom
Affiliation:
Georgia Institute of Technology
Cynthia J. Atman
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Get access

Summary

Mental models interviews usually produce a rich array of lay beliefs about a risk. If one could afford to do a hundred or more of these interviews, the results should also provide relatively accurate estimates of how common various beliefs are in the population being studied. However, the expense of these interviews typically precludes doing more than a modest number. In order to estimate prevalence, we rely on closed-form questions, which can be much more efficiently administered to large numbers of respondents.

Objectives of Questionnaire Studies

Knowing the frequency of concepts in the target population allows communicators to identify both widely shared correct concepts upon which a message can be built and widespread misconceptions that need to be addressed. Questionnaires can also be used to explore specific issues suggested in the mental models interviews but not adequately resolved. For example, our mental models interviews about climate change suggested that many people confused the terms “climate” and “weather.” Both refer to the condition of the atmosphere, measured in terms of wind, temperature, humidity, atmospheric pressure, precipitation, and so on. In most places, weather can change from hour-to-hour, day-to-day, and season-to-season. In contrast, climate is the average or overall pattern of weather in a place. For example, San Diego has a “Mediterranean climate,” meaning that temperatures are generally moderate year-round, there is limited rainfall, and humidity is typically low.

Type
Chapter
Information
Risk Communication
A Mental Models Approach
, pp. 84 - 96
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×