Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T22:07:44.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2014

Glynis M. Breakwell
Affiliation:
University of Bath
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbey, A., Saenz, C. and Buck, P. O. (2005). The cumulative effects of acute alcohol consumption, individual differences and situational perceptions on sexual decision making. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 82–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abele, A. and Hermer, P. (1993). Mood influences on health-related judgments: appraisal of own health versus appraisal of unhealthy behaviors. European Review of Social Psychology, 23, 613–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abelson, R. P. and Levi, A. (1985). Decision-making and decision theory. In Lindzey, G. and Aronson, E. Jason (eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 231–309). New York: Random House, 19th edition.Google Scholar
Abrams, D., Wetherell, M. S., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A. and Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing what to think by knowing who you are: self-categorisation and the nature of norm formation, conformity and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 97–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abric, J. C. (1994). Les Représentations sociales: aspectes théoretiques. In Abric, J. C. (ed.), Pratiques Sociales et Représentations (pp. 11–35). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Agha, S. (2003). The impact of mass media campaign on personal risk perception, perceived self-efficacy and on other behavioural predictors. AIDS Care, 15, 749–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahern, J., Galea, S., Resnick, H. and Vlahov, D. (2004). Television images and probable post-traumatic stress disorder after Sept 11: the role of background characteristics, event exposure and perievent panic. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 192, 3, 217–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alhakami, A. S. and Slovic, P. (1994). A psychological study of the inverse relationship between perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis, 14, 1085–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breithenbecher, D. L., Yurack, T. J. and Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individualisation and the better than average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 804–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, P. T. (1998). Public participation in resolving environmental disputes and the problem of representativeness. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 9, 297–308.Google Scholar
Andersen, C. F. et al. (2007). The New Orleans Hurricane Protection System: What Went Wrong and Why (PDF). American Society of Civil Engineers Hurricane Katrina External Review Panel. .
Anderson, C. and Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansell, J. (1992). Reliability, industrial risk assessment. In Ansell, J. and Wharton, F. (eds.), Risk: Analysis, Assessment and Management. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Appleton-Knapp, S. L. (2003). Memory dynamics in hindsight biases. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B, The Sciences and Engineering, 63 (7-B), 3488.Google Scholar
Ardelt, M. (2000). Still suitable after all these years? Personality stability theory revisited. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 392–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arden, P. (1993). Create a corporate safety culture. Safety & Health, 60–3.Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R. (1991). Costs and benefits of judgment errors: implications for debiasing. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 486–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Faust, D., Guilmette, T., , J. and Hart, K. (1988). Eliminating hindsight bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 2, 305–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Saville, P. D. and Harkness, A. R. (1981). Hindsight bias among physicians weighing the likelihood of diagnoses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 252–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnstein, S. R. (1969). A ladder of citizen participation. AIP Journal, 35, 4, 216–24.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1997). Bring the family. APS Observer, July/August, 17.Google Scholar
Atman, C. J., Bostrom, A., Fischhoff, B. and Morgan, M. G. (1994). Designing risk communications: completing and correcting mental models of hazards processes, part I. Risk Analysis, 14, 779–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aven, T. (2011). On different types of uncertainties in the context of the precautionary principle. Risk Analysis, 31, 1515–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aven, T. (2013). On how to deal with deep uncertainties in a risk assessment and management context. Risk Analysis, 33, 2082–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, A. B., Buunk, B. P. and Manstead, A. S. (1997). The moderating role of self-efficacy beliefs in the relationship between anticipated feelings of regret and condom use. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 2001–14.Google Scholar
Balzano, Q. and Sheppard, A. R. (2002). The influence of the precautionary principle on science-based decision-making: questionable applications to risks of radiofrequency fields. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 351–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Barke, R. and Jenkins-Smith, H. (1993). Politics and scientific expertise: scientists, risk perception, and nuclear waste policy. Risk Analysis, 13, 425–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barlow, D. H., Rapee, R. M. and Brown, T. A. (1992). Behavioural treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Therapy, 20, 261–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, J. and Breakwell, G. M. (2001). Risk perception and experience: hazard personality profiles and individual differences. Risk Analysis, 21, 171–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, J. and Breakwell, G. M. (2003). The social amplification of risk and the hazard sequence: the October 1995 oral contraceptive pill scare. Health, Risk & Society, 5, 301–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., Hershey, J. C. and Kunreuther, H. (2000). Determinants of priority for risk reduction: the role of worry. Risk Analysis, 20, 413–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barratt, E. O., Cabal, L. F. and Moeller, F. G. (2004). Impulsitivity and sensation seeking: a historical perspective on current challenges. In Stelmack, R. M. (ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 3–15). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartholomew, R. E. and Wessely, S. (2002). Protean nature of mass sociogenic illness: from possessed nuns to chemical and biological terrorism fears. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 300–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Basili, M. and Franzini, M. (2006). Understanding the risk of an avian flu pandemic: rational waiting or precautionary failure?Risk Analysis, 26, 617–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, I. J., Langford, I. H., Turner, R. K., Willis, K. G. and Garrod, G. D. (1995). Elicitation and truncation effects in contingent valuation studies. Ecological Economics, 12, 161–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bea, R. G. (2002). Human and organizational factors in reliability assessment and management of offshore structures. Risk Analysis, 22, 29–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beard, M. (2011). Risk and the humanities: Alea iacta est. In Skinns, L., Scott, M. and Cox, T. (eds.), Risk (pp. 85–108). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, U. (1992). Risk Society: Towards a New Modernity. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Beck, U. (2006). Living in the world risk society. Economy and Society, 35, 329–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begum, H. A. and Ahmed, E. (1986). Individual risk-taking and risky shift as a function of cooperation–competition proneness of subjects. Psychological Studies, 31, 1, 21–5.Google Scholar
Beierle, T. C. (2004). The benefits and costs of disclosing information about risks: what do we know about right-to-know?Risk Analysis, 24, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benthin, A., Slovic, P. and Severson, H. (1993). A psychometric study of adolescent risk perception. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 153–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergstrom, R. L. and McCaul, K. D. (2004). Perceived risk and worry: the effects of 9/11 on willingness to fly. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 1846–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, J., Brabazon, P., Bellamy, L. and Huddleston, J. (1994). The Regulator as a Determinant of the Safety Culture. London: HSE Report.Google Scholar
Bernoulli, D. (1954). A new theory on the measurement of risk, trans. Sommer, L.. Econometrica, 22, 36. Original work published 1738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, N. R. (1990). Fire. In Coddington, R. and Noshpitz, J. D. (eds.), Stressors and the Adjustment Disorders (pp. 260–77). Oxford: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Binder, A. R., Scheufele, D. A., Brossard, D. and Gunther, A. C. (2011). Interpersonal amplification of risk? Citizen discussions and their impact on perceptions of risks and benefits of a biological research facility. Risk Analysis, 31: 324–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blank, H., Musch, J. and Pohl, R. F. (2007). Hindsight bias: on being wise after the event. Social Cognition, 25, 1, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blank, H. and Nestler, S. (2007). Cognitive process models of hindsight bias. Social Cognition, 25, 1, 132–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blume, S. (2006). Anti-vaccination movements and their interpretations. Social Science & Medicine, 62, 628–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boholm, A. (1996). Risk perception and social anthropology: critique of cultural theory. Ethnos, 61, 64–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boholm, A. (2009). Risk and causality in newspaper reporting. Risk Analysis, 29, 1566–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonaiuto, M., Breakwell, G. M. and Cano, I. (1996). Identity processes and environmental threat: the effects of nationalism and local identity upon perception of beach pollution. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 6, 157–75.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boney-McCoy, S., Gibbons, F. X., Reis, T. J., Gerrard, M., Luus, C. A. E. and Sufka, A. V. W. (1992). Perceptions of smoking risk as a function of smoking status. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15, 469–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bord, R. J. and O’Connor, R. E. (1990). Risk communication, knowledge, and attitudes: explaining reactions to a technology perceived as risky. Risk Analysis, 10, 499–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bord, R. J. and O’Connor, R. E. (1997). The gender gap in environmental attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 78, 4, 830–40.Google Scholar
Boscarino, J. A., Adams, R. E. and Figley, C. R. (2004). Mental health service use 1 year after the World Trade Center disaster: implications for mental health care. General Hospital Psychiatry, 26, 346–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bostrom, A., Atman, C., Fischhoff, B. and Morgan, M. (1994a). Evaluating risk communications: completing and correcting mental models of hazardous processes, Part 2. Risk Analysis, 14, 789–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostrom, A., Fischhoff, B. and Morgan, M. G. (1992). Characterizing mental models of hazardous processes: a methodology and an application to radon. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 85–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B. and Read, D. (1994b). What do people know about global climate-change? 1. Mental models. Risk Analysis, 14, 959–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouyer, M., Bagdassarian, S., Chaabanne, S. and Mullet, E. (2001). Personality correlates of risk perception. Risk Analysis, 21, 457–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradfield, A. and Wells, G. L. (2005). Not the same old hindsight bias: outcome information distorts a broad range of retrospective judgments. Memory and Cognition, 33, 120–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brauer, M., Judd, C. M. and Gliner, M. D. (1995). The effects of repeated expressions on attitude polarization during group discussion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1014–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (1986). Coping with Threatened Identities. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (1993). Integrating paradigms, methodological implications. In Breakwell, G. M. and Canter, D. V. (eds.), Empirical Approaches to Social Representations (pp. 180–201). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (1994). The echo of power: a framework for social psychological research. The Psychologist, 17, 65–72.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (1996). Risk estimation and sexual behaviour: a longitudinal study of 16–21 year olds. Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 79–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breakwell, G. M. (2000). Risk communication: factors affecting impact. British Medical Bulletin, 56, 1, 110–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breakwell, G. M. (2001a). Mental models and social representations of hazards: the significance of identity processes. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 341–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2001b). What makes innovation acceptable? In Toutant, J. P. and Balazc, I. (eds.), Molecular Farming (pp. 279–88). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2014a). Identity process theory: clarifications and elaborations. In Jaspal, R. and Breakwell, G. M. (eds.), Identity Process Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2014b). Identity and social representations. In Jaspal, R. and Breakwell, G. M. (eds.), Identity Process Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2014c). Epilogue. In Jaspal, R. and Breakwell, G. M. (eds.), Identity Process Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2014d). Identity process theory. In Sammut, G., Daanen, P., Sartawi, M. and Gaskell, G. (eds.), Resistance, Stability and Social Change: A Handbook of Social Representations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. (2014e). Risk: social psychological perspectives. In Wright, J. D. (ed.), The International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioural Sciences. New York: Elsevier, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. and Barnett, J. (2000). Social influence processes and their effect on risk amplification. In Cottam, M. P., Harvey, D. W., Pape, R. P. and Tait, J. (eds.), Foresight and Precaution (pp. 1181–7). Rotterdam: A. A. Balkema.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. and Barnett, J. (2001). The Impact of Social Amplification on Risk Communication. Contract Research Report 322/2001. Sudbury: HSE Books.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. and Barnett, J. (2003a). The significance of uncertainty and conflict: developing a social psychological theory of risk communication. New Review of Social Psychology, 2, 107–14.Google Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. and Barnett, J. (2003b). Social amplification of risk and the layering method. In Pidgeon, N., Kasperson, R. and Slovic, P. (eds.), The Social Amplification of Risk (pp. 80–101). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G. M., Millward, L. and Fife-Schaw, C. R. (1994). Commitment to ‘safer’ sex as a predictor of condom use amongst 16–20 year olds. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 189–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G. M. and Petts, J. (2000). Stakeholder Participation Methods – Scoping Study. Report to the HSE. RSU Ref 4072/R64.063.
Breakwell, G. M. and Rowett, C. (1982). Social Work: The Social Psychological Approach. Wokingham: Van Nostrand Reinhold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brenkert-Smith, H., Dickinson, K. L., Champ, P. A. and Flores, N. (2013). Social amplification of wildfire risk: the role of social interactions and information sources. Risk Analysis, 33, 800–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenot, J. and Bonnefous, S. (1995). Approche socio-culturelle de la perception des risques. Fontenay-aux-Roses, France: Institut de Protection et de Sûreté Nucléaire.Google Scholar
Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, B. M. and Martin, C. L. (1995). Decision frame, procedural justice, and survivors’ reactions to job layoffs. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Process, 63, 59–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brody, C. J. (1984). Differences by sex in support for nuclear power. Social Issues, 63, 209–28.Google Scholar
Broers, A. N. (2005). The nuclear energy option – will we still need it – and if so – when?Science in Parliament, 62, 16–17.Google Scholar
Bromiley, P. and Curley, S. P. (1992). Individual differences in risk taking. In Yates, J. F. (ed.), Risk-Taking Behaviour (pp. 87–132). Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Bronfman, N. C. and Cifuentes, L. A. (2003). Risk perception in a developing country: the case of Chile. Risk Analysis, 23, 1271–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosius, H. B. and Weimann, G. (1996). Who sets the agenda? Agenda-setting as a two-step flow. Communication Research, 23, 561–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, R., Akter, S., Brander, L. and Haque, E. (2007). Socioeconomic vulnerability and adaptation to environmental risk: a case study of climate change and flooding in Bangladesh. Risk Analysis, 27: 313–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, J. M., Henderson, J. and Armstrong, M. P. (1987). Children’s perceptions of nuclear power stations as revealed through their drawings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 7, 189–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brun, W. (1992). Cognitive components in risk perception: natural versus manmade risks. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 5, 117–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, F. B. and Guilbault, R. L. (2002). ‘I knew it all along’ eventually: the development of hindsight bias in reaction to the Clinton impeachment verdict. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 27–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bull, R. and Carson, D. (eds.) (2001). Handbook of Psychology in Legal Contexts. Chichester: John Wiley.
Burger, J. M. and Burns, L. (1988). The illusion of unique invulnerability and the use of effective contraception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 264–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, A. (2012). Media, risk, and absence of blame for ‘acts of God’: attenuation of the European volcanic ash cloud of 2010. Risk Analysis, 32, 1693–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, C., Mearns, K. and McGeorge, P. (2006). Explicit and implicit trust within safety culture. Risk Analysis, 26, 1139–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, W. J., Peters, E. and Slovic, P. (2012). Risk perception and the economic crisis: a longitudinal study of the trajectory of perceived risk. Risk Analysis, 32, 659–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, W. J., Slovic, P. and Kasperson, R. E. (1993). Incorporating structural models into research on the social amplification of risk: implication for theory construction and decision making. Risk Analysis, 13, 611–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnstein, E. and Vinokur, A. (1977). Persuasive argumentation and social comparison as determinants of attitude polarization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 315–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, R. S. (1999). The social capital of opinion leaders. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 566, 37–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buunk, B. P., Bakker, A. B., Siero, F. W., Eijinden, R. J. and Yzer, M. C. (1998). Predictors of AIDS-preventive behavioural intentions among adult heterosexuals at risk for HIV-infection: extending current models and measures. AIDS Education and Prevention, 10, 149–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Caffray, C. M. and Schneider, S. L. (2000). Why do they do it? Affective motivators in adolescents’ decisions to participate in risk behaviours. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 4, 543–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callaghan, P., Moloney, G. and Blair, D. (2012). Contagion in the representational field of water recycling: informing new environment practice through social representation theory. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 22, 20–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calman, K. C. (2002). Communication of risk: choice, consent and trust. Lancet, 360, 166–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J. D. and Tesser, A. (1983). Motivational interpretations of hindsight bias: an individual difference analysis. Journal of Personality, 51, 605–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, L. F. and Stewart, A. E. (1992). Effects of group membership on perception of risk for AIDS. Psychological Reports, 70, 1075–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canelon, M. L. and Rovira, D. P. (2002). Representaciones sociales de la enfermedad de Chagas en comunidades en riesgo: creencias, actitudes y prevención [Social representation of Chagas disease in at-risk communities: beliefs, attitudes and prevention]. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia, 36, 215–36.Google Scholar
Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E. and Converse, S. A. (1990). Cognitive psychology and team training: training shared mental models of complex systems. Human Factors Bulletin, 33, 1–4.Google Scholar
Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E. and Converse, S. A. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team decision-making. In Castellan, N. J. (ed.), Individual and Group Decision Making: Current Issues. New Jersey: LEA.Google Scholar
Cantor, N. and Mischel, W. (1977). Traits as prototypes: effects on recognition memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 38–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caponecchia, C. (2012). Relative risk perception for terrorism: implications for preparedness and risk communication. Risk Analysis, 32, 1524–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carnes, S. A., Schweitzer, M., Peelle, E. B., Wolfe, A. K. and Munro, J. F. (1998). Measuring the success of public participation on environmental restoration and waste management activities in the US Department of Energy. Technology in Society, 20, 385–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carney, R. E. (1971). Attitudes towards risk. In Carney, R. E. (ed.), Risk Taking Behaviour: Concepts, Methods, and Applications to Smoking and Drug Abuse (pp. 1–27). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Carson, L. (1999). In a sneak preview of the referendum, your average Australian voted yes. University of Sydney News, 6.Google Scholar
Carter, D. A. (1991). Aspects of risk assessment for hazardous pipeline containing flammable substances. Journal of Loss Prevention in the Process Industries, 4, 68–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carvalho, A. and Burgess, J. (2005). Cultural circuits of climate change in UK broadsheet newspapers. Risk Analysis, 25, 1457–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casman, E. A. and Fischhoff, B. (2008). Risk communication planning for the aftermath of a plague bioattack. Risk Analysis, 28, 1327–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellan, N. J. (1993). Individual and Group Decision Making: Current Issues. New Jersey: LEA.Google Scholar
Castro, P. and Gomes, I. (2005). Genetically modified organisms in the Portuguese press: thematization and anchoring. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 35, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, G. B. and Coups, E. J. (2006). Emotion and preventative health behaviour: worry, regret, and influenza vaccination. Health Psychology, 25, 82–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, G. B. and Johnson, E. J. (1999). Anchoring, activation, and the construction of values. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79, 1–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chauvin, B., Hermand, D. and Mullet, E. (2007). Risk perception and personality facets. Risk Analysis, 27, 171–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherpitel, C. J. (1993). Alcohol, injury and risk-taking behaviour: data from a national sample. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research, 17, 762–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chess, C. and Purcell, K. (1999). Public participation and the environment: do we know what works?Environmental Science & Technology, 33, 2685–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, I. J. (2011). Social amplification of risk in the Internet environment. Risk Analysis, 31, 1883–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M. C. and Payne, R. L. (2006). Character-based determinants of trust in leaders. Risk Analysis, 26, 1161–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, S. and Ward, K. (2006). The role of leader influence tactics and safety climate in engaging employees’ safety participation. Risk Analysis, 26, 1175–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cliff, S. M., Wilkins, J. C. and Davidson, E. A. F. (1993). Impulsiveness, venture-someness and sexual risk-taking among heterosexual GUM clinic attenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 403–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coles, R. and Hodgkinson, G. P. (2008). A psychometric study of information technology risks in the workplace. Risk Analysis, 28, 81–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colyvan, M. (2008). Is probability the only coherent approach to uncertainty?Risk Analysis, 28, 645–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, R. (2010). Conundrums with uncertainty factors. Risk Analysis, 30, 330–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T. and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Costa-Font, J., Rudisill, C. and Mossialos, E. (2008). Attitudes as an expression of knowledge and ‘political anchoring’: the case of nuclear power in the United Kingdom. Risk Analysis, 28, 1273–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotgrove, S. (1982). Catastrophe or Cornucopia: The Environment, Politics and the Future. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Cowley, S. and Billings, J. R. (1999). Identifying approaches to meet assessed needs in health visiting. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 8, 527–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, L. A. (2008). Some limitations of ‘risk = threat × vulnerability × consequence’ for risk analysis of terrorist attacks. Risk Analysis, 28, 1749–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, L. A. and Popken, D. A. (2008). Overcoming confirmation bias in causal attribution: a case study of antibiotic resistance risks. Risk Analysis, 28, 1155–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, P., Fischhoff, B., Gerrard, S., Niewöhner, J., Pidgeon, N. and Riley, D. (2001). Developing a Methodology for Designing Messages about Chemical Risks in the Workplace Using the Mental Models Approach. Report to the Health and Safety Executive.
Cox, S. and Cox, T. (1991). The structure of employee attitudes to safety: a European example. Work & Stress, 5, 93–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, S., Jones, B. and Collinson, D. (2006). Trust relations in high-reliability organizations. Risk Analysis, 26, 1123–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coyle, I., Sleeman, S. and Adams, D. (1995). Safety climate. Journal of Safety Research, 22, 247–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crane, S. F. and Carswell, J. W. (1992). A review and assessment of nongovernmental organization-based STD/AIDS education and prevention projects for marginalized groups. Health Education Research, 7, 175–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crisp, B. R. and Barber, J. G. (1995). The effect of locus of control on the association between risk perception and sexual risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 841–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crossland, B., Bennett, P. A., Ellis, A. F., Farmer, F. R., Gittus, J., Godfrey, P. S. et al. (1992). Estimating Engineering Risk. London: Royal Society.Google Scholar
Cvetkovich, G. (1999). The attribution of social trust. In Cvetkovich, G. and Löfstedt, R. E. (eds.), Social Trust and the Management of Risk (pp. 53–61). London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Cvetkovich, G. and Earle, T. C. (eds.) (1991). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, special issue: Risk and Culture, 22, 11–149.
Cvetkovich, G. and Löfstedt, R. E. (eds.) (1999). Social Trust and the Management of Risk. London: Earthscan.
Cvetkovich, G., Siegrist, M., Murray, R. and Tragesser, S. (2002). New information and social trust: asymmetry and perseverance of attributions about hazard managers. Risk Analysis, 22, 359–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlstrom, M. F., Dudo, A. and Brossard, D. (2012). Precision of information, sensational information, and self-efficacy information as message-level variables affecting risk perceptions. Risk Analysis, 32, 155–166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daily Telegraph (2011). Obama oil spill commission’s final report blames disaster on cost-cutting by BP and partners. The Daily Telegraph, 5 January.
Dake, K. (1991). Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk: an analysis of contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 61–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dake, K. and Wildavsky, A. (1991). Individual differences in risk perception and risk-taking preferences. In Garrick, B. J. and Gekler, W. C. (eds.), The Analysis, Communication and Perception of Risk (pp. 98–117). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Das, E., de-Wit, J. and Stroebe, W. (2003). Fear appeals motivate acceptance of action recommendations: evidence for a positive bias in the processing of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 650–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, D. J. and Frendenburg, W. R. (1996). Gender and environment risk concerns: a review and analysis of available research. Environment and Behaviour, 28, 3, 302–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. F. (1993). Field dependence and hindsight bias: output interference in the generation of reasons. Journal of Research in Personality, 27, 222–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. A. and Bobko, P. (1986). Contextual effects on escalation processes in public sector decision-making. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 37, 121–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, I. G. J., Johnson, J. E. V. and Luke, M. A. (2012). Do people believe combined hazards can present synergistic risks?Risk Analysis, 32, 801–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Bruin, W. B., Güvenç, Ü., Fischhoff, B., Armstrong, C. M. and Caruso, D. (2009). Communicating about xenotransplantation: models and scenarios. Risk Analysis, 29, 1105–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Hoog, N., Stroebe, W. and de-Wit, J. B. F. (2005). The impact of fear appeals on processing and acceptance of action recommendations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 24–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jonge, J., Van Trijp, H., Renes, R. J. and Frewer, L. J. (2010). Consumer confidence in the safety of food and newspaper coverage of food safety issues: a longitudinal perspective. Risk Analysis, 30, 125–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Man, A. F., Simpson-Housley, P. and Curtis, F. (1984a). Trait anxiety, perception of potential nuclear hazard, and state anxiety. Psychological Reports, 54, 3, 791–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Man, A. F., Simpson-Housley, P., Curtis, F. and Smith, D. (1984b). Trait anxiety and response to potential disaster. Psychological Reports, 54, 507–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Souza, L. C. G. (2001). The social representations of the car and the traffic behaviour of youths/as representacoes sociais do carro e o comportamento dos jovens no transito. Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, 53, 125–37.Google Scholar
de-Wit, J., Teunis, N., Van Griensven, G. and Sandfort, T. H. (1994). Behavioral risk reduction strategies to prevent HIV infection among homosexual men: a grounded theory approach. AIDS Education and Prevention, 6, 493–505.Google ScholarPubMed
Dedobbeleer, N. and Beland, F. (1991). A safety climate measure for construction sites. Journal of Safety Research, 22, 97–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deery, H. A. and Love, A. W. (1996). The effect of a moderate dose of alcohol on the traffic hazard perception profile of young drink-drivers. Addiction, 91, 815–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deitz, S. R. E. and Thoms, W. E. E. (1991). Pilots, Personality, and Performance: Human Behaviour and Stress in the Skies. New York: Quorum Books.Google Scholar
Dejoy, D. M. (1989). An attribution theory perspective on alcohol-impaired driving. Health Education Quarterly, 16, 359–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dejoy, D. M. (1992). An examination of gender differences in traffic accident risk perception. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 24, 237–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dejoy, D. M. (1994). Managing safety in the workplace: an attribution theory analysis and model. Journal of Safety Research, 25, 3–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dekker, S. W. A. (2004). The hindsight bias is not a bias and not about history. Human Factors and Aerospace Safety, 4, 87–99.Google Scholar
Demicheli, V. et al. (2012). The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, vol. 2, CD004407. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Department of Health (1997). Communicating about Risks to Public Health: Pointers to Good Practice. Department of Health.Google Scholar
Dillon, R. L., Tinsley, C. H. and Cronin, M. (2011). Why near-miss events can decrease an individual’s protective response to hurricanes. Risk Analysis, 31, 440–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dockins, C., Jenkins, R. R., Owens, N., Simon, N. B. and Bembenek Wiggins, L. (2002). Valuation of childhood risk reduction: the importance of age, risk preferences, and perspective. Risk Analysis, 22, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohle, S., Keller, C. and Siegrist, M. (2010). Examining the relationship between affect and implicit associations: implications for risk perception. Risk Analysis, 30, 1116–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doise, W. (1982). Levels of Explanation in Social Psychology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dolinski, D., Wojciech, G. and Zawisza, E. (1987). Unrealistic pessimism. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 511–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, I. and Canter, D. (1994). Employee attitudes and safety in the chemical industry. Journal of Loss Prevention in the Process Industries, 7, 203–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donohew, L., Bardo, M. T. and Zimmerman, R. S. (2004). Personality and risky behaviour: communication and prevention. In Stelmack, R. M. (ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 223–45). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorn, L. and Matthews, G. (1995). Prediction of mood and risk appraisals from trait measures: two studies of simulated driving. European Journal of Personality, 9, 25–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorner, D. (1987). On the difficulties people have in dealing with complexity. In Rasmussen, J., Duncan, K. and Leplat, J. (eds.), New Technologies and Human Error (pp. 97–109). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Dosman, D. M., Adamowicz, W. L. and Hrudey, S. E. (2001). Socioeconomic determinants of health – and food safety – related risk perceptions. Risk Analysis, 21, 307–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, M. (1982). Essays in the Sociology of Perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Douglas, M. (1986). Risk Acceptability According to the Social Sciences. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Douglas, M. and Calvez, M. (1990). The self as risk taker: a cultural theory of contagion in relation to AIDS. Sociological Review, 38, 445–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. and Wildavsky, A. (1982). Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Downs, T. J., Ross, L., Goble, R., Subedi, R., Greenberg, S. and Taylor, O. (2011). Vulnerability, risk perception, and health profile of marginalized people exposed to multiple built-environment stressors in Worcester, Massachusetts: a pilot project. Risk Analysis, 31, 609–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drottz-Sjöberg, B. M. and Sjöberg, L. (1990). Risk perception and worries after the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 10, 135–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunegan, K. J. (1993). Framing cognitive modes, and image theory: toward an understanding of a glass half full. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 491–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durodie, B. and Wessely, S. (2002). Resilience or panic? The public and terrorist attack. Lancet, 360, 1901–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earle, T. C. (2004). Thinking aloud about trust: a protocol analysis of trust in risk management. Risk Analysis, 24, 169–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earle, T. C. (2010). Trust in risk management: a model-based review of empirical research. Risk Analysis, 30, 541–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earle, T. C. and Cvetkovich, G. T. (1995). Social Trust: Toward a Cosmopolitan Society. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
The Economist (2012). Blow-ups happen: nuclear plants can be kept safe only by constantly worrying about their dangers. The Economist, 10 March.
Eidinow, E. (2007). Oracles, Curses, and Risk among the Ancient Greeks. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiser, J. R. and Arnold, B. W. A. (1999). Out in the midday sun: risk behaviour and optimistic beliefs among residents and visitors on Tenerife. Psychology and Health, 14, 529–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiser, J. R., Miles, S. and Frewer, L. J. (2002). Trust, perceived risk, and attitudes toward food technologies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32, 2423–33.Google Scholar
Eiser, J. R., Stafford, T., Henneberry, J. and Catney, P. (2009). ‘Trust me, I’m a scientist (not a developer)’: perceived expertise and motives as predictors of trust in assessment of risk from contaminated land. Risk Analysis, 29, 288–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eitzinger, C. and Wiedemann, P. M. (2008). Trust in the safety of tourist destinations: hard to gain, easy to lose? New insights on the asymmetry principle. Risk Analysis, 28, 843–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Ekenes, J. M. and Cameron, S. (1993). Fostering a safety culture. Light Metals, 21–5, 883–7.Google Scholar
Elcheroth, G., Doise, W. and Reicher, S. (2011). On the knowledge of politics and the politics of knowledge: how a social representations approach helps us rethink the subject of political psychology. Political Psychology, 32, 729–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
El-Hajje, E. and Ahmed, R. (1997). The effect of mood on group decision-making. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B, The Sciences and Engineering, 57, 5976.Google Scholar
Engen, T. (1972). Psychophysics. In Kling, J. W. and Riggs, L. A. (eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 1–46). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 3rd edition.Google Scholar
Englander, T., Farago, K., Slovic, P. and Fischhoff, B. (1986). Comparative analysis of risk perception in Hungary and the United States. Social Behaviour, 1, 55–66.Google Scholar
Englich, B. and Soder, K. (2009). Moody experts: how mood and expertise influence judgmental anchoring. Judgmental and Decision Making, 4, 41–50.Google Scholar
Epley, N. and Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back into the anchoring and adjustment heuristic: differential processing of self-generated and experimenter-provided anchors. Psychological Science, 12, 391–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eroglu, C. and Croxton, K. L. (2010). Biases in judgmental adjustments of statistical forecasts: the role of individual differences. International Journal of Forecasting, 26, 116–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield: C. C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G. and Abdel-Khalex, A. M. (1992). Cross-cultural comparisons of the I-sub-7 impulsiveness scales for Egyptian and English adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 8, 149–53.Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., Daum, I., Schugens, M. M. and Diehl, J. M. (1990). A cross-cultural study of impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy: Germany and England. Zeitschrift für Dfferentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 11, 209–13.Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G. and Eysenck, H. J. (1963). On the dual nature of extraversion. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 2, 46–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G. and Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Impulsiveness and venturesomeness: their position in a dimensional scale of personality description. Psychological Reports, 43, 1247–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G. and Eysenck, H. J. (1980). Impulsiveness and venturesomeness in children. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 73–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G. and Allsopp, J. F. (1985). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 613–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Facione, N. C. (2002). Perceived risk of breast cancer: influence of heuristic thinking. Cancer Practice, 10, 256–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, N. R. and Bailis, D. S. (2001). Air bag safety: media coverage, popular conceptions, and public policy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 444–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, N. M., Cummings, D. A. T., Cauchemez, S. and Fraser, C. (2005). Strategies for containing an emerging influenza pandemic in Southeast Asia. Nature, 7056, 209–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, N. M., Cummings, D. A. T., Fraser, C., Cajka, J. C., Cooley, P. C. and Burke, D. S. (2006). Strategies for mitigating an influenza pandemic. Nature, 7101, 448–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fife-Schaw, C. and Barnett, J. (2003). Measuring optimistic bias. In Breakwell, G. M. (ed.), Doing Social Psychology Research (pp. 54–74). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fife-Schaw, C. and Rowe, G. (1996). Public perceptions of everyday food hazards: a psychometric study. Risk Analysis, 16, 487–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A. S., Slovic, P. and Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risk and benefits. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 13, 1–17.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A., Peters, V., Neebe, M., Vávra, J., Kriel, A., Lapka, M. and Megyesi, B. (2012). Climate change? No, wise resource use is the issue: social representations of energy, climate change and the future. Environmental Policy and Governance, 22: 161–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight? Foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–99.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1994). The psychology of risk characterization. In Sahlin, N. E. and Brehmer, B. (eds.), Future Risks and Risk Management (pp. 125–39). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1995). Risk perception and communication unplugged: twenty years of process. Risk Analysis, 15, 137–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischhoff, B. (1999). Why (cancer) risk communication can be hard. Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 7–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (2001). Defining stigma. In Flynn, J., Slovic, P. and Kunreuther, H. (eds.), Risk, Media and Stigma: Understanding Public Challenges to Modern Science and Technology (pp. 361–8). London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. and Beyth, R. (1975). ‘I knew it would happen’: remembered probabilities of once-future things. Organizational Behaviour and Human Performance, 13, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A. and Quadrel, M. J. (1997). Risk Perception and Communication. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A. and Quadrel, M. J. (2000). Risk Perception and Communication. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Gonzalez, R. M. and Lerner, J. S. S. D. A. (2005). Evolving judgments of terror risks: foresight, hindsight, and emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 124–39.Google ScholarPubMed
Fischhoff, B., Hohenemser, C., Kasperson, R. E. and Kates, R. W. (1978b). Can hazard management be improved?Environment, 20, 16–20, 32–7.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. and Kadvany, J. (2011). Risk: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P. and Lichtenstein, S. (1978a). Fault trees: sensitivity of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 330–44.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S. and Combs, B. (1978c). How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sciences, 9, 127–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Florig, H. K., Granger Morgan, M., Morgan, K. M., Jenni, K. E., Fischhoff, B., Fischbeck, P. S. et al. (2001). A deliberative method for ranking risks (1): overview and test bed development. Risk Analysis, 21, 913–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Floyd, D. L., Prentice-Dunn, S. and Rogers, R. W. (2000). A meta-analysis of research on protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 407–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J., Peters, E., Mertz, C. K. and Slovic, P. (1998). Risk, media, and stigma at Rocky Flats. Risk Analysis, 18, 715–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J., Slovic, P. and Kunreuther, H. (eds.) (2001). Risk, Media and Stigma: Understanding Public Challenges to Modern Science and Technology. London: Earthscan.
Flynn, J., Slovic, P. and Mertz, C. K. (1993). The Nevada Initiative: a risk communication fiasco. Risk in File, 138, 497–502.Google Scholar
Flynn, J., Slovic, P. and Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14, 1101–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontaine, K. and Snyder, L. B. (1995). Optimistic bias in cancer risk perception: a cross-national study. Psychological Reports, 77, 143–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P. E. (2000). Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Franco, K., Belinson, J., Casey, G., Plummer, S., Tamburrino, M. and Tung, E. (2000). Adjustment to perceived ovarian cancer risk. Psychooncology, 9, 411–17.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, D. P., Gayton, E. L., Burton, J., Thorogood, M. and Marteau, T. M. (2002). Measuring perceptions of synergistic circulatory disease risk due to smoking and the oral contraceptive pill. Risk Analysis, 22, 1139–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freudenburg, W. R. (1992). Nothing succeeds like success? Risk analysis and the organizational amplification of risks. Risk: Issues in Health and Safety, 3, 1–53.Google Scholar
Frewer, L. (1999a). Demographic Differences in Risk Perception and Public Priorities for Risk Mitigation. Ministry of Agriculture Fisheries and Food, UK Government.
Frewer, L. (1999b). Risk perception, social trust, and public participation in strategic decision making: implications for emerging technologies. Ambio, 28, 569–74.Google Scholar
Frewer, L. J., Howard, C., Hedderley, D. and Shepherd, R. (1996). What determines trust in information about food-related risks? Underlying psychological constructs. Risk Analysis, 16, 473–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewer, L. J. and Miles, S. (2003). Temporal stability of the psychological determinants of trust: implications for communication about food risks. Health, Risk & Society, 5, 259–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frewer, L. J., Miles, S. and Marsh, R. (2002a). The GM foods controversy: a test of the social amplification of risk model. Risk Analysis, 22, 713–23.Google Scholar
Frewer, L. J., Miles, S. and Marsh, R. (2002b). The media and genetically modified foods: evidence in support of social amplification of risk. Risk Analysis, 22, 701–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewer, L. J., Raats, M. and Shepherd, R. (1994). Modelling the media: the transmission of risk information in the British quality press. Journal of the Institute of Mathematics and its Applications to Industry, 5, 235–47.Google Scholar
Frewer, L. J., Scholderer, J. and Bredahl, L. (2003). Communicating about the risks and benefits of genetically modified foods: the mediating role of trust. Risk Analysis, 23, 1117–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frost, S., Myers, L. B. and Newman, S. P. (2001). Genetic screening for Alzheimer’s disease: what factors predict intentions to take a test?Behavioural Medicine, 27, 101–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fugas, C. S., Silva, S. A. and Meliá, J. L. (2013). Profiling safety behaviors: exploration of the sociocognitive variables that best discriminate between different behavioral patterns. Risk Analysis, 33, 838–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funabashi, Y and Kitazawa, K (2012). Fukushima in review: A complex disaster, a disastrous response. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists, 68, 2, 9–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. and Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of the anchoring effect. Journal of Socio-Economics, 40, 1, 35–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadomska, M. (1994). Risk communication. In Radiation and Society: Comprehending Radiation Risk. International Atomic Energy Agency Proceedings, Vienna, vol. 1, pp. 147–66.Google Scholar
Galanter, E. (1990). Utility functions for nonmonetary events. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 449–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galanter, E. and Pliner, P. (1974). Cross-modality matching of money against other continua. In Moskowitz, H., Sharf, B. and Stevens, J. C. (eds.), Sensation and Measurement: Papers in Honour of S. S. Stevens (pp. 65–76). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallucci, R. (2012). ‘What – me worry?’ ‘Why so serious?’: a personal view on the Fukushima nuclear reactor accidents. Risk Analysis, 32, 1444–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganzach, Y. (2001). Judging risk and return of financial assets. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 83, 353–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, D. (2009) The Science of Fear: How the Culture of Fear Manipulates Your Brain. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Gardner, G. T. and Gould, L. C. (1989). Public perceptions of the risks and benefits of technology. Risk Analysis, 9, 225–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garvin, T. (2001). The epistemological distances between scientists, policy makers and the public. Risk Analysis, 21, 443–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasper, K. and Clore, G. L. (1998). The persistent use of negative affect by anxious individuals to estimate risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1350–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gasper, K. and Clore, G. L. (2000). Do you have to pay attention to your feelings to be influenced by them?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 266, 698–711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geller, E. S. (1994). Ten principles for achieving a total safety culture. Professional Safety, 39, 18–24.Google Scholar
Geller, E. S. (1995). Key to achieving a total safety culture. Professional Safety, 40, 16–22.Google Scholar
Geller, S., Burns, S. E. and Brailer, D. J. (1996). The impact of nonclinical factors on practice variations: the case of hysterectomies. Health Services Research, 30, 723–50.Google ScholarPubMed
Gescheider, G. A. (1976). Psychophysics: Method and Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1990). The Consequences of Modernity. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and Self-Identity. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Giesecke, J. A., Burns, W. J., Barrett, A., Bayrak, E., Rose, A., Slovic, P. and Suher, M. (2012). Assessment of the regional economic impacts of catastrophic events: CGE Analysis of resource loss and behavioral effects of an RDD attack scenario. Risk Analysis, 32, 583–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G. (2006). Out of the frying pan into the fire: behavioural reactions to terrorist attacks. Risk Analysis, 26, 347–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigone, D. and Hastie, R. (1993). The common knowledge effect: information sharing and group judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 959–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girandola, F. (2000). Peur et persuasion: présentations des recherches (1953–1998) et d’une nouvelle lecture [Fear and persuasion: review and re-analysis of the literature (1953–1998)]. Année-Psychologique, 100, 333–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, T. A. (2001). Understanding public response to disasters. Public Health Reports, 116, 69–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glass, T. A. and Schoch-Spana, M. (2002). Bioterrorism and the people: how to vaccinate a city against panic. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 34, 217–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 1, 26–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goma-i-Freixanet, M. (2004). Sensation seeking and participation in physical sports. In Stelmack, R. M. (ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 185–201). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, R., Kozlova, A., Kwiatkowska, A., Luu, L. A. N., Nizharadze, G., Realo, A. et al. (2003). Social representations of HIV/AIDS in Central and Eastern Europe. Social Science and Medicine, 56, 1373–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, R., Kozlova, A., Nizharadze, G. and Polyakova, G. (2004a). HIV/AIDS among adolescents in Eastern Europe: knowledge of HIV/AIDS, social representations of risk and sexual activity amongst school children and homeless adolescents in Russia, Georgia and the Ukraine. Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 381–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, R., Kwiatkowska, A., Realo, A., Kozlova, A., Luu, L. A. N. and Nizharadze, G. (2004b). Social representations of HIV/AIDS in five Central European and Eastern European countries: a multidimensional analysis. AIDS Care, 16, 669–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, K. E., Dooley, J. M., Camfield, P. R., Camfield, C. S. and MacSween, J. (2002). Parents of children with epilepsy are optimistic for their children’s health, but relatively pessimistic when compared with other parents. Epilepsy and Behaviour, 3, 262–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorman, J. M. (2005). In the wake of trauma. CNS Spectrums, 10, 81–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goszczynska, M., Tyszka, T. and Slovic, P. (1991). Risk perception in Poland: a comparison with three other countries. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 4, 179–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, L. C., Gardner, G. T., Deluca, D. R., Tiemann, A. R., Doob, L. W. and Strolwijk, J. A. J. (1988). Perceptions of Technological Risks and Benefits. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Graham, J. D. (2001). Decision-analytic refinements of the precautionary principle. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 2, 127–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. D. and Hsia, S. (2002). Europe’s precautionary principle: promise and pitfalls. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 371–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grasso, B. C., Rothschild, J. M., Jordan, C. W. and Jayaram, G. (2005). What is the measure of a safe hospital? Medication errors missed by risk management, clinical staff, and surveyors. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 11, 268–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grendstad, G. and Grendstad, P. S. (1994). Kultur som Levemate. Politica, 26, 420–38.Google Scholar
Grendstad, G. and Selle, P. (1995). Cultural theory and the new institutionalism. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 7, 1, 5–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grobe, D., Douthitt, R. and Zepeda, L. (1999). A model of consumers’ risk perceptions toward recombinant bovine growth hormone (rbgh): the impact of risk characteristics. Risk Analysis, 19, 661–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunert, K. G., Brunso, K., Bredahl, L. and Beck, A. C. (2001). Food-related lifestyle: a segmentation approach to European food consumers. In Frewer, L. J., Risvik, E. and Schifferstein, R. (eds.), Food, People and Society (pp. 211–30). Berlin: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guldenmund, F. W. (2010). (Mis)understanding safety culture and its relationship to safety management. Risk Analysis, 30, 1466–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurmankin, A. D., Baron, J. and Armstrong, K. (2004). Intended message versus message received in hypothetical physician risk communication: exploring the gap. Risk Analysis, 24, 1337–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafson, P. E. (1998). Gender differences in risk perception. Risk Analysis, 18, 6, 805–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutteling, J. M. and Kuttschreuter, M. (1999). The millennium bug controversy in the Netherlands: expert views versus public perception. In Goossens, L. H. J. (ed.), Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference on Risk Analysis: Facing the New Millennium (pp. 489–93). Delft University Press.Google Scholar
Gutteling, J. M. and Kuttschreuter, M. (2002). The role of expertise in risk communication: lay people’s and experts’ perception of the millennium bug risk in the Netherlands. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 35–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutteling, J. M. and Wiegman, O. (1993). Gender specific reactions to environmental hazards in the Netherlands. Sex Roles, 28, 433–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gwartney-Gibbs, P. A. and Lach, D. H. (1991). Sex differences in attitudes toward nuclear war. Journal of Peace Research, 28, 161–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakes, J. K. and Viscusi, W. K. (2004). Dead reckoning: demographic determinants of the accuracy of mortality risk perceptions. Risk Analysis, 24, 651–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallman, J. (2009). Why We Make Mistakes. New York: Broadway Books.Google Scholar
Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Millstein, S. G., Ellen, J. M., Adler, N. E., Tschann, J. M. and Biehl, M. (2001). The role of behavioural experience in judging risks. Health Psychology, 20, 120–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammitt, J. K., Wiener, J. B., Swedlow, B., Kall, D. and Zhou, Z. (2005). Precautionary regulation in Europe and the United States: a quantitative comparison. Risk Analysis, 25, 1215–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, S. (2000). Using psychometric tests. In Breakwell, G. M., Hammond, S. and Fife-Schaw, C. (eds.), Research Methods in Psychology (pp. 182–209). London: Sage, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Hampson, S. E., Andrews, J. A., Barckley, M., Lee, M. E. and Lichtenstein, E. (2003). Assessing perceptions of synergistic health risk: a comparison of two scales. Risk Analysis, 23, 1021–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampson, S. E., Andrews, J. A., Lee, M. E., Foster, L. S., Glasgow, R. E. and Lichtenstein, E. (1998). Lay understanding of synergistic risk: the case of radon and cigarette smoking. Risk Analysis, 18, 343–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S. and Lerner, J. S. (2009). Decision making. In Sander, D. and Scherer, K. R. (eds.), Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences (pp. 111–13). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Handmer, J. and Penning-Rowsell, E. (1990). Hazards and the Communication of Risk. Aldershot: Gower.Google Scholar
Hanekamp, J. C. (2006). Precaution and cholera: a response to Tickner and Gouveia-Vigeant. Risk Analysis, 26, 1013–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanekamp, J. C., Vera-Navas, G. and Verstegen, S. W. (2005). The historical roots of precautionary thinking: the cultural ecological critique and ‘The Limits to Growth’. Journal of Risk Research, 8, 295–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathology Checklist Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Harmon, K. (2012). Japan’s post-Fukushima earthquake health woes go beyond radiation effects. Nature. .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, G., Rice, M. and Quincy, V. (1993). Violent recidivism of mentally disordered offenders: the development of a statistical prediction instrument. Criminal Justice Behaviour, 20, 315–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P., Middleton, W. and Joiner, R. (2000). The typical student as an ingroup member: eliminating optimistic bias by reducing social distance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 235–54.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
’t Hart, P. and Pijnenburg, B. (1989). The Heizel Stadium tragedy. In Charles, M. T. and Rosenthal, U. (eds.), Coping with Crisis: The Management of Disasters, Riots and Terrorism (pp. 197–224). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G. and Buss, D. M. (2001). Emotional reactions following first-time sexual intercourse: the affective shift hypothesis. Personal Relationships, 8, 357–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hashiguchi, K. (1974). The number of decision makers and the level of risk taking within a group. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 123–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassan, A. (1986). Circumcision in the Sudan. Unpublished MSc dissertation, University of Surrey, Guildford.
Hastings, G., Stead, M. and Webb, J. (2004). Fear appeals in social marketing: strategic and ethical reasons for concern. Psychology & Marketing, 21, 961–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Health and Safety Commission (1991). Major Hazard Aspects of the Transport of Dangerous Substances. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Heath, R. L. and Palenchar, M. (2000). Community relations and risk communication: a longitudinal study of the impact of emergency response messages. Journal of Public Relations Research, 12, 131–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaven, P. C. (1991). Venturesomeness, impulsiveness, and Eysenck’s personality dimensions: a study among Australian adolescents. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 152, 91–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedge, B. (1989). Worried well and psychological issues. AIDS Care, 1, 193–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heffernan, M. (2011). Wilful Blindness. London: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Heilbrun, K. (1999). Basic and advanced issues in risk assessment. Paper presented at Workshop of the Department of Public Welfare, Commonwealth of Pennsylvania.
Heine, S. J. and Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: does the West feel more invulnerable than the East?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 595–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helsloot, I. and Ruitenberg, A. (2004). Citizen response to disasters. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 12, 98–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helweg-Larsen, M. (1999). (The lack of) optimistic biases in response to the 1994 Northridge earthquake: the role of personal experience. Journal of Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 110–29.Google Scholar
Helweg-Larsen, M., Sadeghian, P. and Webb, M. S. (2002). The stigma of being pessimistically biased. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 1, 92–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, V. R., Hennessy, M., Barrett, D. W., Curtis, B., McCoy-Roth, M., Trentacoste, N. et al. (2005). When risky is attractive: sensation seeking and romantic partner selection. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 311–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendrick, S. and Hendrick, C. (1987). Love and sex attitudes, self-disclosure and sensation-seeking. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 281–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertwig, R., Fenselow, C. and Hoffrage, U. (2003). Hindsight bias: knowledge and heuristics affect our reconstruction of the past. Memory, 11, 357–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, A. K. and Weiner, I. B. (eds.) (1999). Handbook of Forensic Psychology. New York: John Wiley, 2nd edition.
Higbee, K. L. (1972). Group risk taking in military decisions. Journal of Social Psychology, 88, 55–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Highhouse, S. and Paese, P. W. (1996). Problem domain and prospect frame: choice under opportunity versus threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 124–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillier, L. M. and Morrongiello, B. A. (1998). Age and gender differences in school-age children’s appraisals of injury risk. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 23, 4, 229–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Himiob-de-Marcano, M. (2000). Cuando el panico es de todos: como conservar la calma para ayudar a nuestros hermanos directamente traumatizados [When panic seizes everybody: how to remain calm to help our fellow-men directly traumatised]. Tropicos: Revista de Psicoanalisis, 8, 149–52.Google Scholar
Hinman, G. W., Rosa, E. A., Kleinhesselink, R. R. and Lowinger, T. C. (1993). Perceptions of nuclear and other risks in Japan and the United States. Risk Analysis, 13, 449–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hiraba, J., Bao, W. N., Lorenz, F. O. and Pechacova, Z. (1998). Perceived risk of crime in the Czech Republic. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 35, 225–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HMSO (2000). The BSE Inquiry. London: The Stationery Office.Google Scholar
HMSO (2013). Report of the Mid Staffordshire NHS Foundation Trust Public Inquiry. Chaired by Robert Francis. London: The Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Hoffrage, U., Hertwig, R. and Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Hindsight bias: a byproduct of knowledge updating?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 566–81.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A., Turner, J. C. and Davidson, B. (1990). Polarized norms and social frames of reference: a test of the self-categorization theory of group polarization. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogg, M. A. and Vaughan, G. M. (1998). Leadership and group decision-making. In Hogg, M. A. and Vaughan, G. M. (eds.), Social Psychology: An Introduction (pp. 300–6). London: Prentice-Hall, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Hohenemser, C. and Renn, O. (1988). Chernobyl’s other legacy: shifting public perceptions of nuclear risk. Environment, 30, 3, 4–11.Google Scholar
Hohl, K. and Gaskell, G. (2008). European public perceptions of food risk: cross-national and methodological comparisons. Risk Analysis, 28, 311–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzl, E. and Kirchler, E. (2005). Causal attribution and hindsight bias for economic developments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 167–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hongo, Jun (2012). Panel lays bare Fukushima recipe for disaster. Japan Times, 29 February, p. 1.Google Scholar
Horlick-Jones, T., Rosenhead, J., Georgiou, I., Ravetz, J. and Löfstedt, R. (2001). Decision support for organisational risk management by problem structuring. Health, Risk & Society, 3, 141–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornig, S. (1992). Framing risks: audience and reader factors. Journalism Quarterly, 69, 679–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornig Priest, S. (1999). The never-ending story of dioxin. In Friedman, S. M., Dunwoody, S. and Rogers, C. L. (eds.), Communicating Uncertainty: Media Coverage of New and Controversial Science (pp. 190–201). New York: LEA.Google Scholar
Horswill, M. S. and McKenna, F. P. (1999a). The effect of interference on dynamic risk taking judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 189–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horswill, M. S. and McKenna, F. P. (1999b). The effect of perceived control on risk taking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 29, 377–91.Google Scholar
Horvath, P. and Zuckerman, M. (1993). Sensation seeking, risk appraisal, and risky behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 41–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hossain, F. and Onyango, B. (2004). Product attributes and consumer acceptance of nutritionally enhanced genetically modified foods. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 28, 255–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houlding, C. and Davidson, R. (2003). Beliefs as predictors of condom use by injecting drug users in treatment. Health Education Research, 18, 145–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
House of Lords (2006). Government Policy on the Management of Risk. Select Committee on Economic Affairs, 5th Report of Session 2005–6 (June 2006).
House of Lords (2011). Behaviour Change (Report 179). London: The Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Howard, R. (1980). On making life and death decisions. In Richard, J., Schwing, C., and Albers, W. A. (eds.), Societal Risk Assessment: How Safe is Safe Enough? (pp. 89–99). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howarth, C. (2006). A social representation is not a quiet thing: exploring the critical potential of social representations theory. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45: 65–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howel, D., Moffatt, S., Prince, H., Bush, J. and Dunn, C. E. (2002). Urban air quality in north-east England: exploring the influences on local views and perceptions. Risk Analysis, 22, 121–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
HSE (1993). ACSNI Study Group on Human Factors. Third Report: Organising for Safety. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
HSE (2001). Reducing Risks, Protecting People: HSE’s Decision-Making Process. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Hsee, C. K. and Weber, E. U. (1999). Cross-national differences in risk preference and lay predictions. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 12, 2, 165–79.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, J. C. (1998). Self organise and survive: disabled people in the British trade union movement. Disability and Society, 13, 587–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunton, J. and Rose, J. M. (2005). Cellular telephones and driving performance: the effects of attentional demands on motor vehicle crash risk. Risk Analysis, 25, 855–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutter, B. M. (2001). Regulation and Risk: Occupational Health and Safety on the Railways. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imrie, R. (1999). The role of access groups in facilitating accessible environments for disabled people. Disability and Society, 14, 463–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
International Nuclear Safety Advisory Group (1991). Safety Series No 75-INSAG-4. Vienna: IAEA.Google Scholar
Irwin, W., Davidson, R. J., Lowe, M. J., Mock, B. J., Sorenson, J. A. and Turski, P. A. (1996). Human amygdala activation detected with echo-planar functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroReport, 7, 1765–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Group polarization: a critical review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1141–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, A. (2009). The pathologies of big data. ACMQueue, 6 July.
Janis, I. L. (1971). Groupthink. Psychology Today, 43–6, 344.Google Scholar
Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of Groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, R. and Frieze, I. H. (1983). A theoretical perspective for understanding reactions to victimization. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaspal, R. and Breakwell, G. M. (eds.) (2014). Identity Process Theory: Identity, Social Action and Social Change. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Jeffcott, S., Pidgeon, N., Weyman, A. and Walls, J. (2006). Risk, trust, and safety culture in the UK train operating companies. Risk Analysis, 26, 1105–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jianguang, Z. (1993). Environmental hazards in the Chinese public’s eyes. Risk Analysis, 13, 509–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joffe, H. (2003). Risk: from perception to social representation. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 55–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joffe, H. and Bettega, N. (2003). Social representations of AIDS among Zambian adolescents. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 616–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joffe, H. and Haarhoff, G. (2002). Representations of far-flung illnesses: the case of Ebola in Britain. Social Science and Medicine, 54, 955–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joffe, H. and Lee, L. (2004). Social representation of a food risk: the Hong Kong avian bird flu epidemic. Health Psychology, 9, 517–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, B. B. (2002a). Gender and race in beliefs about outdoor air pollution. Risk Analysis, 22, 725–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. B. (2004a). Risk comparisons, conflict, and risk acceptability claims. Risk Analysis, 24, 131–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. B. (2004b). Varying risk comparison elements: effects on public reactions. Risk Analysis, 24, 103–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. B. (2010). Trust and terrorism: citizen responses to anti-terrorism performance history. Risk Analysis, 30, 1328–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. B. (2012). Climate change communication: a provocative inquiry into motives, meaning and means. Risk Analysis, 32, 973–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, B. B. and Slovic, P. (1994). ‘Improving’ risk communication and risk management: legislated solutions or legislated disasters?Risk Analysis, 14, 905–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. B. and White, M. P. (2010). The importance of multiple performance criteria for understanding trust in risk managers. Risk Analysis, 30, 1099–115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Junger, S. (1997). The Perfect Storm: A True Story of Man Against the Sea. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Jungermann, H., Schutz, H. and Thuring, M. (1988). Mental models in risk assessment: informing people about drugs. Risk Analysis, 8, 147–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N. and Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science 312, 1908–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979a). Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures. TIMS Studies in Management Science, 12, 313–27.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979b). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (eds.) (2000). Choices, Values and Frames. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kalichman, S. C. (1994). Magic Johnson and public attitudes toward AIDS: a review of empirical findings. AIDS Education and Prevention, 6, 542–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Kallmen, H. (2000). Manifest anxiety, general self-efficacy and locus of control as determinants of personal and general risk perception. Journal of Risk Research, 3, 111–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaptan, G., Shiloh, S. and Önkal, D. (2013). Values and risk perceptions: a cross-cultural examination. Risk Analysis, 33: 318–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasperson, J. and Kasperson, R. E. (2001). Transboundary risks and social amplification. In Linnerooth-Bayer, J. and Löfstedt, R. (eds.), Cross-National Studies of Transboundary Risk Problems (pp. 207–43). London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Kasperson, J., Kasperson, R. E., Pidgeon, N. and Slovic, P. (2003). The Social Amplification of Risk: Assessing 15 Years of Research and Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kasperson, J., Kasperson, R. E. and Turner, B. L. (1995). Regions at Risk: Comparisons of Threatened Environments. Tokyo: United Nations University.Google Scholar
Kasperson, R. E. (1992). The social amplification of risk: progress in developing an integrative framework of risk. In Krimsky, S. and Golding, D. (eds.), Social Theories of Risk (pp. 119–32). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kasperson, R. E., Golding, D. and Tuler, S. (1992). Social distrust as a factor in siting hazardous facilities and communicating risks. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 161–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasperson, R. E., Jhaveri, N. and Kasperson, J. (2001). Stigma, Places, and the Social Amplification of Risk: Toward a Framework of Analysis. London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Kasperson, R. E. and Kasperson, J. (1991). Hidden hazards. In Mayo, D. G. and Hollander, R. D. (eds.), Acceptable Evidence: Science and Values in Risk Management (pp. 9–28). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kasperson, R. E. and Kasperson, J. (1996). The social amplification and attenuation of risk. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 545, 95–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasperson, R. E., Renn, O., Slovic, P., Brown, H., Emel, J., Goble, R. et al. (1988). The social amplification of risk: a conceptual framework. Risk Analysis, 8, 177–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kathlene, L. and Martin, J. A. (1991). Enhancing citizen participation: panel designs, perspectives, and policy formation. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 10, 46–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katsuya, T. (2001). Public response to the Tokai nuclear accident. Risk Analysis, 21, 1039–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, E. and Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal Influence: The Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communications. Foundations of Communications Research, 2. A report of the Bureau of Applied Social Research, Columbia University. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.Google Scholar
Kellens, W., Zaalberg, R., Neutens, T., Vanneuville, W. and De Maeyer, P. (2011). An analysis of the public perception of flood risk on the Belgian coast. Risk Analysis, 31, 1055–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, C., Siegrist, M. and Gutscher, H. (2006). The role of the affect and availability heuristics in risk communication. Risk Analysis, 26, 971–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, C., Visschers, V. and Siegrist, M. (2012). Affective imagery and acceptance of replacing nuclear power plants. Risk Analysis, 32, 464–477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. A. (1999). Converting the unconverted: the effect of inclination and opportunity to discount health-related fear appeals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 403–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. In Jones, E. E., Kanouse, D. E., Kelley, H. H., Nisbett, R. E., Valmis, S. and Weiner, B. (eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behaviour (pp. 41–58). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Kellstedt, P. M., Zahran, S. and Vedlitz, A. (2008). Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Analysis, 28: 113–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keown, C. F. (1989). Risk perceptions of Hong Kongese vs. Americans. Risk Analysis, 9, 401–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kida, T. E. (2006). Don’t Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Kitchin, R. (2000). The researched opinions on research: disabled people and disability research. Disability and Society, 15, 25–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klar, Y. and Ayal, S. (2004). Event frequency and comparative optimism: another look at the indirect elicitation method of self-others risks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 805–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, C. T. F. and Helweg-Larsen, M. (2002). Perceived control and the optimistic bias: a meta-analytic review. Psychology and Health 17, 437–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinhesselink, R. R. (1992). Risk perceptions, risk regulatory motivations, and personality: US/Japan comparisons. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 308–11.Google Scholar
Kleinhesselink, R. R. and Rosa, E. A. (1991). Cognitive representation of risk perceptions: a comparison of Japan and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 222, 11–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kogan, N. and Wallach, M. A. (1967). Risky-shift phenomenon in small decision-making groups: a test of the information-exchange hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 75–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovacs, D. C., Fischhoff, B. and Small, M. J. (2001). Perceptions of PCE use by dry cleaners and dry cleaning customers. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 353–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, N., Malmfors, T. and Slovic, P. (1992). Intuitive toxicology: expert and lay judgments of chemical risks. Risk Analysis, 12, 215–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, N. and Slovic, P. (1988). Taxonomic analysis of perceived risk: modeling individual and group perceptions within homogeneous hazard domains. Risk Analysis, 8, 435–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krewski, D., Slovic, P., Bartlett, S., Flynn, J. and Mertz, C. K. (1995a). Health risk perception in Canada I: rating hazards, sources of information and responsibility for health protection. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 2, 117–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krewski, D., Slovic, P., Bartlett, S., Flynn, J. and Mertz, C. K. (1995b). Health risk perception in Canada II: worldviews, attitudes and opinions. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 2, 231–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kung, Y.-W. and Chen, S.-H. (2012). Perception of earthquake risk in Taiwan: effects of gender and past earthquake experience. Risk Analysis, 32, 1535–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunreuther, H., Easterling, D., Desvousges, W. and Slovic, P. (1990). Public attitudes toward siting a high-level nuclear waste repository in Nevada. Risk Analysis, 10, 469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuttschreuter, M. and Gutteling, J. M. (2004). Experience based processing of risk information: the case of the millennium bug. Journal of Risk Research, 7, 3–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachlan, K. and Spence, P. R. (2010). Communicating risks: examining hazard and outrage in multiple contexts. Risk Analysis, 30, 1872–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laforet, S. and Saunders, J. (2005). Managing brand portfolios: how strategies have changed. Journal of Advertising Research, 45, 314–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, J.-M., Hwang, Y.-T. and Chou, C.-C. (2012). Modeling exotic highly pathogenic avian influenza virus entrance risk through air passenger violations. Risk Analysis, 32, 1093–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, F. N. (1989). The decline of deference: the political context of risk communication. Risk Analysis, 9, 543–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, J. and Meeker, J. W. (2003). Ethnicity, information sources, and fear of crime. Deviant Behaviour, 24, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, J. T. and Hallman, W. K. (2005). Who does the public trust? The case of genetically modified food in the United States. Risk Analysis, 25, 1241–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langford, I. H., Georgiou, S., Day, R. J. and Bateman, I. J. (1999). Comparing perceptions of risk with willingness to pay. Risk Decision and Policy, 4, 201–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larrain, P. and Simpson-Housley, P. (1990). Geophysical variables and behaviour, Lonquimay and Alhme, Chile: tension from volcanic and earthquake hazard. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 296–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, J. R., Foster-Fishman, P. G. and Keys, C. B. (1994). Discussion of shared and unshared information in decision-making groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 446–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavery, B., Siegel, A. W., Cousins, J. H. and Rubovits, D. S. (1993). Adolescent risk-taking: an analysis of problem behaviours in problem children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 277–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazo, J. K., Kinnell, J. C. and Fisher, A. (2000). Expert and layperson perceptions of ecosystem risk. Risk Analysis, 20, 179–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K. L., Meyer, R. J. and Bradlow, E. T. (2009). Analyzing risk response dynamics on the web: the case of Hurricane Katrina. Risk Analysis, 29, 1779–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T. R. (1993). Seeking a safety culture: psychological aspects of safety in the nuclear industry. Atom, 429, 20–3.Google Scholar
Lee, T. R. (1995a). The role of attitudes in the safety culture. Paper presented at the Conference of the American Nuclear Society, International Topical Meeting, Washington.
Lee, T. R. (1995b). The roles of attitudes in the safety culture and how to change them. Paper presented to the Conference on Understanding of Risk Perception, Aberdeen.
Lee, T. R. (1995c). Social and psychological consequences of the Chernobyl accident: an overview of the first decade. Paper presented at the International Conference on Health Consequences of the Chernobyl and other Radiological Accidents, Geneva.
Lee, T. R., Macdonald, S. M. and Coote, J. A. (1993). Perceptions of risk and attitudes to safety at a nuclear reprocessing plant. Paper presented to the Society for Risk Assessment (Europe) Fourth Conference, Rome.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1982). Locus of Control: Current Trends in Theory and Research. Hillsdale, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Lehrer, J. (2009). How We Decide. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Leiserowitz, A. A. (2005). American risk perceptions: is climate change dangerous?Risk Analysis, 25, 1433–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lek, Y. and Bishop, G. D. (1995). Perceived vulnerability to illness threats. Psychology and Health, 10, 205–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. S. and Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leventhal, H. (1970). Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. V (pp. 119–86). London and New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H. (1984). A perceptual-motor theory of emotion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 117–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, I. P. and Gaeth, G. J. (1988). Framing of attributes information before and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 374–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L. and Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, I. P., Schnittjer, S. K. and Thee, S. L. (1988). Information framing effects in social and personal decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 122, 173–81.Google Scholar
Levin-Coburn, (2011). Senate Financial Crisis Report, 2011. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Levine, J. M. and Moreland, R. L. (1999). Knowledge transmission in work groups: helping newcomers to succeed. In Thompson, L. L., Levine, J. M. and Merrick, D. M. (eds.), Shared Cognition in Organisations: The Management of Knowledge (pp. 267–96). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Levy-Leboyer, C. (1988). Success and failure in applying psychology. American Psychologist, 43, 779–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, R. E. and Tyshenko, M. G. (2009). The impact of social amplification and attenuation of risk and the public reaction to mad cow disease in Canada. Risk Analysis, 29, 714–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M. and Combs, B. (1978). Judged frequency and lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 551–78.Google Scholar
Lima, M. L. (2004). On the influence of risk perception on mental health: living near an incinerator. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 71–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lima, M. L., Barnett, J. and Vala, J. (2005). Risk perception and technological development at a societal level. Risk Analysis, 25, 1229–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, F., He, D., Jin, Y., Tao, Y. and Jiang, Z. (2013). Mapping the central structure core in social representation of pain. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 1931–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, X.-W., Chiang, C.-T., Shih, T.-H., Jiang, Y.-N. and Chou, C.-C. (2009). Foot-and-mouth disease entrance assessment model through air passenger violations. Risk Analysis, 29, 601–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindell, M. K. and Earle, T. C. (1983). How close is close enough: public perception of the risks of industrial facilities. Risk Analysis, 3, 245–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindell, M. K. and Perry, R. W. (1990). Effects of the Chernobyl accident on public perceptions of nuclear plant accident risks. Risk Analysis, 10, 393–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindhjem, H., Navrud, S., Braathen, N. A. and Biausque, V. (2011). Valuing mortality risk reductions from environmental, transport, and health policies: a global meta-analysis of stated preference studies. Risk Analysis, 31, 1381–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linville, P. W., Fisher, G. W. and Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk perceptions and decision biases. In Pryor, J. B. and Reeder, G. D. (eds.), The Social Psychology of HIV Infection (pp. 5–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Livet, P. (2010). Rational choice, neuroeconomy and mixed emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 265, 259–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löfstedt, R. (2013). Communicating food risks in an era of growing public distrust: three case studies. Risk Analysis, 33, 192–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowenstein, G. and Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In Davidson, R., Scherer, K. and Goldsmith, H. (eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 619–42). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lowenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K. and Wells, G. L. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowrance, W. W. (1976). Of Acceptable Risk: Science and the Determination of Safety. Los Altos, CA: William Kaufman.Google Scholar
Lu, H., Plataniotis, K. N. and Venetsanopoulos, A. N. (2011). A survey of multilinear subspace learning for tensor data. Pattern Recognition, 44, 1540–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundgren, R. E. and McMakin, A. H. (eds.) (2013). Risk Communication. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley, 5th edition.CrossRef
Lyons, E. (1996). Changing European identities: social psychological analyses of social change. In Breakwell, G. M. and Lyons, E. (eds.), Coping with Social Change: Processes of Social Memory in the Reconstruction of Identities (pp. 31–9). Chichester: Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
Maass, A. and Clark, R. D. (1984). Hidden impact of minorities: fifteen years of minority influence research. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 428–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacGregor, D., Slovic, P., Mason, R. G. and Detweiler, J. (1994). Perceived risks of radioactive waste transport through Oregon: results of a statewide survey. Risk Analysis, 14, 5–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machlis, G. E. and Rosa, E. A. (1990). Desired risk: broadening the social amplification of risk framework. Risk Analysis, 10, 161–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macintyre, K., Rutenberg, N., Brown, L. and Karim, A. (2004). Understanding perceptions of HIV risk among adolescents in KwaZulu Natal. AIDS and Behaviour, 8, 237–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddux, J. E. and Rogers, R. W. (1983). Protection motivation and self-efficacy: a revised theory of four appeals and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 469–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, M. J. (1977). Publication prejudices: an experimental study of confirmatory bias in the peer review system. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 161–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manyika, J., Chui, M., Bughin, J., Brown, B., Dobbs, R., Roxburgh, C. and Byers, A. H. (2011). Big Data: The Next Frontier for Innovation, Competition, and Productivity. McKinsey Global Institute.Google Scholar
Marks, A. Z. and Arkes, H. R. (2010). The effects of mental contamination on the hindsight bias: source confusion determines success in disregarding knowledge. Journal of Behavioral Decision-Making, 23, 131–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marquis, D. G. (1962). Individual responsibility and group decisions involving risk. Industrial Management Review, 3, 8–23.Google Scholar
Marris, C., Langford, I. H. and O’Riordan, T. (1998). A quantitative test of the cultural theory of risk perceptions: comparisons with the psychometric paradigm. Risk Analysis, 18, 635–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, J. R. and Garvin, T. (2006). Place, culture, and the social amplification of risk. Risk Analysis, 26, 437–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, T., Shiomi, T. and Nakayachi, K. (2005). Evaluation of risk communication from the perspective of the information source: focusing on public relations officers for nuclear power generation. Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 201–7.Google Scholar
Mattila, M., Rantanen, E. and Hyttinen, M. (1994). The quality of work environment, supervision and safety in building construction. Safety Science, 17, 257–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazur, A. (1990). Nuclear power, chemical hazards, and the quantity of reporting. Minerva, 28, 294–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArdle, S. C., Rosoff, H. and John, R. S. (2012). The dynamics of evolving beliefs, concerns emotions, and behavioral avoidance following 9/11: a longitudinal analysis of representative archival samples. Risk Analysis, 32, 744–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McComas, K. A. (2003). Public meetings and risk amplification: a longitudinal study. Risk Analysis, 23, 1257–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R. and Terracino, A. (2005). Members of the personality profiles of cultures project (2005) universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniels, T. L., Axelrod, L. J., Cavanagh, N. S. and Slovic, P. (1997). Perception of ecological risk to water environments. Risk Analysis, 17, 341–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniels, T. L., Gregory, R. S. and Fields, D. (1999). Democratizing risk management: successful public involvement in local water management decisions. Risk Analysis, 19, 497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, N. and Ryan, F. (1992). Constraints on the development of safety culture: a preliminary analysis. Irish Journal of Psychology, 13, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W. (1918). An Introduction to Social Psychology. Boston: J. W. Luce and Co.Google Scholar
McKenna, F. P. (1985). Do safety measures really work? An examination of risk homoeostasis theory. Ergonomics, 28, 489–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, F. P. (1988). What role should the concept of risk play in theories of accident involvement?Ergonomics, 31, 469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, F. P. and Albery, I. P. (2001). Does unrealistic optimism change following a negative experience?Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 1146–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, F. P. and Horswill, M. S. (2006). Risk taking from the participant’s perspective: the case of driving and accident risk. Health Psychology, 25, 163–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, F. P. and Myers, L. B. (1997). Illusory self-assessments: can they be reduced?British Journal of Psychology, 88, 39–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, F. P., Warburton, D. M. and Winwood, M. (1993). Exploring the limits of optimism: the case of smokers’ decision making. British Journal of Psychology, 84, 389–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMath, B. F. and Prentice-Dunn, S. (2005). Protection motivation theory and skin cancer risk: the role of individual differences in responses to persuasive appeals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 631–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, I. M., Dunlop, P. D., Heath, J. B., Skinner, T. C. and Morrison, D. L. (2013). Expecting the unexpected: predicting physiological and psychological wildfire preparedness from perceived risk, responsibility, and obstacles. Risk Analysis, 33, 1829–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Mearns, K., Whitaker, S. M. and Flin, R. (2001). Benchmarking safety climate in hazardous environments: a longitudinal, interorganizational approach. Risk Analysis, 21, 771–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mechitov, A. I. and Rebrick, S. B. (1990). Studies of risk and safety perception in the USSR. In Borcherding, D. I., Larichev, D. I. and Messick, D. M. (eds.), Contemporary Issues in Decision Making (pp. 261–70). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Mehta, M. D. and Simpson-Housley, P. (1994). Perception of potential nuclear disaster: the relation of likelihood and consequence estimates of risk. Perception and Motor Skills, 79, 1119–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meijnders, A., Midden, C., Olofsson, A., Öhman, S., Matthes, J., Bondarenko, O., Gutteling, J. and Rusanen, M. (2009). The role of similarity cues in the development of trust in sources of information about GM food. Risk Analysis, 29, 1116–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merrick, J. and Parnell, G. S. (2011). A comparative analysis of PRA and intelligent adversary methods for counterterrorism risk management. Risk Analysis, 31, 1488–510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Middleton, W., Harris, P. and Surman, M. (1996). Give ’em enough rope: perception of health and safety risks in bungee jumpers. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 68–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, S. and Frewer, L. J. (2001). Investigating specific concerns about different food hazards: higher and lower order attributes. Food Quality and Preference, 12, 47–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, S. and Rowe, G. (2004). The laddering technique. In Breakwell, G. M. (ed.), Doing Social Psychology Research (pp. 305–43). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Millar, M. G. and Millar, K. U. (1996). The effects of direct and indirect experience on affective and cognitive responses and the attitude-behaviour relation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 561–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, B. and Sinclair, J. (2012). Risk perceptions in a resource community and communication implications: emotion, stigma, and identity. Risk Analysis, 32, 483–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., Zimmerman, R. S., Logan, T. K., Leukefeld, C. and Clayton, R. (2004). The utility of the Five Factor Model in understanding risky sexual behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1611–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, R. L. and Mulligan, R. D. (2002). Terror management: the effects of mortality salience and locus of control on risk-taking behaviours. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1203–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minsky, H. P. (1975). John Maynard Keynes. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, S. C. and Savage, I. (2005). Why has safety improved at rail-highway grade crossings?Risk Analysis, 25, 867–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moliner, P. and Tafani, E. (1997). Attitudes and social representations: a theoretical and experimental approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 687–702.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moloney, G., Hall, R. and Walker, I. (2005). Social representations and themata: the construction and functioning of social knowledge about donation and transplantation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 415–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, S. M. and Rosenthal, D. A. (1993). Venturesomeness, impulsiveness, and risky behaviour among older adolescents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 76–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moray, N. (1994). Human factors in waste management. Radwaste Magazine, October, 58–63.Google Scholar
Morgan, K. M., DeKay, M. L., Fischbeck, P. S., Granger Morgan, M., Fischhoff, B. and Florig, H. K. (2001). A deliberative method for ranking risks (II): evaluation of validity and agreement among risk managers. Risk Analysis, 21, 923–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A. and Atman, C. J. (2002). Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, S. E. (2009). The intersection of conversation, cognitions, and campaigns: the social representation of organ donation. Communication Theory, 19, 29–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovici, S. (1976). Social Influence and Social Change. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Moscovici, S. (1988). Notes towards a description of social representations. Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 211–50.Google Scholar
Moscovici, S. and Farr, R. (eds.) (1984). Social Representations. Cambridge University Press.
Moscovici, S., Mugny, G. and van Avermaet, E. (1985). Perspectives on Minority Influence. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovici, S. and Zavalloni, M. (1969). The group as a polarizer of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 125–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motoyoshi, T., Takao, K. and Ikeda, S. (2004). Determinant factors of residents’ acceptance of flood risk. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 58–67.Google Scholar
Muller, S. and Johnson, B. T. (1990). Fear and persuasion: a linear relationship? Paper presented to the Eastern Psychological Association Convention, New York.
Murch, S. (2003). Separating inflammation from speculation in autism. The Lancet, 362, 1498–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musch, J. (2003). Personality differences in hindsight bias. Memory, 11, 473–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mv Herald of Free Enterprise (1987). Report of Court No. 8074. Chapter 14. London: HMSO.
Myers, D. G. and Lamm, H. (1976). The group polarization phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 602–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. R., Henderson-King, D. H. and Henderson-King, E. I. (1997). Facing technological risks: the importance of individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, S. and Nix, C. L. (1989). Relations between preventative health behaviour and hardiness. Psychological Reports, 65, 339–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakayachi, K. (2013). The unintended effects of risk-refuting information on anxiety. Risk Analysis, 33, 80–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakayachi, K. and Cvetkovich, G. (2010). Public trust in government concerning tobacco control in Japan. Risk Analysis, 30, 143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Commission on the BP Deepwater Horizon Oil Spill and Offshore Drilling (2011). Deep Water: The Gulf Oil Disaster and the Future of Offshore Drilling. Washington, DC: US Government.Google Scholar
Navas, E. (2002). Problems associated with potential massive use of antimicrobial agents as prophylaxis or therapy of a bioterrorist attack. Clinical Microbiology and Infection, 8, 534–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevid, J. S. and Maria, N. L. S. (1999). Multicultural issues in qualitative research. Psychology & Marketing, 16, 305–25.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. and Ross, L. (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Nordhøy, F. (1962). Group Interaction in Decision-Making Under Risk. School of Industrial Management, MIT.Google Scholar
Nyland, L. G. (1993). Risk Perception in Brazil and Sweden. RHIZIKON: Risk Research Report 15.
O’Connor, R. E., Yarnal, B., Dow, K., Jocoy, C. L. and Carbone, G. J. (2005). Feeling at risk matters: water managers and the decision to use forecasts. Risk Analysis, 25, 1265–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oechssler, J., Roider, S. and Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioural biases. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 147–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olofsson, A. and Rashid, S. (2011). The white (male) effect and risk perception: can equality make a difference?Risk Analysis, 31, 1016–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, O. (2002). A Question of Trust. BBC Reith Lectures. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ostrom, L., Wilhelmsen, C. and Kaplan, B. (1993). Assessing safety culture. Nuclear Safety, 34, 163–72.Google Scholar
Otten, W. and van der Pligt, J. (1996). Context effect in the measurement of comparative optimism in probability judgements. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 80–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otway, H., Haastrup, P., Cannell, W., Gianitsopoulos, G. and Paruccini, M. (1988). Risk communication in Europe after Chernobyl: a media analysis of seven countries. Industrial Crisis Quarterly, 2, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paltrinieri, N., Dechy, N., Salzano, E., Wardman, M. and Cozzani, V. (2012). Lessons learned from Toulouse and Buncefield disasters: from risk analysis failures to the identification of atypical scenarios through a better knowledge management. Risk Analysis, 32, 1404–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parnell, G. S., Smith, C. M. and Moxley, F. I. (2010). Intelligent adversary risk analysis: a bioterrorism risk management model. Risk Analysis, 30, 32–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parry, S. M., Miles, S., Tridente, A. and Palmer, S. R. (2004). Differences in perception of risk between people who have and have not experienced Salmonella food poisoning. Risk Analysis, 24, 289–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pastel, R. H. (2001). Collective behaviours: mass panic and outbreaks of multiple unexplained symptoms. Military Medicine, 166, 44–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paté-Cornell, E. (2012). On ‘black swans’ and ‘perfect storms’: risk analysis and management when statistics are not enough. Risk Analysis, 32, 1823–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pease, M. E., McCabe, A. E., Brannon, L. A. and Tagler, M. J. (2003). Memory distortions for pre Y2K expectancies: a demonstration of the hindsight bias. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 137, 397–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira-Henriques, A. M. and Lima, M. L. (2003). Estados afectivos, percepcao do risco e do suporte social: a familiaridade e a relevancia como moderadores nas respostas de congruencia com o estado de espirito [Affective states, perception of risk and of social support: familiarity and relevance as moderators in responses of congruency with mood states]. Analise Psicologica, 21, 375–92.Google Scholar
Perko, T., van Gorp, B., Turcanu, C., Thijssen, P. and Carle, B. (2013). Communication in nuclear emergency preparedness: a closer look at information reception. Risk Analysis, 33, 1987–2001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perloff, L. S. and Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self–other judgements and perceived vulnerability to victimisation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 502–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E., Burraston, B. and Mertz, C. K. (2004). An emotion based model of risk perception and stigma susceptibility: cognitive appraisals of emotion, affective reactivity, worldviews, and risk perceptions in the generation of technological stigma. Risk Analysis, 24, 1349–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, E., Finucane, M., MacGregor, D. and Slovic, P. (2000). The unbearable lightness of aging: judgment and decision processes in older adults. In Stern, P. C. and Carstensen, L. L. (eds.), The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research (pp. 144–65). Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Peters, E., Kunreuther, H., Sagara, N., Slovic, P. and Schley, D. R. (2012). Protective measures, personal experience, and the affective psychology of time. Risk Analysis, 32, 2084–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E. and Slovic, P. (1996). The role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions in the perception and acceptance of nuclear power. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1427–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E., Slovic, P., Hibbard, J. H. and Tusler, M. (2006a). Why worry? Worry, risk perceptions, and willingness to act to reduce medical errors. Health Psychology, 25, 144–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, E., Vastfjall, D., Garling, T. and Slovic, P. (2006b). Affect and decision making: a ‘hot’ topic. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 19, 79–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, R. G., Covello, V. T. and McCallum, D. B. (1997). The determinants of trust and credibility in environmental risk communication: an empirical study. Risk Analysis, 17, 43–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, D. (1993). Establishing good ‘safety culture’ helps mitigate workplace dangers. Occupational Health & Safety, 62, 20–4.Google Scholar
Peterson, M. (2006). The precautionary principle is incoherent. Risk Analysis, 26, 595–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson-Strömbäck, A. E., Liljelind, I. E., Nordin, S. and Järvholm, B. (2010). Workers’ mental models of chemical exposure in the workplace. Risk Analysis, 30, 488–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, R. E. and Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communications and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pezzo, M. V. (2003). What removes hindsight bias?Memory, 11, 421–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phimister, J. R., Oktem, U., Kleindorfer, P. R. and Kunreuther, H. (2003). Near-miss incident management in the chemical process industry. Risk Analysis, 23, 445–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PHLS (2000). AIDS/HIV Quarterly Surveillance Tables: UK Data to End September 1999. Report 44: 99/3. PHLS.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). La Construction du réel chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachanx et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Pidgeon, N. F. (1991). Safety culture and risk management in organizations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, special issue: Risk and Culture, 22, 129–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pidgeon, N., Hood, C., Jones, D., Turner, B. and Gibson, R. (1992). Risk perception. In Royal Society (ed.), Risk: Analysis, Perception and Management (pp. 56–72). London: Royal Society.Google Scholar
Pidgeon, N., Harthorn, B. and Satterfield, T. (2011). Nanotechnology risk perceptions and communication: emerging technologies, emerging challenges. Risk Analysis, 31, 1694–1700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pidgeon, N., Kasperson, R. E. and Slovic, P. (eds.) (2003). The Social Amplification of Risk. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Pidgeon, N. F. and Turner, B. A. (1997). Man-Made Disasters. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
Pillisuk, M. and Acredolo, C. (1988). Fear of technological hazards: one concern or many?Social Behaviour, 3, 17–24.Google Scholar
Pillisuk, M., Parks, S. H. and Hawkes, G. (1987). Public perception of technological risk. Social Science Journal, 24, 403–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohl, R. F., Bender, M. and Lachmann, G. (2002). Hindsight bias around the world. Experimental Psychology, 49, 270–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pohl, R. F., Eisenhauer, M. and Hardt, O. (2003). SARA: a cognitive process model to stimulate the anchoring effect and hindsight bias. Memory, 11, 337–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohl, R. F. and Hell, W. (1996). No reduction in hindsight bias after complete information and repeated testing. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 67, 49–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poortinga, W., Cox, P. and Pidgeon, N. F. (2008). The perceived health risks of indoor radon gas and overhead powerlines: a comparative multilevel approach. Risk Analysis, 28, 235–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poortinga, W. and Pidgeon, N. F. (2003). Exploring the dimensionality of trust in risk regulation. Risk Analysis, 23, 961–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poortinga, W. and Pidgeon, N. F. (2004). Trust, the asymmetry principle, and the role of prior beliefs. Risk Analysis, 24, 1475–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poortinga, W. and Pidgeon, N. F. (2005). Trust in risk regulation: cause or consequence of the acceptability of GM food?Risk Analysis, 25, 199–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poumadère, M., Mays, C., Le Mer, S. and Blong, R. (2005). The 2003 heat wave in France: dangerous climate change here and now. Risk Analysis, 25, 148–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priest, S., Lane, T., Greenhalgh, T., Hand, L. J. and Kramer, V. (2011). Envisioning emerging nanotechnologies: a three-year panel study of South Carolina citizens. Risk Analysis, 31, 1718–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulcino, T., Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., Foley, M. and Vlahov, D. (2003). Posttraumatic stress in women after the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. Journal of Women’s Health, 12, 809–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purvis-Roberts, K. L., Werner, C. A. and Frank, I. (2007). Perceived risks from radiation and nuclear testing near Semipalatinsk, Kazakhstan: a comparison between physicians, scientists, and the public. Risk Analysis, 27, 291–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raats, M., Sparks, P., Geekie, M. A. and Shepherd, R. (1999). The effects of providing personalized dietary feedback: a semi-computerized approach. Patient Education and Counseling, 37, 177–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabinovich, A. and Morton, T. A. (2012). Unquestioned answers or unanswered questions: beliefs about science guide responses to uncertainty in climate change risk communication. Risk Analysis, 32, 992–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, S. (1988). Muddling through metaphors to maturity: a commentary on Kasperson et al. The Social Amplification of Risk. Risk Analysis, 8, 201–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, S. and Cantor, R. (1987). How fair is safe enough? The cultural approach to societal technology choice. Risk Analysis, 7, 3–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, D. and Morgan, M. (1998). The efficacy of different methods for informing the public about the range dependency of magnetic fields from high voltage power lines. Risk Analysis, 18, 603–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reason, J. (1987). The Chernobyl errors. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 40, 201–6.Google Scholar
Reason, J. (1990). Human Error. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reason, J. (1995). A system approach to organizational error. Ergonomics, 38, 1708–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reason, J. (1998). How necessary steps in a task get omitted: reviving old ideas to combat a persistent problem. International Journal of Cognitive Technology, 3, 24–32.Google Scholar
Reason, J. (2000a). A cognitive engineering perspective on maintenance errors. In Amalberti, R. and Sarter, N. (eds.), Cognitive Engineering in the Aviation Domain (pp 309–25). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Reason, J. (2000b). The Freudian slip revisited. Psychologist, 13, 610–11.Google Scholar
Reason, J., Parker, D. and Free, R. (1994). Bending the Rules: The Varieties, Origins and Management of Safety Violations. Leiden: Rijks Universiteit Leiden.Google Scholar
Redmond, E. C. and Griffith, C. J. (2004). Consumer perceptions of food safety risk, control and responsibility. Appetite, 43, 309–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renn, O. (1990). Public responses to the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 10, 151–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renn, O. (1991). Risk communication and the social amplification of risk. In Kasperson, J. and Stallen, P.-J. M. (eds.), Communicating Risks to the Public: International Perspectives (pp. 287–324). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renn, O. (2011). The social amplification/attenuation of risk framework: application to climate change. WIREs Climate Change, 2: 154–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renn, O. and Levine, D. (1991). Credibility and trust in risk communication. In Kasperson, J. and Stallen, P.-J. M. (eds.), Communicating Risks to the Public: International Perspectives (pp. 157–218). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.Google Scholar
Renn, O., Webler, T. and Johnson, B. (1991). Public participation in hazard management: the use of citizen panels in the US. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 2, 197–226.Google Scholar
Renner, B. (2003). Hindsight bias after receiving self-relevant health risk information: a motivational perspective. Memory, 11, 455–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richard, D. E. and Peterson, S. J. (1998). Perception of environmental risk related to gender, community socioeconomic setting, age and locus of control. Journal of Environmental Education, 30, 11–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J. and De Vries, N. (1996). Anticipated regret and time perspective: changing sexual risk-taking behaviour. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 9, 185–99.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, B., Sorenson, J. and Soderstrom, E. J. (1987). Explaining the social and psychological impacts of a nuclear power plant accident. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 16–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, K. (2001). Risk news in the world of Internet newsgroups. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 5, 50–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridley, D., Young, P. D. and Johnson, D. E. (1981). Salience as a dimension of individual and group risk taking. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 109, 283–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimal, R. N. and Real, K. (2003). Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change. Human Communication Research, 29, 370–99.Google Scholar
Rios, J. and Insua, D. R. (2012). Adversarial risk analysis for counterterrorism modeling. Risk Analysis, 32, 894–915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rip, A. (1988). Should social amplification of risk be counteracted?Risk Analysis, 8, 193–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivers, L., Arvai, J. and Slovic, P. (2010). Beyond a simple case of black and white: searching for the white male effect in the African-American community. Risk Analysis, 30, 65–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, J. (1999). Trying to get it right: undertaking research involving people with learning difficulties. Disability and Society, 14, 421–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roese, N. J. and Vohs, K. D. (2012). Hindsight bias. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 411–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roeser, S. (2012). Risk communication, public engagement, and climate change: a role for emotions. Risk Analysis, 32, 1033–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 91, 93–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrmann, B. (1991). A survey of social-scientific research on risk perception. Studies on Risk Communication, 26, 1–56.Google Scholar
Romer, D., Jamieson, K. H. and Aday, S. (2003). Television news and the cultivation of fear of crime. Journal of Communication, 53, 88–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosa, E. A. (1998). Metatheoretical foundations for post-normal risk. Journal of Risk Research, 1, 15–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosa, E. A. (2003). The logical structure of the social amplification of risk framework: metatheoretical foundations and policy implications. In Royal Society (ed.), Risk: Analysis, Perception and Management. London: Royal Society.Google Scholar
Rosenbloom, T. (2003). Sensation seeking and risk taking in mortality salience. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1809–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosoff, H., John, R. S. and Prager, F. (2012). Flu, risks, and videotape: escalating fear and avoidance. Risk Analysis, 32, 729–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothman, A. J., Klein, W. M. and Weinstein, N. D. (1996). Absolute and relative biases in estimations of personal risk. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1213–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothschild, C., McLay, L. and Guikema, S. (2012). Adversarial risk analysis with incomplete information: a level-k approach. Risk Analysis, 32, 1219–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rottenstreich, Y. H. and Hsee, C. K. (2001). Money, kisses and electric shocks: on the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12, 185–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. (1966). Generalised expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80.Google Scholar
Rowe, G. and Frewer, L. J. (2000). Public participation methods: a framework for evaluation. Science Technology & Human Value, 25, 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, G. and Wright, G. (2001). Differences in expert and lay judgments of risk: myth or reality?Risk Analysis, 21, 341–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royal Society (ed.) (1992). Risk: Analysis, Perception and Management. London: Royal Society.
Royal Society (ed.) (2006). Pandemic Influenza: Science to Policy. London: Royal Society.
Rudiger, F. (2007). Ways to assess hindsight bias. Social Cognition, 25, 14–31.Google Scholar
Rundmo, T. (1994). Associations between organisational factors and safety and contingency measures on offshore petroleum platforms. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 20, 122–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundmo, T. (2002). Associations between affect and risk perception. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 119–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, C. T., Park, H. S. and Wrigley, B. J. (2003). Optimistic bias and perceptions of bioterrorism in Michigan corporate spokespersons. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 130–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandin, P. (1999). Dimensions of the precautionary principle. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 5, 889–907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandin, P., Peterson, M., Hanson, S. O., Ruden, C. and Juthe, A. (2002). Science and the precautionary principle. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 53–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandman, P. (1989). Hazard versus outrage in the public perception of risk. In Covello, V. T., McCallum, D. B. and Pavlova, M. T. (eds.), Effective Risk Communication (pp. 45–9). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanquist, T. F., Mahy, H. and Morris, F. (2008). An exploratory risk perception study of attitudes toward homeland security systems. Risk Analysis, 28, 1125–33.Google ScholarPubMed
Sarita, Y. and Boyd, D. (2010). Dynamic debates: an analysis of group polarization over time on Twitter. Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society, 30, 316–27.Google Scholar
Satterfield, T. A., Mertz, C. K. and Slovic, P. (2004). Discrimination, vulnerability, and justice in the face of risk. Risk Analysis, 24, 115–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saucier, G., Tsaousis, I., Georgiades, S. and Goldberg, L. R. (2005). The factor structure of Greek personality adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 856–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, I. (1993). Demographic influences on risk perceptions. Risk Analysis, 13, 4, 413–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schank, R. C. and Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Chichester: Wiley (Halsted Press).Google Scholar
Schapira, M. M., Nattinger, A. B. and McHorney, C. A. (2001). Frequency or probability? A qualitative study of risk communications formats used in healthcare. Medical Decision Making, 21, 459–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, F. N. and Gifford, R. (1989). A dispositional approach to hazard perception: preliminary development of the environmental appraisal inventory. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9, 57–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, M. R. and Wei, W. (2006). Loss of agro-biodiversity: uncertainty and perceived control: a comparative risk perception study in Austria and China. Risk Analysis, 26, 455–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitz, P. G. (2004). On the alternative five-factor model: structure and correlates. In Stelmack, R. M. (ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 65–87). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulze, W. and Wansink, B. (2012). Toxics, Toyotas, and terrorism: the behavioral economics of fear and stigma. Risk Analysis, 32, 678–694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumacher, J. and Roth, M. (2004). Sensation seeking, gesundheitsbezogene Kognitionen und Partizipation am Risikosport [Sensation seeking, health-related cognitions and participation in high-risk sports]. Zeitschrift für Gesund-heitspsychologie, 12, 148–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutz, H. and Wiedermann, P. M. (2000). Hazardous incident information for the public: is it useful?Australasian Journal of Disaster and Trauma Studies, 4.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An Overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2. .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, M. and Thompson, M. (1990). Divided We Stand: Redefining Politics, Technology and Social Choice. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R. (1994). Optimism, vulnerability, and self-beliefs as health-related cognitions. Psychology and Health, 9, 161–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scottish Poverty Information Unit (1999). Poverty in Scotland 1999. Glasgow Caledonian University.Google Scholar
Sekizawa, J. (2013). Other aspects of BSE issues in East Asian countries. Risk Analysis, 33, 1952–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. L., McCombs, M., Weaver, D. H. and Hamm, B. J. (1999). Individuals, groups and agenda melding: a theory of social dissonance. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 11, 2–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrader-Frechette, K. and Cooke, R. (2004). Ethics and choosing appropriate means to an end: problems with coal mine and nuclear workplace safety. Risk Analysis, 24, 147–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiloh, S., Güvenç, G. and Önkal, D. (2007). Cognitive and emotional representations of terror attacks: a cross-cultural exploration. Risk Analysis, 27, 397–409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M. (2010). Trust and confidence: the difficulties in distinguishing the two concepts in research. Risk Analysis, 30, 1022–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M. (2013). The necessity for longitudinal studies in risk perception research. Risk Analysis, 33, 50–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M. and Cvetkovich, G. (2000). Perception of hazards: the role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Analysis, 20, 713–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M., Earle, T. C. and Gutscher, H. (2003). Test of a trust and confidence model in the applied context of electromagnetic field (EMF) risks. Risk Analysis, 23, 705–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M., Earle, T. C., Gutscher, H. and Keller, C. (2005b). Perception of mobile phone and base station risks. Risk Analysis, 25, 1253–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M. and Gutscher, H. (2006). Flooding risks: a comparison of laypeople’s perceptions and expert’s assessments in Switzerland. Risk Analysis, 26, 971–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegrist, M., Keller, C., Kastenholz, H., Frey, S. and Wiek, A. (2007). Laypeople’s and experts’ perception of nanotechnology hazards. Risk Analysis, 27, 59–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M., Keller, C. and Kiers, H. (2005a). A new look at the psychometric paradigm of perception of hazards. Risk Analysis, 25, 209–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siero, F. W., van Diem, M. T., Voorrips, R. and Willemsen, M. C. (2004). Perconceptional smoking: an exploratory study of determinants of change in smoking behaviour among women in the fertile age range. Health Education Research, 19, 418–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silver, N. and Braun, C. C. (1999). Behaviour. In Wogalter, M. S. E., Dejoy, D. M. and Langhery, K. R. (eds.), Warnings and Risk Communication (pp. 245–62). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Simarra, J., de Paul, J. and San Juan, C. (2002). Malos tratos infantiles: representaciones sociales de la población general y de los profesionales del ambito de la infancía en el caribe colombiano [Child abuse: social representations from the general public and from professionals working with children in the Caribbean area of Colombia]. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 815–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson-Housley, P., de Man, A. F. and Yachnin, R. (1986). Trait-anxiety and appraisal of flood hazard: a brief comment. Psychological Reports, 58, 509–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjöberg, L. (1995). Explaining Risk Perception: An Empirical and Quantitative Evaluation of Cultural Theory. RHIZIKON: Risk Research Report 22.
Sjöberg, L. (1997). Explaining risk perception: an empirical evaluation of cultural theory. Risk Decision and Policy, 2, 113–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sjöberg, L. (1999). Risk perception in western Europe. Ambio, 28, 555–68.Google Scholar
Sjöberg, L. (2001). Limits of knowledge and the limited importance of trust. Risk Analysis, 21, 189–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjöberg, L. (2002). Are received risk perception models alive and well?Risk Analysis, 22, 665–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjöberg, L. and Drottz-Sjöberg, B. M. (1993). Attitudes Toward Nuclear Waste. RHIZIKON: Risk Research Report 12.
Sjöberg, L. and Engelberg, E. (2010). Risk perception and movies: a study of availability as a factor in risk perception. Risk Analysis, 30, 95–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjöberg, L. and Wåhlberg, A. (2002). Risk perception and new age beliefs. Risk Analysis, 22, 751–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skarlatidou, A., Cheng, T. and Haklay, M. (2012). What do lay people want to know about the disposal of nuclear waste? A mental model approach to the design and development of an online risk communication. Risk Analysis, 32, 1496–511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P. (1993). Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Analysis, 13, 675–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (1996). Perception of risk from radiation. Radiation Protection Dosimetry, 68, 165–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (1997). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. In Bazerman, M. H., Messick, D. M., Tenbrunsel, A. E. and Wade-Benzoni, K. A. (eds.), Environment, Ethics and Behaviour (pp. 277–313). San Francisco: New Lexington.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (2000). The Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (2001). Cigarette smokers: rational actors or rational fools? In Slovic, P. (ed.), Smoking: Risk, Perception and Policy (pp. 97–124). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (2010). The Feeling of Risk. London: EarthscanGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B. and Lichtenstein, S. (1979). Rating the risks. Environment, 21, 36–9.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B. and Lichtenstein, S. (1980). Societal risk assessment: how safe is safe enough? In Schwing, J. E. R. C. and Alberts, W. A. (eds.), Facts and Fears: Understanding Perceived Risk (pp. 137–44). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B. and Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception?Risk Analysis, 2, 283–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B. and Lichtenstein, S. (1985). Characterising perceived risk. In Kates, R. W., Hohenemser, C. and Kasperson, J. X. (eds.), Perilous Progress: Managing the Hazards of Technology (pp. 156–71). Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E. and MacGregor, D. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24, 311–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P., Flynn, J., Mertz, C. K., Mays, C. and Poumadère, M. (1996). Nuclear Power and the Public: A Comparative Study of Risk Perception in France and the United States. Report 96–6. Eugene, OR: Decision Research.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Flynn, J., Mertz, C. K. and Mullican, L. (1993). Health Risk Perception in Canada. Ottawa: Department of National Health and Welfare.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., Letzel, H. and Malmfors, T. (1989). Risk perception of prescription drugs: report on a survey in Sweden. Pharmaceutical Medicine, 4, 43–65.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Layman, M., Kraus, N., Flynn, J., Chalmers, J. and Gesell, G. (1991). Perceived risk, stigma, and potential economic impacts of a high-level nuclear waste repository in Nevada. Risk Analysis, 11, 683–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S. and Edwards, W. (1965). Boredom-induced changes in preferences among bets. American Journal of Psychology, 78, 208–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P., Malmfors, T., Krewski, D., Mertz, C. K., Neil, N. and Bartlett, S. (1995). Intuitive toxicology II: expert and lay judgments of chemical risks in Canada. Risk Analysis, 15, 661–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Peters, E., Finucane, M. L. and MacGregor, D. G. (2005). Affect, risk and decision making. Health Psychology, 24, S35–S40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smerecnik, C. M. R., Mesters, I., Kessels, L. T. E., Ruiter, R. A. C., De Vries, N. K. and De Vries, H. (2010). Understanding the positive effects of graphical risk information on comprehension: measuring attention directed to written, tabular, and graphical risk information. Risk Analysis, 30, 1387–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. (2005). Dangerous news: media decision making about climate change risk. Risk Analysis, 25, 1471–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R. and Frewer, L. J. (1994). Gene technology, food production and public opinion: a UK study. Agriculture and Human Values, 11, 19–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, L., Ritchie, J., Lewis, J. and Dillon, L. (2003). Quality in Qualitative Evaluation: Framework for Assessing Research Evidence. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Spiegelhalter, D. (2011). Quantifying uncertainty. In Skinns, L., Scott, M. and Cox, T. (eds.), Risk (pp. 17–33). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spigner, C., Hawkins, W. and Loren, W. (1993). Gender differences in perception of risk associated with alcohol and drug use among college students. Women and Health, 20, 87–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzenstetter, F. (2003). Biais d’optimisme et biais de mesure: l’évaluation relative ou absolue du risque personnel [Optimistic bias and measurement bias: relative or absolute valuation of the personal risk]. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 58, 19–27.Google Scholar
Stacy, A. W., Newcomb, M. D. and Bentler, P. M. (1991). Personality, problem drinking and drunk driving: mediating, moderating and direct-effect models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 795–811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahlberg, D. and Maass, A. (1998). Hindsight bias: impaired memory or biased reconstruction?European Review of Social Psychology, 8, 105–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starr, C. (1969). Social benefits versus technological risk. Science, 165, 1232–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starr, C. (2003). The precautionary principle versus risk analysis. Risk Analysis, 23, 1–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steger, M. A. and Witte, S. L. (1989). Gender differences in environmental orientations: a comparison of publics and activists in Canada and the US. Western Political Quarterly, 42, 627–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J. G. (1988). International negotiation: a multidisciplinary perspective. Negotiation Journal, 4, 221–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steptoe, A., Doherty, S., Kerry, S. and Rink, E. S. (2000). Sociodemographic and psychological predictors of changes in dietary fat consumption in adults with high blood cholesterol following counselling in primary care. Health Psychology, 19, 411–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T. and Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environment and Behaviour, 25, 322–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1936). A scale for the measurement of a psychological magnitude: loudness. Psychological Review, 43, 405–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1961). To honor Fechner and repeal his law. Science, 133, 80–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1962). The surprising simplicity of sensory metrics. American Psychologists, 17, 29–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoner, J. A. F. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions including risk. Thesis quoted in Brown, R., Social Psychology. New York: Free Press, 1965.Google Scholar
Summers, C. and Hine, D. W. (1997). Nuclear waste goes on the road: risk perceptions and compensatory tradeoffs in single-industry communities. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 29, 211–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunstein, C. R. (2002). Risk and Reason. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sutherland, S. (2007). Irrationality. London: Pinter and Martin, 2nd edition.Google Scholar
Svenson, O. (1988). Mental models of risk communication and action: reflections on social amplification of risk. Risk Analysis, 8, 199–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, E. R. and Fosnocht, D. E. (2000). Anthrax threats: a report of two incidents from Salt Lake City. Journal of Emergency Medicine, 18, 229–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tait, J. (2001). More Faust than Frankenstein: the European debate about the precautionary principle and risk regulation for genetically modified crops. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 175–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takano, K. I. and Reason, J. (1997). Modelling of human errors in cognitive processes observed in dynamic environments. In Reason, J. (ed.), Managing the Risks of Organisational Accidents (pp. 63–70). Burlington, VT: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Taleb, N. N. (2001). Fooled by Randomness. London: Texere.Google Scholar
Taleb, N. N. (2007). The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable. London: Penguin, 2nd edition 2010.Google Scholar
Tanaka, Y. (2004). Major psychological factors affecting acceptance of gene-recombination technology. Risk Analysis, 24, 1575–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E. and Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teigen, K. H., Brun, W. and Slovic, P. (1988). Societal risks as seen by a Norwegian public. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 1, 111–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terpstra, T. (2011). Emotions, trust, and perceived risk: affective and cognitive routes to flood preparedness behavior. Risk Analysis, 31, 1658–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terwel, B. W., Harinck, F., Ellemers, N. and Daamen, D. D. L. (2009). Competence-based and integrity-based trust as predictors of acceptance of carbon dioxide capture and storage (CCS). Risk Analysis, 29, 1129–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesser, A., Martin, L. and Mendolia, M. (1995). The impact of thought on attitude extremity and attitude-behaviour consistency. In Petty, R. E. and Krosnick, J. A. (eds.), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences (pp. 73–92). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H. and Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, K. M. (2002). Variability and uncertainty meet risk management and risk communication. Risk Analysis, 22, 647–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, M., Ellis, R. and Wildavsky, A. (1990). Cultural Theory. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Thomson, M. E., Önkal, D., Avcioğlu, A. and Goodwin, P. (2004). Aviation risk perception: a comparison between experts and novices, Risk Analysis, 24, 1585–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tickner, J. and Gouveia-Vigeant, T. (2005). The 1991 cholera epidemic in Peru: not a case of precaution gone awry. Risk Analysis, 25, 495–502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, E., Clarke, D. D. and Travis, B. (2004). Effects of context and feelings on perceptions of genetically modified food. Risk Analysis, 24, 1369–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trumbo, C. W. (1999). Heuristic-systematic information processing and risk judgment. Risk Analysis, 19, 391–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trumbo, C. W., Lueck, M., Marlatt, H. and Peek, L. (2011). The effect of proximity to Hurricanes Katrina and Rita on subsequent hurricane outlook and optimistic bias. Risk Analysis, 31, 1907–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tullar, W. L. and Johnson, D. F. (1973). Group decision making and the risky shift: a trans-national perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 8, 117–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulloch, J. (1999). Fear of crime and the media: sociocultural theories of risk. In Lupton, D. (ed.), Risk and Sociocultural Theory: New Directions and Perspectives (pp. 34–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, B. A. (1991). The development of a safety culture. Chemistry & Industry, 1, 241–3.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: a social cognitive theory of group behaviour. In Lawler, E. J. (ed.), Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research (vol. II, pp. 77–122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C. (1991). Social Influence. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C. and Giles, H. (1981). Intergroup Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D. and Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Wetherell, M. S. and Hogg, M. A. (1989). Referent informational influence and group polarisation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 135–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twigger-Ross, C. L. and Breakwell, G. M. (1999). Relating risk experience, venturesomeness and risk perception. Journal of Risk Research, 2, 73–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tykocinski, O. E., Pick, D. and Kedmi, D. (2002). Retroactive pessimism: a different kind of hindsight bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 577–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulleberg, P. and Rundmo, T. (2003). Personality, attitudes and risk perception as predictors of risky driving behaviour among young drivers. Safety Science, 41, 427–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Pligt, J. (1992). Nuclear Energy and the Public. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
van der Pligt, J. and Richard, R. (1994). Changing adolescents’ sexual behaviour: perceived risk, self-efficacy and anticipated regret. Patient Education and Counseling, 23, 187–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Velde, F. W., Hooykaas, C. and van der Pligt, J. (1992). Risk perception and behavior: pessimism, realism, and optimism about AIDS-related health behaviors. Psychology and Health, 6, 23–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughan, E. and Seifert, M. (1992). Variability in the framing of risk issues. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 119–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verplanken, B. (1989). Beliefs, attitudes, and intentions toward nuclear energy before and after Chernobyl in a longitudinal within-subjects design. Environment and Behaviour, 21, 371–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidmar, B. and Burdeny, T. C. (1971). Effects of group size and item type in the ‘group shift’ effect. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 3, 393–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viklund, M. J. (2003). Trust and risk perception in western Europe: a cross-national study. Risk Analysis, 23, 727–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villejoubert, G. (2005). Could they have known better?Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 140–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visschers, V. H. M., Meertens, R. M., Passchier, W. W. F. and De Vries, N. N. K. (2009). Probability information in risk communication: a review of the research literature. Risk Analysis, 29, 267–287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visschers, V. H. M. and Siegrist, M. (2013). How a nuclear power plant accident influences acceptance of nuclear power: results of a longitudinal study before and after the Fukushima disaster. Risk Analysis, 33, 333–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlek, C. (2011). Straightening out the grounds for precaution: a commentary and some suggestions about Terje Aven’s ‘On different types of uncertainties’. Risk Analysis, 31, 1534–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C. and Kuhlicke, C. (2013). The risk perception paradox: implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Analysis, 33, 1049–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, K. (2007). Mental models of flash floods and landslides. Risk Analysis, 27, 671–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wåhlberg, A. (2001). The theoretical features of some current approaches to risk perception. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 237–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, A. J., Murch, S. H., Anthony, A., Linnell, J., Casson, D. M., Malik, M., Berelowitz, M., Dhillon, A. P., Thomson, M. A., Harvey, P., Valentine, A., Davies, S. E. and Walker-Smith, J. A. (1998). Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. The Lancet, 351, 637–41 (Retracted).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, G., Simmons, P., Wynne, B. and Irwin, A. (1998). Public Perception of Risks Associated with Major Accident Hazards. Sudbury: Health and Safety Executive Books.Google Scholar
Wallach, M. A., Kogan, N. and Bem, D. J. (1962). Group influence on individual risk-taking. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 65, 75–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallach, M. A., Kogan, N. and Bem, D. J. (1964). Diffusion of responsibility and level of risk-taking in groups. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 68, 263–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walls, J., Pidgeon, N. F., Weyman, A. and Horlick-Jones, T. (2003). Critical trust: understanding lay perceptions of health and safety risk regulation. Journal of Risk Research (Report). Health Risk and Society, 6, 133–50.Google Scholar
Wallston, K. A. (2005). Overview of special issue on research with the multidimensional health locus of control scales. Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 619–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Liao, C., Yang, S., Zhao, W., Liu, M. and Shi, P. (2012). Are people willing to buy natural disaster insurance in China? Risk awareness, insurance acceptance, and willingness to pay. Risk Analysis, 32, 1717–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weimann, G. (1991). The influentials: back to the concept of opinion leaders. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 267–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weimann, G. and Brosius, H. B. (1994). Is there a two-step flow of agenda-setting?International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 6, 323–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 5, 441–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D. (1984). ‘Why it won’t happen to me’: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. Health Psychology, 3, 431–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1987a). Understanding and Encouraging Self-Protective Behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1987b). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 10, 481–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science, 246, 1232–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D. and Lyon, J. (1999). Mindset, optimistic bias about personal risk and health-protective behavior. British Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 289–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D., Lyon, J. E., Rothman, A. J. and Cuite, C. L. (2000). Preoccupation and affect as predictors of protective action following natural disaster. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 351–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D., Rothman, A. J. and Nicholich, K. (1998). Use of correlational data to examine the effects of risk perceptions on precautionary behaviour. Psychology and Health, 13, 479–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D., Sandman, P. M. and Roberts, N. E. (1991). Perceived susceptibility and self-protective behaviour. Health Psychology, 10, 25–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wernstedt, K. and Hersh, R. (1998). Through a lens darkly: superfund spectacles on public participation at brownfield sites. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 9, 153–73.Google Scholar
Wessely, S. (2002). The Gulf War and its aftermath. In Cwikel, J. G. and Havenaar, J. M. (eds.), Toxic Turmoil: Psychological and Societal Consequences of Ecological Disasters (pp. 101–27). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Social identity and group polarization. In Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D. and Wetherell, M. S. (eds.), Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory (pp. 142–70). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., King, P. S., Jamner, L. D. and Levine, L. (2004). Adolescents react to the events of September 11, 2001: focused versus ambient impact. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., O’Neil, R., Hollingshead, J., Holman, A. and Moore, B. (1994). Optimism in children’s judgment of health and environmental risks. Health Psychology, 13, 319–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, M. P. and Eiser, J. R. (2005). Information specificity and hazard risk potential as moderators of trust asymmetry. Risk Analysis, 25, 1187–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, M. P., Eiser, J. R. and Harris, P. R. (2004). Risk perceptions of mobile phone use while driving. Risk Analysis, 24, 323–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2013). Health risk assessment from the nuclear accident after the 2011 Great East Japan earthquake and tsunami, based on a preliminary dose estimation. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Wiedemann, P., Schütz, H., Spangenberg, A. and Krug, H. F. (2011). Evidence maps: communicating risk assessments in societal controversies: the case of engineered nanoparticles. Risk Analysis, 31, 1770–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiegman, O. and Gutteling, J. M. (1995). Risk appraisal and risk communication: some empirical data from the Netherlands reviewed. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16, 227–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, J. B. and Rogers, M. D. (2002). Comparing precaution in the United States and Europe. Journal of Risk Research, 5, 317–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildavsky, A. (1993). The comparative study of risk perception: a beginning. In Rnck, Bayerisohe (ed.), Risk is a Construct (pp. 89–94). Munich: Knesebeck.Google Scholar
Williams, J. C. (1988). A data-based method for assessing and reducing human error to improve operational performance. Paper presented to the Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers Fourth Conference on Human Factors and Power Plants, New York.CrossRef
Williams, P. R. D. and Hammitt, J. K. (2001). Perceived risks of conventional and organic produce: pesticides, pathogens, and natural toxins. Risk Analysis, 21, 319–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, S., Zainuba, M. and Jackson, R. (2003). Affective influences on risk perceptions and risk intention. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18, 126–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willis, H. H., DeKay, M. L., Granger Morgan, M., Florig, H. K. and Fischbeck, P. S. (2004). Ecological risk ranking: development and evaluation of a method for improving public participation in environmental decision making. Risk Analysis, 24, 363–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D. K., Kaplan, R. M. and Schneiderman, L. J. (1987). Framing of decisions and selections of alternatives in healthcare. Social Behaviour, 2, 51–9.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. K., Purdon, S. E. and Wallston, K. A. (1988). Compliance to health recommendations: a theoretical overview of message framing. Health Education Research, 3, 161–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, K., Leonard, B., Wright, R., Graham, I., Moffet, J., Pluscauskas, M. et al. (2006). Application of the precautionary principle by senior policy officials: results of a Canadian survey. Risk Analysis, 26, 981–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. M., Brekke, N. (1996). A new look at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 125, 387–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59, 329–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, N. D. and Reading, A. E. (1989). Personality correlates of Type A behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 991–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, M. D., Bostrom, A., Bridges, T. and Linkov, I. (2012a). Cognitive mapping tools: review and risk management needs. Risk Analysis, 32, 1333–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, M., Kovacs, D., Bostrom, A., Bridges, T. and Linkov, I. (2012b). Flood risk management: US Army Corps of Engineers and layperson perceptions. Risk Analysis, 32, 1349–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, G., Bolger, F. and Rowe, G. (2002). An empirical test of the relative validity of expert and lay judgments of risk. Risk Analysis, 22, 1107–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, G., Pearman, A. and Yardley, K. (2000). Risk perception in the UK oil and gas production industry: are expert loss-prevention managers’ perceptions different from those of members of the public. Risk Analysis, 20, 681–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynne, B. (1989). Sheepfarming after Chernobyl. Environment, 31, 11–39.Google Scholar
Xie, X., Wang, M. and Xu, L. (2003). What risks are Chinese people concerned about?Risk Analysis, 23, 685–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, A. (2004). The effects of mass media reports on risk perception and images of victims: an explorative study. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 152–64.Google Scholar
Yin, J. (1999). Elite opinion and media diffusion: exploring environmental attitudes. Harvard International Journal of Press-Politics, 4, 62–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
You, X. and Tonon, F. (2012). Event-tree analysis with imprecise probabilities. Risk Analysis, 32, 330–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioural decision making. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 12, 93–106.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeelenberg, M. and Beattie, J. (1997). Consequences of regret aversion 2: additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision making. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 72, 63–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, J. H., Watt, J. H., Snyder, L. B., Yan, J. T. and Jiang, Y. S. (1993). Public issue priority formation: media agenda-setting and social-interaction. Journal of Communication, 43, 8–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zohar, D. (1980). Safety climate in industrial organizations: theoretical and applied implications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 96–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation Seeking: Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1987). Is sensation seeking a predisposing trait for alcoholism? In Gotheil, E., Druley, K. A., Pashkey, S. and Weinstein, S. P. (eds.), Stress and Addiction (pp. 283–301). New York: Brunder/Mazel.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1992). What is a basic factor and which factors are basic? Turtles all the way down. Personality and Individual Factors, 13, 675–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1996). The psychobiological model for impulsive unsocialized sensation seeking: a comparative approach. Neuropsychobiology, 34, 125–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman Kuhlman personality questionnaire (ZKPQ): an alternative five-factorial model. In de Read, B. and Perugini, M. (eds.), Big Five Assessment (pp. 376–92). Göhingen: Hogrefe Verlagsgruppe.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (2003). Biological bases of personality. Handbook of Psychology: Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 85–116.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (2004). The shaping of personality: genes, environments, and chance encounters. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82, 11–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (2005a). Psychobiology of Personality. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2nd edition.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (2005b). Faites vos jeux à nouveau: still another look at sensation seeking and pathological gambling. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 361–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Ball, S. and Black, J. (1990). Influences of sensation seeking, gender, risk appraisal and situational motivation on smoking. Addictive Behaviours, 15, 209–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. and Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 65, 96–102.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Thornquist, M. and Kiers, H. (1991). Five (or three) robust questionnaire scale factors of personality without culture. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 929–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Tushup, R. and Finner, S. (1976). Sexual attitudes and experience: attitude and personality correlations and changes produced by a course in sexuality. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Glynis M. Breakwell, University of Bath
  • Book: The Psychology of Risk
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139061933.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Glynis M. Breakwell, University of Bath
  • Book: The Psychology of Risk
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139061933.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Glynis M. Breakwell, University of Bath
  • Book: The Psychology of Risk
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139061933.012
Available formats
×