Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T19:49:25.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Writing as an Interaction with Ideas

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2010

Scott Barry Kaufman
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
Get access

Summary

Creative studies benefit in numerous ways from biographical and autobiographical accounts. The benefits may be especially obvious in studies of writers, given that the evidence is often quite explicit and well articulated. After all, writers are experts at self-expression. Assuming that the biographical or autobiographical account is itself written or is in some way linguistic, it is likely that the data provided by writers are more informative than, say, those provided by a dancer or painter who writes about his or her life. Rothenberg's (1990) account of the novelist John Cheever comes to mind, as does Albert (1996) on the Brontes; Ippolito and Tweney (2003) on Virginia Wolfe; Henrickson (2003) on Mark Twain; and my own modest work on Sylvia Plath (1998).

Certainly there are methodological concerns with all biographical studies. They do not provide the same kind of data as experimental and controlled studies and are open to various biases that can undermine internal validity. Still, they provide useful illustrations and suggest hypotheses that can later be tested in a more controlled fashion. Additionally, biographical studies retain a realistic level of analysis. There is little reductionism, for example, just to name one experimental problem that is largely avoided by biographical studies (Runco & Okuda, 1993). The level of analysis in biographical studies is the creative individual rather than one particular personality trait, one particular cognitive process, or one particular psychological or social need. Experimental studies provide reliable data, but their advantage – experimental control – is also their disadvantage.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, R. S. (1996). What the study of eminence can teach us. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 307–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1987). Creativity and mental illness: Prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1288–1292.Google ScholarPubMed
Baer, J. (1998). The case for domain specificity of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 173–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, F. (1963). The needs for order and for disorder as motives in creative activity. In Taylor, C. W. & Barron, F. (Eds.), Scientific creativity: Its recognition and development (pp. 153–160). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Barron, F. (1968). Creativity and personal freedom. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Barron, F. (1995). No rootless flower: An ecology of creativity. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Brower, R. (1999). Dangerous minds: Eminently creative people who spent time in jail. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 3–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and mind (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, L. M. (1989). A continuum of adaptive creative behaviors. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 169–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cramer, J. S. (2007). I to myself: An annotated selection from the journal of Henry David Thoreau. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. J. (1967). Creativity. London: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The dangers of originality: Creativity and the artistic process. In Gedo, M. M. (Ed.), Psychoanalytic perspectives on art (pp. 213–224). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). The domain of creativity. In Runco, M. A. & Albert, R. S. (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp. 190–212). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Eco, U. (1992). Overinterpreting texts. In Eco, U. (Ed.), Interpretation and Overinterpretation (pp. 45–67). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberger, R., & Shanock, L. (2003). Rewards, intrinsic motivation, and creativity: A case study of conceptual and methodological isolation. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 121–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenman, R. (1992). Creativity in prisoners: Conduct disorders and psychotics. Creativity Research Journal, 5, 175–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, H. E. (1988). The evolving systems approach to creative work. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 27–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, H. E. (1996). The life space of a scientist: The visionary function and other aspects of Jean Piaget's thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 251–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, D. M., & Anderson, S. M. (1981). Creativity, masculinity, femininity, and three models of psychological androgyny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 744–757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrickson, G. P. (2003). Mark Twain, criticism, and the limits of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 15 (2/3), 253–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ippolito, M., & Tweney, R. (2003). Virginia Woolf and the journey to Jacob's Room: The “network of enterprise” of Virginia Woolf's first experimental novel. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 25–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamison, K. R. (1989). Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychiatry, 52, 125–134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K., Runco, M. A., Dorinan, C., & Freeland, D. C. (1997). Influential factors in artists' lives and themes in their art work. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 221–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasof, J. (1995). Explaining creativity: The attributional perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 311–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaun, D. E. (1991). Writers die young: The impact of work and leisure on longevity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 12, 381–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, M. S. (1992). Creativity in aging artists: Contributions from the humanities to the psychology of aging. Creativity Research Journal, 5, 211–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludwig, A. (1998). Method and madness in the arts and sciences. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 93–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Dickerhoof, R. (2006). The costs and benefits of writing, talking, and thinking about life's triumphs and defeats. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 692–708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClary, R. (2007). Healing the psyche through music, myth, and ritual. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 155–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science. Science, 159, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, H. (1981). The “new scholarship”: Textual evidence and its implications for criticism, literary theory, and aesthetics. Studies in American Fiction, 9, 181–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, H. (1984). Flawed texts and verbal icons: Literary authority in American fiction. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1997). Disclosure of traumas and immune function: Health implications for psychotherapy. In Runco, M. A. & Richards, R. (Eds.), Eminent creativity, everyday creativity, and health (pp. 287–302). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Carmichael's handbook of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A. (1998). Beware of simple conclusions: The case for content generality of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 179–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, S. S. (2007a, April 15). Kinfolks: Falling off the family tree. Los Angeles Times Review of Books.Google Scholar
Reynolds, S. S. (2007b, October 7). Review of J. S. Cramers's Annotated Journal of Henry David Thoreau (Yale University Press). Los Angeles Times Review of Books.Google Scholar
Richards, R. (1990). Everyday creativity, eminent creativity, and health: “Afterview” for Creativity Research Journal issues on creativity and health. Creativity Research Journal, 3(4), 300–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity, mental health, and alcoholism. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 179–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, A., & Hausman, C. R. (Eds.) (1976). The creativity question (pp. 86–92). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Runco, M. A. (1988). Creativity research: Originality, utility, and integration. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (Ed.). (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Runco, M. A. (1994). Creativity and its discontents. In Shaw, M. P. & Runco, M. A. (Eds.), Creativity and affect (pp. 102–123). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1996). Personal creativity: Definition and developmental issues. New Directions for Child Development, 72 (Summer), 3–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1998). Suicide and creativity: The case of Sylvia Plath. Death Studies, 22, 637–654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runco, M. A. (1999). Contrarianism. In Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (pp. 367–371). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2006). Creativity is always personal and only sometimes social. In Schaler, J. (Ed.), Howard Gardner under fire: The rebel psychologist faces his critics (pp. 169–182). Chicago: Open Court Publishers.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity and education. New Horizons in Education, May, 96–104.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2008). A hierarchical framework for the study of creativity. New Horizons in Education [www.scpe.ied.edu.hk/newhorizon].Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Charles, R. (1993). Judgments of originality and appropriateness as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 537–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Okuda Sakamoto, S. (1993). Reaching creatively gifted children through their learning styles. In Milgram, R. M., Dunn, R., & Price, G. E. (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents: An international learning style perspective (pp. 103–115). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Sigelman, C., & Rider, E. (2008). Lifespan human development (6th ed.). New York: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1995). Exceptional personal influence: An integrative paradigm. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 371–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1983). Intellectual self-management in old age. American Psychologist, 38, 239–244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, W. (2007, April 15). Review of Stubbs' John Donne: Reformed soul. Los Angeles Times Review of Books.Google Scholar
Solomon, B., Powell, K., & Gardner, H. (1999). Multiple intelligences. In Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 259–273). San Diego, CA: Academic.Google Scholar
Stubbs, J. (2007). John Donne: The reformed soul. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1996). Presymbolic processes in scientific creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 163–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weeks, D. J., & Ward, K. (1988). Eccentrics: The scientific investigation. Stirling: Stirling University Press.Google Scholar
Weeks, D. J., & Ward, K. (1999). Eccentricity. In Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (pp. 613–621). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×