Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:18:41.204Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2a - Developmental models in pediatric neuropsychology

from Section I - Theory and models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2010

Jacobus Donders
Affiliation:
Mary Free Bed Rehabilitation Hospital
Scott J. Hunter
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Get access

Summary

“A single good model is worth a thousand empirical studies”

James Heckman (Nobel Prize, Economics, 2000) quoted by David Kirp

“Good models are like good tools: they do a certain job reasonably well … simple models that work well for a wide variety of jobs are especially valuable … (they yield) islands of conceptual clarity in the midst of otherwise mind-numbing complexity and diversity”

Richerson and Boyd

Introduction

On what grounds does a hard-nosed number-crunching economist make such a claim? What does he mean? What are the implications for the elaboration of the knowledge base? For clinical practice?

A model is a tool for thinking, for organizing a body of data into a theoretically coherent construct whose validity can be tested. Thinking in both research and clinical arenas is based on a constant interaction between models and evidence. The challenge of empirical data (evidence) is that at any one point there may be much to make sense of. Data are not always internally consistent; and, until established by multiple replications across data sets, evidence is constantly subject to discussion, argument, and change. Models may not be subject to as rapid change as the evidence base. They cannot, nonetheless, be static: as evidence accumulates, models must be scrutinized and reformulated.

There are two major sources of change in science.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Kirp, DL. The Sandbox Investment. The Preschool Movement and Kids-First Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2007.Google Scholar
Richerson, PJ, Boyd, R. Not by Genes Alone. How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, DT. Evolutionary psychology, cognitive science and dynamical systems. Building an integrative paradigm. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001;10:13–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preuss, TM, Caceres, M, Oldham, MC, Geschwind, DH. Human brain evolution: insights from microarrays. Nat Rev 2004;5(11):850–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dayan, P, Abbott, IF. Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2005.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Beyond Modularity. A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1992.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. Animal Species and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumont, L. The modern conception of the individual: notes on its genesis and that of concomitant institutions. Contrib Indian Sociol 1965;8:13–61.Google Scholar
Wertsch, JV, Sammarco, JG. Social precursors to individual cognitive functioning: the problem of units of analysis. In Wertsch, JV, ed. Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1985.Google Scholar
,Institute of Medicine (U.S.). Committee on Quality of Health Care in America. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Washington DC: National Academy Press; 2001.Google Scholar
Tversky, A, Kahneman, D.Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 1974;185(4157):1124–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahn, W, Kim, NS. Causal theories of mental disorder concepts. Psychol Sci Agenda 2008;22(6).Google Scholar
Bernstein, JH. Developmental neuropsychological assessment. In Yeates, KO, Ris, DM, Taylor, HG, eds. Pediatric Neuropsychology; Research, Theory, and Practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2000: 405–38.Google Scholar
Ash, J, Gallup, GG. Paleoclimatic variation and brain expansion during human evolution. Human Nat 2007;18(2):109–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvin, WH. A Brain for All Seasons: Human Evolution and Abrupt Climate Change. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2002.Google Scholar
Gunnar, MR. Quality of early care and the buffering of neuroendocrine stress reactions: potential effects on the developing human brain. Prev Med 1998;27:208–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, AB, Ziegler, TE, Kurian, JR, Jacoris, S, Pollak, SD. Early experience in humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating social behavior. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 2005;102(47):17237–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, CA, ed. The Effects of Early Adversity on Neurobehavioral Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000.
Hart, B, Risley, T. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experiences of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 2005.Google Scholar
Taylor, HG, Yeates, KO, Wade, SL, Drotar, D, Stancin, T, Burant, C. Bidirectional child-family influences on outcomes of traumatic brain injury in children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2001;7(6):755–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, K. Models of Cognitive Development. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 1998.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A, Scerif, G, Ansari, D.Double dissociations in developmental disorders? Theoretically misconceived, empirically dubious. Cortex. 2003 Feb;39(1):161–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llinas, RR. I of the Vortex. From Neurons to Self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2001.Google Scholar
Gibbs, RW. Embodiment and Cognitive Science. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2006.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1983.Google Scholar
Tooby, J, Cosmides, L. The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, JB, Cosmides, L, Tooby, J, eds. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992: 19–36.Google Scholar
Buller, DJ, Hardcastle, VG. Evolutionary psychology, meet developmental neurobiology: against promiscuous modularity. Brain Mind 2000;1:307–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Leary, DD. Development of connectional diversity and specificity in the mammalian brain by the pruning of collateral projections. Curr Op Neurobiol 1992;2(1):70–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, DVM. Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders: uncomfortable bedfellows. Q J Exp Psychol 1997;50a(4):899–923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, MH, Grossmann, T, Cohen, Kadosh K.Mapping functional brain development: building a social brain through interactive specialization. Dev Psychol 2009;45(1):151–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, JL, Bates, EA, Johnson, MH, Karmiloff-Smith, A, Parisi, D, Plunkett, K. Rethinking innateness. A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1996.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. The concept and development of general intellectual ability. In Reed, J, Warner-Rogers, J, eds. Child Neuropsychology, Concepts, Theory and Practice. Malden, MA: Wiley–Blackwell; 2008.Google Scholar
Laughlin, SB, Sejnowski, TJ. Communication in neuronal networks. Science 2003;301(5641):1870–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petronis, A, Gottesman, II, Crow, TJ, DeLisi, , Klar, AJ, Macciardi, F, et al. Psychiatric epigenetics: a new focus for the new century. Mol Psychiatry 2000;5(4):342–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R, Spinath, FM. Genetics and general cognitive ability (g). Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6(4):169–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovas, Y, Plomin, R. Generalist genes: implications for the cognitive sciences. Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10(5):198–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A, Sugden, K, Moffitt, TE, Taylor, A, Craig, IW, Harrington, H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 2003;301(5631):386–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascual-Leone, A, Amedi, A, Fregni, F, Merabet, LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 2005;28:377–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buonomano, DV, Merzenich, MM. Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annu Rev Neurosci 1998;21:149–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merabet, LB, Rizzo, JF, Amedi, A, Somers, DC, Pascual-Leone, A.What blindness can tell us about seeing again: merging neuroplasticity and neuroprostheses. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6(1):71–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, VS.Plasticity and functional recovery in neurology. Clin Med 2005;5(4):368–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flor, H.Phantom limb pain: characteristics, causes and treatment. Lancet 2002;1:182–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, B, Kunkel, A, Muhlnickel, W, Villringer, K, Taub, E, Flor, H. Plasticity in the motor system related to therapy-induced improvement of movement after stroke. Neuroreport 1999;10(4):807–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaser, C, Schlaug, G. Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;999:514–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atherton, M, Zhuang, J, Bart, WM, Hu, XP, He, S.A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. I. The game of chess. Cogn Brain Res 2003;16:26–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, J, Reinecke, K, Liesen, H, Weiss, M.Cortical activity of skilled performance in a complex sports related motor task. Eur J Appl Physiol 2008;104(4):625–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burton, H.Visual cortex activity in early and late blind people. J Neurosci 2003;23(10):4005–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, J.The fundamentals of brain development. Integrating Nature and Nurture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2008.Google Scholar
Silverstein, FS, Jensen, FE. Neonatal seizures. Ann Neurol 2007;62(2):112–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, EL. Contrasting concepts of the critical period for language. In Carey, S, Gelman, R, eds. The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991: 111–31.Google Scholar
Filley, CM.The Behavioral Neurology of White Matter. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001.Google Scholar
Yakovlev, PI, Lecours, AR.The Myelogenetic Cycles of Regional Maturation of the Brain. Oxford: Blackwell; 1967.Google Scholar
Paus, T, Zijdenbos, A, Worsley, K, Collins, DL, Blumenthal, J, Giedd, JN, et al. Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science 1999;283(5409):1908–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowell, ER, Peterson, BS, Thompson, PM, Welcome, SE, Henkenius, AL, Toga, AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci 2003;6(3):309–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasan, KM, Kamali, A, Iftikhar, A, Kramer, , Papanicolaou, AC, Fletcher, JM, et al. Diffusion tensor tractography quantification of the human corpus callosum fiber pathways across the lifespan. Brain Res 2009;1249:91–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, JH, Waber, DP. Executive capacities from a developmental perspective. In Meltzer, L, Understanding Executive Function: Implications and Opportunities for the Classroom. New York: Guilford Publications; 2007.Google Scholar
Jerison, HJ.Evolution of prefrontal cortex. In Krasnegor, NA, Lyon, GR, Goldman-Rakic, P, eds. Development of the Prefrontal Cortex. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1997: 9–26.Google Scholar
Pennington, BF. Dimensions of executive functions in normal and abnormal development. In Krasnegor, NA, Lyon, GR, Goldman-Rakic, P, eds. Development of the Prefrontal Cortex. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1997: 265–81.Google Scholar
Roth, G, Dicke, U. Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9(5):250–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M-M.Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S-J, Choudhury, S.Brain development during puberty: state of the science. Dev Sci 2006;9(1):11–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M, Landry, SH, Barnes, M, Fletcher, JM. A model of neurocognitive function in spina bifida over the life span. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12(2):285–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E, Reilly, J, Wulfeck, B, Dronkers, N, Opie, M, Fenson, J, et al. Differential effects of unilateral lesions on language production in children and adults. Brain Lang 2001;79:223–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciana, M.Cognitive development in children born preterm: implications for theories of brain plasticity following early injury. Dev Psychopathol 2003;15(4):1017–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Als, H.Program Guide – Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP): An Education and Training Program for Health Care Professionals. Boston: NIDCAP Federation International 2008.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, EL, O'Keefe, PA.What do children do when they cannot go to school? In Sternberg, RJ, Grigorenko, EL, eds. Culture and Competence: Contexts of Life Success. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 2004: 23–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersson, KM, Reis, A, Ingvar, M.Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: a review of some recent behavioral and functional neuroimaging data. Scand J Psychol 2001;42(3):251–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecours, AR, Mehler, J, Parente, MA, Caldeira, A, Cary, L, Castro, MJ, et al. Illiteracy and brain damage–1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects). Neuropsychologia 1987;25(1B):231–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manly, JJ, Jacobs, DM, Sano, M, Bell, K, Merchant, CA, Small, SA, et al. Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999;5(3):191–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E, McCrory, E, Fazio, F, Menoncello, L, Brunswick, N, Cappa, SF, et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nat Neurosci 2000;3(1):91–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A.Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants' performance on AB. Child Dev 1985;56(4):868–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A.The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the AB and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1990;608:267–309; discussion 309–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A.Evidence for the importance of dopamine for prefrontal cortex functions early in life. Phil Trans R Soc Lond 1996;351(1346):1483–93; discussion 94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A, Prevor, MB, Callender, G, Druin, DP.Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1997;62(4):i–v, 1–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A.Consequences of variations in genes that affect dopamine in prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 2007;17 Suppl 1:i161–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A, Briand, L, Fossella, J, Gehlbach, L.Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161(1):125–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U, Lichtenberger, L, Jones, W, Lai, Z, St George, M.The neurocognitive profile of Williams Syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. J Cogn Neurosci 2000;12 Suppl 1:7–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, SJ, Brown, JH, Gsodl, MK, Johnson, MH, Karmiloff-Smith, A.Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science 1999;286(5448):2355–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, SJ, Girelli, L, Butterworth, B, Karmiloff-Smith, A.Are numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006;47(2):190–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, KO, Bigler, ED, Dennis, M, Gerhardt, CA, Rubin, KH, Stancin, T, et al. Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: a heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. Psychol Bull 2007;133(3):535–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, TW.The symbolic species. The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: Norton; 1997.Google Scholar
Saffran, JR.Words in a sea of sounds: the output of infant statistical learning. Cognition 2001;81(2):149–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, HJ, Mills, DL, Lawson, DS.Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex 1992;2(3):244–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locke, JL.A theory of neurolinguistic development. Brain Language 1997;58(2):265–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, MI, Rothbart, MK.Educating the Human Brain. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 2007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M, Carpenter, M.Shared intentionality. Dev Sci 2007;10(1):121–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, Müller, U, Frye, D, Marcovitch, S, Argitis, G, Boseovski, J, et al. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2003;68:vii–137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, . The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. New York: Houghton Mifflin; 1979.Google Scholar
Rey-Casserly, C, Bernstein, JH.Making the transition to adulthood for individuals with learning disorders. In Wolf, , Schreiber, HE, Wasserstein, J, eds. Adult Learning Disorders: Contemporary Issues. New York: Psychology Press; 2008: 363–88.Google Scholar
Holmes-Bernstein, JM, Waber, DP.Developmental neuropsychological assessment. The systemic approach. In Boulton, AA, Baker, GB, Hiscock, M, eds. Neuromethods, vol. 17: Neuropsychology. Clifton, NJ: Humana Press; 1990: 311–71.Google Scholar
Wakefield, JC.Evolutionary versus prototype analyses of the concept of disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 1999;108:374–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satz, P.Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: a formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology 1993;7:273–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y.What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8(3):448–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M, Yeates, KO, Taylor, HG, Fletcher, JM.Brain reserve capacity, cognitive reserve capacity, and age-based functional plasticity after congenital and acquired brain injury in children. In Stern, Y, ed. Cognitive Reserve. Theory and Applications. New York: Taylor and Francis; 2007: 53–83.Google Scholar
Masten, AS.Resilience in developing systems: progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Dev Psychopathol 2007;19:921–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everson-Rose, SA, Lewis, TL.Psychosocial factors and cardiovascular diseases. Annu Rev Public Health 2005;26:469–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zunzunegui, M, Alvarado, BE, Del Ser, T, Otero, A.Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2003;58:S93–S100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toga, AW, Thompson, PM.Genetics of brain structure and intelligence. Annu Rev Neurosci 2005;28:1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M, Nelson, J.Individual differences and cognitive models of the mind: using the differentiation hypothesis to distinguish general and specific cognitive processes. In Duncan, J, McLeod, P, Phillips, L, eds. Measuring the Mind: Speed, Control and Age. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005: 89–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dally, JM, Emery, NJ, Clayton, NS.Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 2006;312(5780):1662–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A, Thomas, M, Annaz, D, Humphreys, K, Ewing, S, Brace, N, et al. Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: the importance of building developmental trajectories. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45(7):1258–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×