Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:41:39.497Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

IV - THE ASSESSMENT OF PRAGMATIC ABILITY

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2012

Kenneth R. Rose
Affiliation:
City University of Hong Kong
Gabriele Kasper
Affiliation:
University of Hawaii, Manoa
Get access

Summary

Despite the fact that communicative language teaching has now been with us for more than 3 decades, and a considerable amount of attention has been focused on developing ways to teach learners to use language for communication, far less effort has been expended in developing methods for assessing learners' ability to communicate in a second language. This has begun to change, particularly with work on the assessment of interlanguage pragmatics being carried out by established researchers in language testing.

One of the most frequently used formats for assessing oral proficiency is the oral proficiency interview or the simulated oral proficiency interview (SOPI). In the SOPI, candidates have to address a variety of communicative acts to interlocutors in different constellations of social relationships. Whether or not the SOPI elicits samples of candidates' performance that allow valid interpretations of their L2 sociolinguistic and pragmatic abilities is a central problem of construct validity. John Norris examines this issue in Chapter 12 by analyzing the use of address terms by American high school and university learners of German as a foreign language on the German Speaking Test (GST). For English-speaking learners of German, developing control over the address system involves the acquisition of complex interactions between sociopragmatic and pragmalinguistic aspects of the L2. Six tasks from the GST were identified (asking questions, giving directions, explaining a process, apologizing, requesting, and giving advice) which elicited substantial use of direct address forms and shared similar contextual constraints which likely encouraged the use of direct address.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×