Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-swr86 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T12:05:08.158Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - The whole-object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meanings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2012

Ellen M. Markman
Affiliation:
Stanford University
Get access

Summary

Starting at about 18 months of age, children become remarkably capable of learning the vocabulary of natural languages. Yet word learning presents a problem of induction that must somehow be solved by such very young children, with their limited information processing abilities. In order for children to acquire language as rapidly as they do, they must have biases that enable them to rule out many alternative hypotheses for the meanings of a word and that lead them instead to focus on hypotheses that are reasonably likely to be correct (Markman, 1989; Markman & Hutchinson, 1984; Markman & Wachtel, 1988). A sophisticated, intelligent adult, let alone a 2-year-old, would never be able to settle on the meaning of a word by open-mindedly considering every possible hypothesis and waiting for evidence that would rule out all but one (Quine, 1960). It has been suggested that children use several assumptions to solve the inductive problem posed by word learning; among these are the whole-object assumption, the taxonomic assumption, and the assumption of mutual exclusivity. In this chapter, I will review some of the evidence that children rely on these assumptions to guide their initial hypotheses about what words mean and will try to reconcile this argument with some conflicting evidence about whether these constraints are available to young children just starting to acquire language.

The taxonomic and whole-object assumptions

When an adult points to an object and labels it, the novel term could refer to an object category, but it could also refer to a part of the object or to its substance, color, or weight, and so on.

Type
Chapter
Information
Perspectives on Language and Thought
Interrelations in Development
, pp. 72 - 106
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×