Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-06T08:36:58.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part III - Language and Communicative Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Aronova, E. (2007). Karl Popper and Lamarckism. Biological Theory, 2(1), 3751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Seghier, M. L., Baquero, S., Estévez, A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature, 461, 983986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Changeux, J.-P., Courrége, P., & Danchin, A. (1973). A theory of the epigenesis of neuronal networks by selective stabilization of synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 70, 29742978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Dias, B. G., & Ressler, K. J., (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D. (2014). The instruction of imagination: Language and its evolution as a communication technology. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 105125). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2000). From cultural selection to genetic selection: A framework for the evolution of language. Selection, 1, 3355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2010). Plasticity and canalization in the evolution of linguistic communication, In Larson, R. K., Déprez, V., & Yamakido, H. (Eds.), The evolution of human language (pp. 135147). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2014). Why we need to move from gene-culture co-evolution to culturally-driven co-evolution. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 1530). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473, 7982. doi: 10.1038/nature09923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, G. (1974). The problem of molecular recognition by a selective system. In Ayala, F. & Dobzhansky, T. (Eds.), Studies in the philosophy of biology (pp. 4556). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429492. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999094XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, T. B., Russig, H., Weiss, I. C., Gräff, J., Linder, N., Michalon, A., Vizi, S., & Mansuy, I. M., (2010). Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 408415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gapp, K., Jawaid, A., Sarkies, P., Bohacek, J., Pelczar, P., Prados, J., Farinelli, L., Miska, E., & Mansuy, I. M. (2014). Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 667669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2014). Memory, imagination and the evolution of modern language. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.). The social origins of language (pp. 317324). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, R. (1935). Gen und Ausseneigenschaft. Zeitschrift für Induktive Abstammungs und Verebunglehre, 69, 38131.Google Scholar
Goodwin, B. (2009). Beyond the Darwinian Paradigm: Understanding Biological Forms. In Ruse, M. & Travis, J. (Eds.). Evolution: The first four billion years (pp. 299312). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2007). The developmental construction of heredity. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 808817,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, E., Ginsburg, S., & Dor, D. (2012). The co-evolution of language and emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 21522159. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (1995). Epigenetic inheritance and evolution: The Lamarckian dimension. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2007a). Précis of “Evolution in four dimensions.” Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 353365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2007b). Bridging the gap: The developmental aspects of evolution. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 378392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2014). Evolution in four dimensions (2nd edn.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka-Tavory, E. (1982). Genocopies and the evolution of interdependence. Evolutionary Theory, 6, 167170.Google Scholar
Laland, K., Uller, T., Feldman, M., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E., & Odling-Smee, J. (2014). Does evolutionary theory need a rethink? Yes, urgently! Nature, 514, 161164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Longa, V. M. (2013). The evolution of the faculty of language from a Chomskyan perspective: Bridging linguistics and biology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 91, 148.Google ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo, G., & Longa, V. M. (2003). Minimizing the genes for grammar. The minimalist program as a biological framework for the study of language. Lingua, 113, 643657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manly, J. J., Byrd, D., Touradji, P., Sanchez, D., & Stern, Y. (2004). Literacy and cognitive change among ethnically diverse elders. International Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 4760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemann, H-G. (2014). Karl Popper and the two new secrets of life. Tubingen, Germany: Mohr Siebeck.Google Scholar
Noble, D., Jablonka, E., Joyner, M. J., Müller, G. B., & Omholt, S. W. (2014). Evolution evolves: Physiology returns to centre stage. Journal of Physiology, 592, 22372244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, A. R. (2009). Animal asymmetry. Current Biology, 19, R473R477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1928, Trans. 1995). Genetic logic and sociology. In Smith, L. (Ed.), Piaget, J., Sociological Studies (pp. 184214). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regulations and cognitive processes. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1978). Behavior and evolution. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (Ed.). (1980). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M., & Müller, G. B. (Eds.). (2010). Evolution: The extended synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puglia, M. H., Lillard, T. S., Morris, J. P., & Connelly, J. J. (2015). Epigenetic modification of the oxytocin receptor gene influences the perception of anger and fear in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 112, 33083313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1953). The Baldwin effect. Evolution 7, 110117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, E. J., & Lloyd, S. S. (2015). Soma-to-germline feedback is implied by the extreme polymorphism at IGHV relative to MHC. BioEssays, 37, 557569. doi 10.1002/bies.201400213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, S., Fridmann-Sirkis, Y., Braun, E., & Soen, Y. (2012). Epigenetically heritable alteration of fly development in response to toxic challenge. Cell Reports, 1, 528542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szyf, M. (2015). Nongenetic inheritance and transgenerational epigenetics. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 21, 134144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thaler, L., Arnott, S. R., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Neural correlates of natural human echolocation in early and late blind echolocation experts. PLoS ONE, 6, e20162. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Boston, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In Gelfand, M., Chiu, C.-y., & Hong, Y.-y (Eds.) Advances in Culture and Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 551). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vecchi, D., & Baravalle, L. (2015). A soul of truth in things erroneous: Popper’s “amateurish” evolutionary philosophy in light of contemporary biology. History and Philosophy of Life Sciences, 36, 525545. doi 10.1007/s40656-014-0047-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes, Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E. (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Kozulin, A., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1975). The evolution of an evolutionist. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

References

Adamson, L. B. 1995. Communication development during infancy. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. 2010. Mindreaders. New York, NY: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.) 2013. Navigating the social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. 1976. Language and context. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. 2011. Children’s thinking. Belmont: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. and Pellegrini, A. 2002. The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 1994. Grounds for cognition. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 1997. Interpreting minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2000. Minding minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2001. Developing mental abilities by representing intentionality, Synthese, 129, 233258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2007. Inside loops, Synthese, 159, 235252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2009. Predicative minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2010. Our own minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2013. Mindvaults. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. 1983. Child’s talk. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. and Gergely, G. 2013. Teleological understanding of actions. In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 3743). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. 1971. Intentional systems. Journal of Philosophy 8, 87106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, M. 2009. Theory of mind. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. and Csibra, G. 2003. Teleological reasoning in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Science 7, 287292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. and Wellman, H. 1992. Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language 7, 145171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, P. 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review 66, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. 1993. Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Howhy, J. 2014. The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond modularity. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. 2013. Origins of social cognition. In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 139144). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, H. and Sperber, D. 2011. Why do humans reason? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34, 57111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. 1996. Language in cognitive development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. 2007. Young minds in social worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J. 1991. Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Moll, H.. 2013. Why don’t apes understand false belief? In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 8187). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. 1993. The self born in intersubjectivity. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The perceived self. (pp. 121173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. 2011. The generation of human meaning. In Seemann, A. (Ed.), Joint attention. (pp. 73135). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar

References

Agrillo, C., Ranpura, A., & Butterworth, B. (2010). Time and numerosity estimation are independent: Behavioral evidence for two different systems using a conflict paradigm. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 96101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcock, K. J., & Krawczyk, K. (2010). Individual differences in language development: Relationship with motor skills at 21 months. Developmental Science, 13(5), 677691. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00924.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R., & Carey, S. (2012). Core cognition and beyond: The acquisition of physical and numerical knowledge. In Pauen, S. (Ed.) Early childhood development and later outcome (pp. 3365). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., & Williams, S. C. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(6), 18911898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Elman, J., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1998). Innateness and emergentism. In Bechtel, W. and Graham, G. (Eds.). A companion to cognitive science (pp. 590601). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Roe, K. (2001). Language development in children with unilateral brain injury. In Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M. (Eds.). Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 281307). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., Levi, D. M., Li, R. W., Dan, Y., & Hensch, T. K. (2010). Removing brakes on adult plasticity: from molecular to behavioural interventions. Journal of Neuroscience. 30(45), 1496414971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R, Richardson, R. C, & Laland, K. N. (2011). Darwin in mind: New opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLoS Biology, 9 (7), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brownman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1995). Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In Port, R. & Van Gelder, T. (Eds.). Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 175–193). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B. (1999). The mathematical brain. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B. (2008). State-of-science review: Dyscalculia. In Goswami, U. C. (Ed.) Foresight mental capital and mental wellbeing. London: Office of Science and Innovation.Google Scholar
Cardoso-Martins, C., & Mervis, C. B. (1985). Maternal speech to prelinguistic children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 451458.Google ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (2011). The Origin of Concepts: A précis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 113167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain. What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(3), 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castle, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 47(2), 149180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 533549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Temple, C. (2003). Words and rules in children with Williams syndrome. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.). Language competence across populations (pp. 323352). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders. 43(1), 140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen Kadosh, K., Henson, R., Cohen Kadosh, R., Johnson, M. H., & Dick, F. (2009). Task-dependent activation of face-sensitive cortex: an fMRI adaptation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 903917.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., Bahrami, B., Walsh, V., Butterworth, B., Popescu, T., & Price, C. J. (2011). Specialization in the human brain: The case of numbers. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience 5: 62. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00062Google ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., Barrett, H. C., & Tooby, J. (2010). Adaptive specializations, social exchange, and the evolution of human intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 107, 90079014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Haan, M., Humphreys, K., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Developing a brain specialized for face processing: A converging methods approach. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 200212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Charles, L., King, J. R., & Marti, S. (2014). Toward a computational theory of conscious processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 7684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56(2), 384398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B. (2000). Developmental propagnosia with normal configural processing. NeuroReport, 11(1), 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B, Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2001). Evolutionary psychology and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(1), 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2005). Dissociations of face and object recognition in developmental prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 249261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B. & Nakayama, K. (2006). Developmental prosopagnosia: A window to content-specific face processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 166173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchaine, B., Nieminen-von Wendt, T., New, J., & Kulomaki, T. (2003). Dissociations of visual recognition in a developmental agnosic: Evidence for separate developmental processes. Neurocase, 9, 380389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duchaine, B., Yovel, G., Butterworth, E., & Nakayama, K. (2006). Prosopagnosia as an impairment to face-specific mechanisms: elimination of the alternative explanations in a developmental case. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 714747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Hohenberger, A., Herwegen, J., Campos, R., Serres, J., de Schoenen, S., Aschersleben, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013). Narrowing perceptual sensitivity to the native language in infancy: Exogenous influences on developmental timing. Behavioral Sciences, 3(1),120132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finlay, B. L. (2007). E pluribus unum: Too many unique human capacities and too many theories. In Gangestad, S. and Simpson, J. (Eds.). The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 294304). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, R., & Kreitzman, L. (2010). The rhythms of life: The biological clocks that control the daily lives of every living thing. London: Profile.Google Scholar
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, R. (1986). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J. & Mehler, J. (2010). Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 191218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A., Takahashi, D. Y., Mathur, N. A., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). Cineradiography of monkey lipsmacking reveals putative origins of speech dynamics. Current Biology, 22, 11761182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giedd, J., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N., Castellanos, F., Liu, H., & Zijdenbos, A. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J., Rumsey, J., Castellanos, F., Rajapakse, J., Kaysen, D., & Vaituzis, A. (1996). A quantitative MRI study of the corpus callosum in children and adolescents. Developmental Brain Research, 91, 274280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R., Bootzin, R. R., & Nadel, L. (2006). Naps promote abstraction in language-learning infants. Psychological Science, 17, 670674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, M. (1990). Genetic basis of grammar defect. Nature, 347(6288), 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of mathematics. In Gazzaniga, M (Ed.) The Cognitive Neurosciences, Vol. 3, pp. 853864. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hensch, T. K. (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 877888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepper, P. G., Scott, D., & Shahidullah, B. S. (1993). Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 11, 147153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horváth, K., Myers, K., Foster, R., & Plunkett, K. (2015). Napping facilitates word learning in early lexical development. Journal of Sleep Research, 24(5), 503509. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hupbach, A., Gomez, R. L., Bootzin, R. R., & Nadel, L. (2009). Nap-dependent learning in infants. Psychological Science, 12(6), 10071012. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00837.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, P. R., & de Courten, C. (1987) The development of synapses in striate cortex of man. Human Neurobiology, 6, 19.Google ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167178.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, D. C., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: Evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. Developmental Science, 14(2), 360371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37(02), 229261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 475483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaffman, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2007). Neurodevelopmental sequelae of postnatal maternal care in rodents: clinical and research implications of molecular insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 224244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Transforming a partially structured brain into a creative mind. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 17(4), 732745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2(10), 389398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Interplay of Biology and Environment, Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 5663.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Neuroimaging of the developing brain: Taking “developing” seriously. Human Brain Map, 31(6), 934941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2012). Brain: The neuroconstructivist approach. In Farran, E. K. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Eds.). Neuro-developmental disorders across the lifespan: A neuroconstructivist approach (pp. 3758). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013). Challenging the use of adult neuropsychological models for explaining neurodevelopmental disorders: Developed versus developing brains. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Aschersleben, G., de Schonen, T., Elsabbagh, M., Hohenberger, A., & Serres, J. (2010). Constraints on the timing of infant cognitive change: Domain-specific or domain-general? European Journal of Developmental Science, 4(1), 3145.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Plunkett, K., Johnson, M., Elman, J. L., & Bates, E. (1998). What does it mean to claim that something is “innate”? Mind and Language, 13(4), 588597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2005). Can developmental disorders be used to bolster claims from Evolutionary Psychology? A neuroconstructivist approach. In Taylor Parker, S., Langer, J., and Milbrath, C. (Eds.). Biology and knowledge revisited: From neurogenesis to psychogenesis (pp. 307322), Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Press.Google Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistic learning in infancy: evidence for a domain-general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35B42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krishnan, S., Alcock, K. J., Mercure, E., Leech, R., Barker, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Dick, F. J. (2013). Articulating novel words: children’s oromotor skills predict nonword repetition abilities. Speech Language and Hearing Research, 56(6), 18001812. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388 (2013/12–0206)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krishnan, S., Bergström, L., Alcock, K. J., Dick, F., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2015). Williams syndrome: a surprising deficit in oromotor praxis in a population with proficient language production. Neuropsychologia. 67, 8290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.032. Epub 2014 Nov 27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krishnan, S., Leech, R., Mercure, E., Lloyd-Fox, S. O., & Dick, F. (2014). Convergent and divergent fMRI responses in children and adults to increasing language production demands. Cerebral Cortex, 23(9), 22612268. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs213Google Scholar
Landau, B., Hoffman, J. E., & Kurz, N. (2005). Object definitions with severe spatial deficits in Williams syndrome: sparing and breakdown. Cognition 100, 483510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy. In Rubenstein, J. L. & Rakic, P., (Eds). Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Neural circuit development and function in the brain, Vol. 3 (pp. 231248). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). A modular geometric mechanism for reorientation in children. Cognitive Psychology 61(2), 152176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1992). Pretense, autism, and the theory-of-mind-module. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 1821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. S. C., & Westermann, G. (2007) Neuroconstructivism: Vol. I. How the brain constructs cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, D. L., Coffy-Corins, S., & Neville, H. (1997). Language comprehension and cerebral specialisation from 13–20 months. Developmental Psychology, 13, 397445.Google Scholar
Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., Naoi, N., & Kojima, S. (2007). Neural attunement processes in infants during the acquisition of language-specific phonemic contrasts. Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 315321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molko, N., Cachia, A., Rivière, D., Mangin, J.-F., Bruandet, M., Le Bihan, D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron, 40, 847858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, C., & Fifer, W. P. (2000) Evidence of transnatal auditory learning. Journal of Perinatology, 20, S37S44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, H., Mills, D., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Effects of altered auditory sensitivity and age of language acquisition on the development of language-relevant neural systems: Preliminary studies of William syndrome. In Broman, S. and Grafman, J. (Eds.). Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function (pp. 6783). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsödl, M. K., Johnson, M. H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders, Science, 286, 23552358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, S. J., Heim, S., Friedman, J. T., Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2006). Development of structure and function in the infant brain: Implications for cognition, language and social behaviour. Neuroscience Biobehavior Review, 30: 10871105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2001). Speaking of learning: How do we acquire our marvellous facility for expressing ourselves in words? Nature, 411, 887888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate. The modern denial of human nature. New York, NY: Penguin Group.Google Scholar
Quartz, S., & Sejnowski, T. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral Brain Sciences 20, 537596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. (1999). Specific grammatical limitations in children with Specific Language Impairment. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (Ed.). Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 331360). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Genes and environment: What does interaction really mean? Trends in Genetics, 17, 418419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shalev, R. S., Manor, O., & Gross-Tsur, V. (2005). Developmental dyscalculia: a prospective six-year follow-up. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 2, 121125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(8), 343348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2009). Innateness, learning and rationality. Child Development Perspectives 3, 9698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, J. (2009). The fundamentals of brain development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stiles, J. (2012). Neural plasticity and cognitive development: Insights from children with perinatal brain injury. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sur, M., Pallas, S. L., & Roe, A. W. (1990). Crossmodal plasticity in cortical development: Differentiation and specification of sensory neocortex. TINS, 13, 227233.Google ScholarPubMed
Temple, C. M. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and its application to children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 2752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. S. C., Knowland, V. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2011). Mechanisms of developmental regression in autism and the broader phenotype: a neural network modeling approach. Psychological Review, 118(4), 637654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomalski, P., Moore, D. G., Ribeiro, H., Axelsson, E. L., Murphy, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Kushnerenko, E. (2013). Socioeconomic status and functional brain development – Associations in early infancy. Developmental Science, 16, 676687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, L. K., Shafto, M. A., Randall, B., Wright, P., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Stamatakis, E. A. (2010). Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 352364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical specific language impairment. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 5359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Pinker, S. (2014). The biological basis of language: Insights from developmental grammatical impairments. Trends in Cognitive Science, 18(11), 586595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, M. J., Bachevalier, J., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Development and plasticity of visual memory circuits. In Julesz, B. & Kovacs, I. (Eds.). Maturational windows and adult cortical plasticity in human development: Is there reason for an optimistic view? Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Weir, R. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), 7583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, G., Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Neuroconstructivism. In Goswami, U. (Ed.). Handbook of childhood development, (pp. 723748). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense in human infants. Developmental Science, 8(1), 88101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Ambridge, B. & Lieven, E. V. M. (2011). Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Baird, J., (2005). Why language matters to theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (2007). Remembering the times of our lives: Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (2014). (Eds.). The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1990). Remembering what happened next: Very young children’s recall of event sequences. In Fivush, R. & Hudson, J. A. (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blum-Kulka, S., & Snow, C. E. (2002). Talking to adults: The contribution of multiparty discourse to language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2003). Interpreting minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2009). Predicative minds: The social ontogeny of propositional thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2010). Our own minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2013). Mindvaults. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cekaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Grover, V., & Teubal, E. (2014). (Eds.). Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: Coevolution of language and the brain. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (in press). The evolutionary origins of human cultural memory. In Wagoner, B. (Ed.). Oxford handbook of culture and memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 59 (5).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R. (2008). Remembering and reminiscing: how individual lives are constructed in family narratives. Memory Studies, 1, 4554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. (2011). The development of autobiographical memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 559582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. New York, NY: Crowell.Google Scholar
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J. (1994). (Eds.). Input and interaction in language acquisition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasekl, K., Bloom, L., Smith, L. B., Woodward, A. L., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M., & Hollich, G. (2000). (Eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Edwin Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampson, J., & Nelson, K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 20, 313342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday lives of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. (2010). Folk psychological narratives: The socio-cultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1999). Attention to functional properties in toddlers’ naming and problem-solving. Cognitive Development, 14, 77100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. T., & McNeill, D. (2015). Narrative development of young children: Gesture, imagery, and cohesion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (2004). Varieties of meaning: The 2002 Nicod Lectures. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (12, Serial No. 149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 81, 267285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1978). How young children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language. In Siegler, R. S. (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways to the community of minds. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. (Eds.), Why language matters to theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2014a). A matter of meaning: reflections on forty years of JCL. Journal of Child Language, 14 (Supplement S1), 93104.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2014b). Sociocultural theories of memory development. In Bauer, P. J. & Fivush, R. (Eds.). The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2015). Making sense with private speech. Cognitive Development, 36 · October–December 2015, 171179. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 1119, 486511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In Lamb, M. & Brown, A. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology, Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Hampson, J., & Kessler-Shaw, L. (1993). The noun bias in early lexicons: Evidence, explanations, and implications. Journal of Child Language, 20, 6184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, K. P., & Blades, M. (2000). (Eds.). Children’s source monitoring. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Assoc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation: Conversation in the preschool classroom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schaller, S. (1991). A man without words. New York, NY: Summit Books.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shipley, T. F., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). (Eds.). Understanding events: From perception to action. New York, NY: Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. (2014). Input to interaction to instruction: Three key shifts in the history of child language research. Journal of Child Language, 14 (Supplement S1), 117123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Moore, C. (2011). Introduction to special issue: The development of episodic foresight. Cognitive Development, 26, 295298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, S. (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origin and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (pp. 103130).Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1993). What is episodic memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2 (3), 6770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.) The missing link in Cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (pp. 354).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, G., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Qi. (2014). The cultured self and remembering. In Bauer, P. J. & Fivush, R. (Eds.) The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley Blackwell (pp. 605625).Google Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winsler, A. (2009). Still talking to ourselves after all these years: A review of current research on private speech. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I., (Eds.). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (pp. 341).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar

References

Bahtiyar, S., & Küntay, A. C. (2008). Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language, 35, 529555.Google Scholar
Benveniste, E. (1971). Problems in general linguistics. Coral Gables, Florida.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2004). Between emergence and mastery: The long developmental route of language acquisition. In Berman, R. (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 934). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rispoli, M., Gartner, B. & Hafitz, J. (1989). Acquisition of complementation. Journal of Child Language. 16, 101120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budwig, N. (1995). A developmental-functionalist approach to child language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Budwig, N., Stein, S., & O’Brien, C. (2001). Non-agent subjects in early child language: A crosslinguistic comparison. In Nelson, K. E., Aksu-Koc, A., & Johnson, C. (Eds.), Children’s language, Vol. 11: Interactional contributions to language development (pp. 4967). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A., & Aronsson, K. (2004). Repetition and joking in children’s second language conversations: Playful recyclings in an immersion classroom. Discourse Studies, 6(3), 373392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cekaite, A., & Aronsson, K. (2014). Language play, peer group improvisations, and L2 Learning. In Cekaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Grøver, V., & Teubal, E. (Eds.), Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other (pp. 194213). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P. M. (2009). Dialogic priming and the acquisition of argument marking in Korean. In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S. M., Nakamura, K., & Özcalişkan, S. (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the study of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 105117). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, J., & Kyratzis, A. (2001a). Child discourse. In Schiffrin, D., Tannen, D., & Hamilton, H. (Eds.), A handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 590611). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, J., & Kyratzis, A. (2001b). Pretend play: Trial ground for the simple present. In Putz, M., Niemeier, S., & Dirven, R. (Eds.), Applied cognitive linguistics I: Theory and language acquisition (pp. 4161). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Maynard, D. (1996). Format tying in discussion and argumentation among Italian and American children. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.). Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 157174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
de León, L. (2007). Parallelism, metalinguistic play, and the interactive emergence of Zinacantec Mayan siblings’ culture. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40(4), 405436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A corpus-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 131152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Du Bois, J. W. (2007). The stance triangle. In Englebretson, R. (Ed.), Stancetaking in discourse: Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction (pp. 139182. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Du Bois, J. W. (2014). Towards a dialogic syntax. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(3), 349410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duranti, A., & Ochs, E. (1996). Use and acquisition of genitive constructions in Samoan. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 175189). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1986). Activity structure as scaffolding for children’s second language learning. In Corsaro, W., Cook-Gumperz, J., & Streeck, J. (Eds.), Children’s language and children’s worlds (pp. 327358). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1991). Play in language development. In Scales, B., Almy, M., Nicolopoulou, A., & Ervin Tripp, S. M. (Eds.), Play and the social context of development in early care and education (pp. 8497). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1993). Constructing syntax from discourse. In Clark, E. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Stanford child language research forum (pp. 333341). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1996). Context in language. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 2136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (2012). Pragmatics as a facilitator for child syntax development. In Meeuwis, M. & Östman, J.-O. (Eds.), Pragmatizing understanding: Studies for Jef Verschueren (pp. 77100). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M., & Gordon, D. (1986). The development of requests. In Schiefelbush, R. L. (Ed.), Language competence: Assessment and intervention (pp. 6195). San Diego, CA: College Hill.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. & Miller, W. (1964). The development of grammar in child language. In Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (Eds.), The acquisition of language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29(1), 934.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.) (1977). Child discourse. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Evaldsson, A. C. (2002). Boys’ gossip telling: Staging identities and indexing (unacceptable) masculine behavior. Text, 22(2), 199225.Google Scholar
Gerhardt, J., & Savasir, I. (1986). The use of the simple present in the speech of two three-year-olds: Normativity not subjectivity. Language in Society, 15, 501536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2013). The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics, 46(1), 823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (2004). Participation. In Duranti, A. (Ed.), A companion to linguistic anthropology (pp. 222244). Malden, MA: Blackwel.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C., Goodwin, M. H., & Olsher, D. (2002). Producing Sense with Nonsense Syllables: Turn and Sequence in Conversations with a Man with Severe Aphasia. In Fox, B, Ford, C, & Thompson, S. A (Eds.), The Language of Turn and Sequence (pp. 5680). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (2006). The hidden life of girls: Games of stance, status, and exclusion. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (2007). Participation and embodied action in preadolescent girls’ assessment activity. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40 (4), 353375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1987). Children’s arguing. In Philips, S., Steele, S. & Tanz, C., (Eds.), Language, gender, and sex in comparative perspective (pp. 200–48). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., & Kyratzis, A. (Eds.) (2007). Children socializing children: Practices for negotiating the social order among peers. Special issue of Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40(4).Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., & Kyratzis, A. (2012). Peer language socialization. In Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), The handbook of language socialization (pp. 391419). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. (1979). Aspect and foregrounding in discourse. In Givon, T. (Ed.), Discourse and syntax (pp. 213241). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1977). Making it last: Repetition in children’s discourse. In Ervin-Tripp, S. M. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.). Child discourse (pp. 125139). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köymen, B., & Küntay, A (2013). Turkish children’s conversational oppositions: Usage of two discourse markers. Discourse Processes, 50, 388406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köymen, S. B., & Kyratzis, A. (2009). Format tying and the acquisition of syntax in toddlers’ peer interactions. Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 202210). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Köymen, B., & Kyratzis, A. (2014). Dialogic syntax and complement constructions in toddlers’ peer interactions. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(3), 497521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A., & Senay, I. (2003). Narratives beget narratives: Rounds of stories in Turkish preschool conversations. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 559587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1996). Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 157174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kyratzis, A. (2007). Using the social organizational affordances of pretend play in American preschool girls’ interactions. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40(4), 321352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyratzis, A. (2009). “He take one of my tools!” vs. “I’m building”: Transitivity and the grammar of accusing, commanding, and perspective-sharing in toddler’s peer disputes. In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S. M., Nakamura, K., & Özcalişkan, Ş. (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the study of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 4154). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kyratzis, A. (2014). Peer interaction, framing, and literacy in preschool bilingual pretend play. In Cekaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Grøver, V., & Teubal, E. (Eds.), Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other (pp. 129147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyratzis, A., & Cook-Gumperz, J. (2015). Child discourse. In Tannen, D., Hamilton, H., & Schiffrin, D. (Eds.), The handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 681704). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyratzis, A., Guo, J., & Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1990). Pragmatic conventions influencing children’s use of causal expressions in natural discourse. In Proceedings of the sixteenth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 16, 205215. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Kyratzis, A., Ross, T S., & Köymen, S. B. (2010). Validating justifications in preschool girls’ and boys’ friendship group talk: Implications for linguistic and socio-cognitive development. Journal of Child Language, 37(1), 115144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langacker, R. W. (2001). Cognitive linguistics, language pedagogy, and the English present tense. In Putz, M., Niemeier, S., & Dirven, R. (Eds.), Applied cognitive linguistics I: Theory and language acquisition (pp. 439). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1/2), 1135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., Brockmeyer, C., de Sá, A., & Ilgaz, H. (2014). Narrative performance, peer group culture, and narrative development in a preschool classroom. In Cekaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Grøver, V., & Teubal, E. (Eds.). Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other (pp. 4262). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (Eds.). (1979). Developmental pragmatics. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Shweder, R. & LeVine, R. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp. 276320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (2012). The theory of language socialization. In Duranti, A., Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (Eds.), The handbook of language socialization (pp. 122). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1932, 1997). The moral judgment of the child. London: Free Press.Google Scholar
Reynolds, J. F. (2007). “Buenos dias/((Military salute))”: The natural history of a coined insult. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40(4). 437465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. 1996. The microgenesis of competence: Methodology in language socialization. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context and language: Essays in Honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 251–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1981). The origins of grammatical encoding of events. In Deutsch, W. (Ed.), The child’s construction of language (pp. 185200). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 11571256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking.” In Gumperz, J. J. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity. Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language, 17 (pp. 7096). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1999). Social perspectives on the emergence of language. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 257276). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thompson, S. A., & Hopper, P. J. (2001). Transitivity, clause structure, and argument structure: Evidence from conversation. In Bybee, J. L. & Hopper, P. J. (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 2760). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
White, H. (1980). The value of narrativity in the representation of reality. In Mitchell, W. J. T. (Ed.), On narrative (pp. 123). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

References

Amsel, E., & Smetana, J. (Eds.). (2011). Adolescent vulnerabilities and opportunities: Developmental and constructivist perspectives. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). The problem of speech genres. In Emerson, C. & Holquist, M. (Eds.), Speech genres and other late essays (pp. 60102). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bamberg, M. (2004a). Considering counter-narratives. In Bamberg, M. & Andrews, M. (Eds.), Considering counter-narratives: Narrating, resisting, making sense (pp. 351371). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M. (2004b). Positioning with Davey Hogan: Stories, tellings, and identities. In Daiute, C. & Lightfoot, C. (Eds.), Narrative analysis: Studying the development of individuals in society (pp. 135158). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Bamberg, M., & Georgakopoulou, A. (2008). Small stories as a new perspective in narrative and identity analysis. Text & Talk, 28(3), 377396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkenkotter, C., & Huckin, T. (1995). Genre knowledge in disciplinary communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Betancourt, T. S., Ito, B. S., Lilienthal, G. M., Abdi, S., Agalab, N., & Ellis, H. (2015). We left one war and came to another: Resource loss, acculturative stress, and caregiver–child relationships in Somali refugee families. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 21(1), 114125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (2002). Making stories: Law, literature, life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Daiute, C. (2004). Creative use of cultural genres. In Daiute, C & Lightfoot, C (Eds.) Narrative analysis: Studying the development of individuals in society (pp. 111134). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C. (2010). Human development and political violence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C. (2011). Trouble – In, around, and between narratives. Narrative Inquiry, 21(2), 329336. doi 10.1075/ni.21.2.11daiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C. (2014). Narrative inquiry: A dynamic approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C., & Kreniske, P. (2016). Hopes, misunderstandings, and possibilities of narrating for inclusive education. In Surian, A (Ed.), Proceedings of “Open Spaces for Interaction and Learning Diversities” (pp. 5367). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C., & Nelson, K. A. (1997). Making sense of the sense-making function of narrative evaluation. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7(1–4), 207215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daiute, C., Todorova, R. S., & Kovacs-Cerovic, T. (2015). Narrating to manage participation and power relations in an education reform program. Language & Communication, 45, 4658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, A., & Zittoun, T. (2013) Meaning making in motion: Bodies and minds moving through institutional and semiotic structures. Culture & Psychology, 19(4), 518532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, D. (2007). A brief history of neo-liberalism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heft, H. (2007). The social construction of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecological Psychology, 19(2), 85105.Google Scholar
Honwana, A. (2006). Child soldiers: Community healing and rituals in Mozambique and Angola. In Daiute, C., Beykont, Z., Nucci, L., & Higson-Smith, C. (Eds.), International perspectives on youth conflict and development (pp. 225245). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotsou, I., Nelis, D., Gregoire, J., & Mikolajczak, M. (2011). Emotional plasticity: Conditions and effects of improving emotional competence in adulthood. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 827839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kress, G. (2014). Genre as social process. In Cope, B. & Kalantzis, M. (Eds.). The powers of literacy (RLE Edu I): A genre approach to teaching writing (pp. 2237). Oxon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (2008) Formal operations from a twenty-first century perspective. Human Development, 51, 4855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucic, L, (2013). Use of evaluative devices by youth for sense-making of cultural diverse interpersonal interactions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37(4), 434449. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2005). The redemptive self: Stories Americans live by. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1998). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ogle, C. M., Rubin, D. C., & Siegler, I. C. (2013). The Impact of the developmental timing of trauma exposure on PTSD symptoms and psychosocial functioning among older adults. Developmental Psychology, 49(11), 21912200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overton, W. F., & Lerner, R. M. (2014). Fundamental concepts and methods in developmental science: A relational perspective. Research in Human Development, 11(1), 6373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1968). Six psychological studies. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Sen, A. (2000). Development as freedom. New York, NY: Anchor Press.Google Scholar
Sen, A. (2009). The idea of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Smorti, A., Del Buffa, O., & Matteini, C. (2007). Narrazione autobiografica di eventi dolorosi: Analisi degli aspetti formali e contenutistici del testo [Autobiographical narrative of the painful events: Analysis of the formal aspects and content of the text]. Rassegna di Psicologia, 24(3), 1133.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: Age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 343355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (2002). Voices of collective remembering. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
UNHCR.org (2014). The world at war: Global trends, forced displacement in 2014: www.unhcr.org.Google Scholar
Wortham, S. (2001). Narratives in action: A strategy for research and analysis. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×