Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T04:27:43.655Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Developmental Evolution: Rethinking Stability and Variation in Biological Systems

from Part I - Cognitive Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amundson, R. (2005). The changing role of the embryo in evolutionary thought: Roots of evo-devo. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Arthur, W. (2004). Biased embryos and evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioral inheritance in evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Badyaev, A. (2009). Evolutionary significance of phenotypic accommodation in novel environments: An empirical test of the Baldwin effect. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 11251141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balakrishnan, C. N., & Sorenson, M. D. (2006). Premating reproductive isolation among sympatric indigobird species and host races. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 473478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G., & Gluckman, P. (2011). Plasticity, robustness, development, and evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bearhop, S., Fiedler, W., Furness, R. W., Votier, S. C., Waldron, S., Newton, J., … & Farnsworth, K. (2005). Assortative mating as a mechanism for rapid evolution of a migratory divide. Science, 310, 502504.Google Scholar
Belyaev, D. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. Journal of Heredity, 70, 301308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48, 647657.Google Scholar
Blumberg, M. S. (2005). Basic instinct: The genesis of novel behavior. New York, NY: Thunder’s Mouth Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 130.Google Scholar
Casey, M. B, & Karpinski, S. (1999). The development of turning bias is influenced by prenatal visual experience in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). The Psychological Record, 49, 6774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, M. B., & Lickliter, R. (1998). Prenatal visual experience influences the development of turning bias in bobwhite quail chicks (Colinus virginianus). Developmental Psychobiology, 32, 327338.Google Scholar
Clark, M. M., & Galef, B. (1981). Environmental influence on development, behavior, and endocrine morphology of gerbils. Physiology & Behavior, 27, 761765.Google Scholar
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., Goetz, C. D., Lewis, D. M. … & Buss, D. M. (2010). Evolutionary psychology: Controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. American Psychologist, 65, 110126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (1975). The selfish gene. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Beer, G. (1940). Embryos and ancestors. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Denenberg, V. H., & Rosenberg, K. M. (1967). Nongenetic transmission of information. Nature, 216, 549550.Google Scholar
Denenberg, V. H., & Whimbey, A. E. (1963). Behavior of adult rats is modified by the experiences their mothers had as infants. Science, 142, 11921193.Google Scholar
Deng, C., & Rogers, L. J. (2002). Social recognition and approach in the chick: Lateralization and effect of visual experience. Animal Behaviour, 63, 697706.Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, T. (1937). Genetics and the origin of species, 1st edn. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Duntley, J., & Buss, D. (2008). Evolutionary psychology is a metatheory for psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 19, 3034.Google Scholar
Feder, J. L., Roethele, J. B., Wlazlo, B., & Berlocher, S. H. (1997). Selective maintenance of allozyme differences among sympatric host races of the apple maggot fly. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 94, 1141711421.Google Scholar
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 11551158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garstang, W. (1922). The theory of recapitulation: A critical re-statement of the biogenetic law. Journal of the Linnean Society of London, Zoology, 35, 81101.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Huffman, K. J. (2002). Brain and cognitive evolution: forms of modularity and functions of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 667698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, S. F., & Epel, D. (2009). Ecological developmental biology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, R. (1940). The material basis of evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1971). The development of species-identification in birds. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavior development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 413.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature-nurture: Prenatal roots of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2002). Developmental-behavioral initiation of evolutionary change. Psychological Review, 109, 211218.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. E., & Stotz, K. (2013). Genetics and philosophy: An introduction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, B. K., Pearson, R. D., & Müller, G. B. (2004). Environment, development, and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harshaw, C., & Lickliter, R. (2007). Interactive and vicarious acquisition of auditory preferences in Northern bobwhite chicks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 320331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harshaw, C., & Lickliter, R. (2011). Biased embryos: Prenatal experience alters the postnatal malleability of auditory preferences in bobwhite quail. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 291302.Google Scholar
Harshaw, C., Tourgeman, I., & Lickliter, R. (2008). Stimulus contingency and the malleability of species-typical auditory preferences in Northern bobwhite hatchlings. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 460472.Google Scholar
Huxley, J. (1942). Evolution: The modern synthesis. London, UK: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2006). Genes as followers in evolution: A post-synthesis synthesis? Biology and Philosophy, 21, 143154.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D., & Gottlieb, G. (1990). Neophenogenesis: A developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 147, 471495.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D., & Lickliter, R. (2009). A developmental systems theory perspective on psychological change. In Spencer, J. P., Thomas, M., & McClelland, J. M. (Eds.), Toward a unified theory of development: Connectionism and dynamic systems theory re-considered (pp. 285296). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
King, J. A. (1968). Species specificity and early experience. In Newton, G. & Levine, S. (Eds.), Early experience and behavior (pp. 4264). Springfield, MA: Thomas.Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1967). The dynamics of behavior development: An epigenetic view. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Muller, G. B., Moczek, A., …. & Odling-Smee, J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions, and predictions. Proceeding of the Royal Society, B 282, 114.Google Scholar
Ledon-Rettig, C. C., Pfennig, D. W., Chunco, A., & Dworkin, I. (2014). Cryptic genetic variation in natural populations: A predictive framework. Integrated Comparative Biology, 54, 783793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerhman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 337363.Google Scholar
Lerner, R. M. & Benson, J. B. (2013). Embodiment and epigenesis: Theoretical and methodological issues in understanding the role of biology within the relational developmental system. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 44.Google Scholar
Levine, S. (1956). A further study of infantile handling and adult avoidance learning. Journal of Personality, 25, 7080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lickliter, R. (2005). Prenatal sensory ecology and experience: Implications for perceptual and behavioral development in precocial birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 235274.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R. (2009). The fallacy of partitioning: Epigenetics’ validation of the organism-environment system. Ecological Psychology, 21, 138146.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Berry, T. D. (1990). The phylogeny fallacy: Developmental psychology’s misapplication of evolutionary theory. Developmental Review, 10, 348364.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R. & Harshaw, C. (2010). Canalization and malleability reconsidered: The developmental basis of phenotypic stability and variability. In Hood, K. E., Halpern, C. T., Greenberg, G., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental science, behavior, and genetics (pp. 491526.). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 819835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2009). Rethinking epigenesis and evolution in light of developmental science. In Blumberg, M., Freeman, J., & Robinson, S. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 3047). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2013). A developmental evolutionary framework for psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17, 11841189.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2015). Biology, development, and human systems. In: Overton, W. F. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (Vol. Eds.) Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 1: Theory & method (7th edn., pp. 162207). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R., & Ness, J. (1990). Domestication and comparative psychology: Status and strategy. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 211218.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K., & Herschberger, S. L. (2005). Theoretical issues in the study of evolution and development. In Burgess, R. & MacDonald, K. (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human development (2nd edn., pp. 2172). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Malausa, T., Bethend, M. T., Bontemps, A., Bourguet, D., Cornuet, J. M., et al. (2005). Assortative mating in sympatric host races of the European corn borer. Science, 308, 258260.Google Scholar
Mameli, M. (2004). Nongenetic selection and nongenetic inheritance. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 55, 3571.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1985). Sexual selection, handicaps, and true fitness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 115, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1942). Systematics and the origins of species. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of Gene × Environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 4179.Google Scholar
Miller, D. B. (1997). The effects of nonobvious forms of experience on the development of instinctive behavior. In Dent-Reed, C. & Zukow-Goldring, P. (Eds), Evolving explanations of development (pp. 457507). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Minelli, A., & Pradeu, T. (2014). Towards a theory of development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moczek, A. (2015). Re-evaluating the environment in developmental evolution. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 3, 18.Google Scholar
Moczek, A., Sears, K., Stollewerk, A., Wittkopp, P., Diggle, P., Dworkin, I., … & Extavour, C. (2015). The significance and scope of evolutionary developmental biology: A vision for the 21st century. Evolution and Development, 17, 198219.Google Scholar
Moore, D. S. (2008). Espousing interactions and fielding reactions: Addressing laypeople’s beliefs about genetic determinism. Philosophical Psychology, 21, 331348.Google Scholar
Moore, D. S. (2015). The developing genome: An introduction to behavioral epigenetics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nijhout, H. F. (2003). Development and evolution of adaptive polyphenisms. Evolution & Development, 5, 918.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F. (2015). Processes, relations, and relational developmental systems. In Overton, W. F. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (Vol. Eds.) Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 1: Theory & method (7th edn., pp. 962). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2015). Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 1: Theory and method. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1993). Constraints and development. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 616.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Price, E. O. (1999). Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 65, 245271.Google Scholar
Price, E. O., & King, J. (1968). Domestication and adaptation. In Hafez, E. S. (Ed.), Adaptation of domestic animals (pp. 3445). Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.Google Scholar
Provine, W. (1971). The origins of theoretical population genetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Renner, M. J., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1987). Enriched and impoverished environments: Effects on brain and behavior. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Richardson, K. (1998). The origins of human potential: Evolution, development and psychology. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Robert, J. S. (2004). Embryology, epigenesis, and evolution: Taking development seriously. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, L. J. (1995). The development of brain and behavior in the chicken. Wallingford, UK: CAB International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronca, A. E., & Alberts, J. R. (1994). Sensory stimuli associated with gestation and parturition evoke cardiac and behavioral responses in fetal rats. Psychobiology, 55, 270282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozenzweig, M. R., & Bennett, E. L. (1972). Cerebral changes in rats exposed individually to an enriched environment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 80, 304313.Google Scholar
Russell, E. S. (1930). The interpretation of development and heredity. Oxford, England: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Schmalhausen, I. (1949). Factors of evolution: The theory of stabilizing selection. Oxford, England: Blakiston.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1944). Tempo and mode in evolution. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Spear, N. E, & McKenzie, D. L. (1994). Intersensory integration in the infant rat. In Lewkowicz, D. J. & Lickliter, R. (Eds.), The development of intersensory perception: Comparative perspectives (pp. 133161). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sporns, O. (2011). Networks of the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stotz, K. (2014). Extended evolutionary psychology: The importance of transgenerational developmental plasticity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 114.Google Scholar
Urban, J. B., Osgood, N. D., & Mabry, P. L. (2011). Developmental systems science: Exploring the application of systems science methods to developmental science questions. Research in Human Development, 8, 125.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1942). The epigenotype. Endeavour, 1, 1820.Google Scholar
Wallace, M. T., & Stein, B. E. (2007). Early experience determines how the senses will interact. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 921926.Google Scholar
West, M. J., King, A. P., & White, D. J. (2003). The case for developmental ecology. Animal Behaviour, 66, 617622.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×