Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-03T00:06:26.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Developing Motivation through Peer Interaction

A Cross-Cultural Analysis

from Part Two - Cultural Practice Motives and Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2011

Mariane Hedegaard
Affiliation:
University of Copenhagen
Anne Edwards
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Marilyn Fleer
Affiliation:
Monash University, Victoria
Get access

Summary

This chapter focuses on how peer interactions in play constitute a setting which contributes to the children’s acquisition of motives. We also argue that the study of peer interactions can shed light on two gaps which persist in the explanation of psychological development within the cultural-historical theory (Vygotsky, 1978; Wertsch, 1985). The first is that, although this theoretical approach has placed great emphasis on the social and cultural construction of psychological development, in this construction the role of adults such as caregivers and teachers is highlighted and that of peers has been neglected. The adults are seen as holding the keys to the cultural adult world, organising the interactions and regulating children’s behaviours in ways which give access to the cultural system of values and cultural norms. The second gap is, as Stetsenko and Arievitch (2004) point out, that although the cultural-historical theory recognises the active role of individuals in the appropriation of tools and systems of motives and cultural values, it has paid much less attention to the active role of individuals in creating and changing culture itself. Hedegaard (2005, 2008) has shown that the child is not a passive participant in these exchanges. The position, the way of behaving, and the degree of engagement of children in everyday practices can transform the practices substantially, playing a leading role in how these practices are developed. By focusing on peer interaction without adult direct supervision, we are able to analyse the active role of children acting in a setting in which they take over the values and norms of the adult world.

To illustrate this argumentation, we present a research on peer interaction in two different cultures. We will use the Corsaro’s (1997) concept of peer-cultures to analyse how children create and recreate the adult cultures in their interactions. The distinction between individualistic and collectivist cultures will be the backbone on which we will build our argument. Based on observational studies of pre-schoolers’ behaviour during free playtime at school, we show that the way children play and resolve their conflicts reflects and recreates the society’s motive system. We argue that peer interactions are a distinctive setting for socialisation and development in which children acquire the motives to act in relation to their adult world of reference.

Type
Chapter
Information
Motives in Children's Development
Cultural-Historical Approaches
, pp. 97 - 114
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Asher, S. R.Coie, J. D. 1990 Peer rejection in childhoodNew YorkCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Chen, X.French, D. C. 2008 Children’s social competence in cultural contextAnnual Review of Psychology 59 591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X.French, D. C.Schneider, B. 2006 Chen, X.French, D. C.Schneider, B.Peer relationships in cultural contextNew YorkCambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coie, J. D.Dodge, K. A. 1998 Damon, W.Eisenberg, N.Handbook of child psychology. Social, emotional and personality developmentNew YorkWileyGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. 1986 Adler, P. A.Adler, P.Sociological studies of child developmentGreenwich, CTJAI PressGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. 1988 Routines in the peer culture of American and Italian nursery school childrenSociology of Education 61 1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. 1992 Interpretive reproduction in children’s peer culturesSocial Psychological Quarterly 55 160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. 1997 The sociology of childhoodThousand Oaks, CASageGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A.Rizzo, T. A. 1988 Discussione and friendship: Socialization processes in the peer culture of Italian nursery school childrenAmerican Sociological Review 53 879CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A.Schwarz, K. 1991 Scales, B.Almy, M.Nicolopoulou, A.Erwing-Tripp, S.Play and the social context of development in early care and educationNew YorkTeachers College PressGoogle Scholar
Damon, W. 1994 Commentary to the paper of M. VerbaHuman Development 37 140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeRosier, M. E.Kupersmidt, J. B. 1991 Costa Rican children’s perceptions of their social networksDevelopmental Psychology 27 203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R.Garvey, C. 1981 Children’s use of verbal strategies in resolving conflictsDiscourse Processes 4 149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farver, J. A.Howes, C. 1988 Cross-cultural differences in social interaction. a comparation of American and Indonesian childrenJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 19 203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogel, A. 1993 Developing through relationships. Origins of communication, self, and cultureHertfordshireHarvester WheatsheafGoogle Scholar
Goudena, P. P.Sánchez, J. A. 1996 Peer interaction in Andalucia and Holland: A comparative studyInfancia y Aprendizaje 75 49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goudena, P. P.Vermande, M. M. 2002 A review of cross-cultural studies of observed peer interactionEarly Child Development and Care 172 141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudykunst, W. B.Bond, M. H. 1997 Berry, J. W.Segall, M. H.Kagitçibasi, Ç.Handbook of cross-cultural psychologyNeedham Heights, MAAllyn & BaconGoogle Scholar
Hartup, W. W. 1983 Damon, W.Social personality development: Essays on the growth of the childNew YorkW.W. Norton & CompanyGoogle Scholar
Hartup, W. W. 1990 Peer relationships and social skills in childhood: An international perspectiveHuman Development 33 221Google Scholar
Hedegaard, M. 2005 Strategies for dealing with conflicts in value positions between home and school: Influences on ethnic minority students’ development of motives and identityCulture Psychology 11 187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedegaard, M. 2008 van Oers, B.Wardekker, W.Elbers, E.Van der Weer, R.The transformation of learning. Advances in cultural-historical activity theoryCambridgeCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Hinde, R. A. 1987 Individuals, relationships and cultureCambridgeCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Hold-Cavell, B. C. L.Attili, G.Schleidt, M. 1986 A cross-cultural comparison of children’s behavior during their first year in a preschoolInternational Journal of Behavioral Development 9 471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagitçibasi, Ç 1997 Berry, J. W.Segall, M. H.Kagitçibasi, Ç.Handbook of cross-cultural psychologyNeedham Heights, MAAllyn & BaconGoogle Scholar
Kinoshita, Y.Saito, K.Matsunaga, A. 1993 Developmental changes in antecedents and outcomes of peer conflict among preschool children: A longitudinal studyJapanese Psychology Research 35 57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolominskii, I. L.Zhiznevskii, B. P. 1992 A sociopsychological analysis of conflict among children during playJournal of Russian and East European Psychology, 0 72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Lozano, V.Sánchez-Medina, J. A.Goudena, P. P. 2010 A cross-cultural study of observed conflicts between young childrenJournal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyGoogle Scholar
Phinney, J. S. 1986 The structure of 5 years-olds’ verbal quarrels with peers and siblingsJournal of Genetic Psychology 147 47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. 1990 Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social contextNew YorkOxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. 1995 Wertsch, J.del Río, P.Alvarez, A.Sociocultural studies of mindCambridgeCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. 2004 The cultural nature of human developmentNew YorkOxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Ross, D. D.Rogers, D. L. 1990 Social competence in kindergarten: Analyses of social negotiations during peer playEarly Child Development and Care 64 15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H. 1986 Gottfried, A. W.Brown, C. C.Play interactionsLexington, MALexington BooksGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H.Bukowsky, W.Parker, J. G. 1998 Damon, W.Eisenberg, N.Handbook of child psychology. Social, emotional and personality developmentNew YorkWileyGoogle Scholar
Sánchez, J. A.Martínez, V. 1997 Interacción social durante el recreo en preescolares. Un estudio observacional en un centro de acogida y una escuela ordinariaInfancia y Aprendizaje 77 113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shantz, C. U.Shantz, D. W. 1985 Berkowitz, M. W.Peer conflict and psychological growth: New directions for child developmentSan FranciscoJossey-BassGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, A. 1990 Pickle fights: Gendered talk in preschool disputesDiscourse Processes 13 5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stetsenko, A.Arievitch, I. M. 2004 The self in cultural-historical activity theory. Reclaiming the unity of social and individual dimensions of human developmentTheory & Psychology 14 475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Super, C. M.Harkness, S. 1986 The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and cultureInternational Journal of Behavioral Development 9 545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Super, C. M. 1997 Berry, J. W.Dasen, P. R.Saraswathi, T. S.Handbook of cross-cultural psychologyNeedham Heights, MAAllyn & BaconGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C. 1990 Berman, J. J.Nebraska symposium on motivation. Cross-cultural perspectives. 1989LincolnUniversity of Nebraska PressGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C. 1995 Individualism and collectivismBoulder, COWestview PressGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C.Marín, G.Hui, C.Lisansky, J.Ottati, V. 1984 Role perceptions of hispanic young adultsJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 15 297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. 1978 Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processesCambridge, MAHarvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. 1985 Vygotsky and the social formation of mindCambridge, MAHarvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V.Stone, C. A. 1985 Wertsch, J. V.Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectivesNew YorkCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Zinchenko, V. P. 1985 Wertsch, J. W.Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectivesNew YorkCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×