Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-rnpqb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-27T14:07:49.636Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Cognitive Predispositions and Cultural Transmission

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Pascal Boyer
Affiliation:
Washington University, St. Louis
James V. Wertsch
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Get access

Summary

We call those concepts and norms that seem to be shared within a group and differ from those of other groups “cultural.” We call concepts and norms “cultural” if people have them because other people in their group have them or had them before. This suggests that transmission of concepts and norms is at the heart of what constitutes human cultures.

To what extent does cultural transmission require memory? The answer of modern cognitive anthropology is slightly surprising. If we understand “memory” in the ordinary sense of information about past situations that we can access and consider explicitly, the answer is that cultural transmission does not actually require as much of that kind of memory as we would generally assume. Indeed, a great deal of cultural transmission takes place outside of explicit memories, as I explain here. But memory, for psychologists, includes more than just explicit memories (Roediger, Wheeler, & Rajaram, 1993). It comprises systematic information about the social and natural environment, what is called “semantic memory,” as well as the many skills and habits known as “procedural memory.” Once we understand memory, as psychologists do, as including all these processes beyond conscious inspection (Roediger, 1990), then memory really is the crux of cultural transmission. In the pages that follow, I will justify these statements on the basis of a few examples of cultural domains where the work of memory (in the wider sense) has been extensively studied.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

,American Psychiatric Association. (1995). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Ascher, M., & Ascher, R. (1981). Code of the quipu: A study in media, mathematics, and culture. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Atran, S. A. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural history. Towards an anthropology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atran, S. A. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 21(4), 547–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (1998). Cognitive constraints on Hindu concepts of the divine. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37, 608–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2002). Smart Gods, dumb Gods, and the role of social cognition in structuring ritual intuitions. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2(3), 183–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language & species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bloch, M. (1974). Symbols, song, dance, and features of articulation: Is religion an extreme form of traditional authority?European Journal of Sociology, 15, 55–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, M., & Parry, J. (Eds.). (1982). Death and the regeneration of life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. In Runciman, W. G., Smith, J. M. & Dunbar, R. I. M. (Eds.), Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and man (pp. 77–93). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1990). Tradition as truth and communication: A cognitive description of traditional discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P. (1994a). Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in culture and cognition (pp. 391–411). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1994b). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2000a). Cultural inheritance tracks and cognitive predispositions: The example of religious concepts. In Whitehouse, H. (Ed.), Mind, evolution and cultural transmission (pp. 57–89). Oxford: Berg Publishers.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2000b). Functional origins of religious concepts: Conceptual and strategic selection in evolved minds [Malinowski Lecture 1999]. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 6, 195–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. Evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P., & Lienard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior in humans? Precaution systems and action-parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 29, 1–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25, 535–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brodkin, K. (1998). How Jews became white folks and what that says about race in America. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A. (1998). The interpretation of semantic category-specific deficits: What do they reveal about the organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain?Neurocase: Case Studies in Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry, & Behavioural Neurology, 4(4–5), 265–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, M. J., & Ziolkowski, J. M. (2002). The medieval craft of memory: An anthology of texts and pictures. Philadelphia, PA.: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Cicero, M. T., & Caplan, H. (1964). Ad C. Herennium de ratione dicendi (Rhetorica ad Herennium). London, Cambridge, MA: W. Heinemann; Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7(1–2), 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Consider the source: The evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In Sperber, D. (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 53–115). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. (2007). Teachers in the wild. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 95–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniel, E. V. (1984). Fluid signs: Being a person the Tamil way. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech. The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dulaney, S., & Fiske, A. P. (1994). Cultural rituals and obsessive-compulsive disorder: Is there a common psychological mechanism?Ethos, 22(3), 243–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution. Genes, cultures and human diversity. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1947). The elementary forms of the religious life. New York: Collier Books.Google Scholar
Evans, A. (1908). The european diffusion of pictography and its bearings on the origin of script. Oxford: At the Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Evans, D. W., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, D., & Pauls, D. L. (1997). Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: The prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Development, 68(1), 58–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P., & Haslam, N. (1997). Is obsessive-compulsive disorder a pathology of the human disposition to perform socially meaningful rituals? Evidence of similar content. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 185(4), 211–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freud, S. (1906[1948]). Zwangsbehandlungen und Religionsübungen. In Freud, A. E. A. (Ed.), Gesammelte Werke von Sigmund Freud, chronologisch geordnet (Vol. 7, pp. 107–116). London: Imago Publishing.Google Scholar
Gelb, I. J. (1963). A study of writing. 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A revision of some Piagetian concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology, vol. 3: Cognitive Development (pp. 121–143). New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Gelman, S., & Coley, J. (1991). Language and categorisation: The acquisition of natural kind terms. In Gelman, S. & Byrnes, J. P. (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrealtions in development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Egyed, K., & Király, I. (2007). On pedagogy. Developmental Science, 10(1), 139–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gil-White, F. (2001). Are ethnic groups ‘species’ to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology, 42, 515–554.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1968). Literacy in traditional societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1986). The logic of writing and the organization of society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greif, A. (2006). Institutions and the path to the modern economy: lessons from medieval trade. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (Eds.), Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(1), 113–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haya, G., Holmer, N. M., & Wassén, H. (1947). Mu-Igala, or the way of Muu; a medicine song from the Cuna Indians of Panama. Göteborg: Elanders boktryckeri aktiebolag.Google Scholar
Hertz, R. (1960). A contribution to the study of the collective representation of death. London: Cohen & West.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1988). On acquiring social categories. Cognitive development and anthropological wisdom. Man, 23, 611–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1994). The acquisition of social categories. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Ed.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in culture and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture and the child's construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: The M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, C., & Laidlaw, J. (1993). Archetypal actions. A theory of ritual as a mode of action and the case of the Jain puja. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R., & Kirby, S. (1995). Neural preconditions for proto-language. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 18(1), 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). How a cockpit remembers its speeds. Cognitive Science, 19(3), 265–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keesing, R. (1982). Kwaio religion. The living and the dead in a Solomon Island society. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234–254). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2001). The social psychophysics of cooperation: Nonverbal communication in a public goods game. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(4), 241–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 187–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(26), 15387–15392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4–5), 727–738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. S., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Explaining clustering in social networks: Towards an evolutionary theory of cascading benefits. Managerial & Decision Economics, 27(2–3), 173–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lienard, P. (2003). Le comportement rituel: Communication, cognition et action. Générations, âges et territoire: contribution à l'ethnographie de deux populations du Cercle Karimojong (Les Turkana du Kenya et les Nyangatom d'Ethiopie). Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles.Google Scholar
Lienard, P., & Boyer, P. (2006). Why cultural rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behaviour. American Anthropologist, 108(4), 814–827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marks, J. (1995). Human biodiversity: Genes, race, and history. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Maryanski, A., & Turner, J. H. (1992). The social cage. Human nature and the evolution of society. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Mathews, C. A., Jang, K. L., Hami, S., & Stein, M. B. (2004). The structure of obsessionality among young adults. Depression & Anxiety, 20(2), 77–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maybin, J. (1994). Language and literacy in social practice: A reader. Clevedon, Avon, England; Philadelphia: Multingual Matters Ltd. in association with the Open University.Google Scholar
Metcalf, P., & Huntington, R. (1991). Celebrations of death. The anthropology of mortuary ritual (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S. J. (1996). The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Nemeroff, C. J. (1995). Magical thinking about illness virulence conceptions of germs from “Safe” versus “Dangerous” others. Health Psychology, 14(2), 147–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordenskiöld, E. (1938). An historical and ethnological survey of the Cuna Indians [arranged and edited by Henry Wassén]. Göteborg: Göteborgs Museum, Etnografiska Advelningen.Google Scholar
Ong, W. J. (1977). Interfaces of the word: Studies in the evolution of consciousness and culture. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. London; New York: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, D. (1993). The interpretation of caste. Oxford; New York: Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reverte, J. M. (1968). Literatura Oral de los Indios Cuna. Panama.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1996). The origins of virtue (1st ed.). London; New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L.. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45(9), 1043–1056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L., Wheeler, M. A., & Rajaram, S. (1993). Remembering, knowing, and reconstructing the past. In Douglas, L. M. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Advances in research and theory, (Vol. 30, pp. 97–134): Academic Press, Inc, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1990). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In Semin, K. F. G. (Ed.), Language and social cognition. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–595). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behav Res Ther, 23(5), 571–583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Severi, C. (1987). The invisible path. Ritual representation of suffering in cuna traditional thought. Res. Anthropology and Aesthetics, 14, 66–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Severi, C. (2002). Memory, reflexivity and belief reflections on the ritual use of language. Social Anthropology, 10(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance. An intergroup theory of social oppression and hierarchy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegal, M. (1988). Children's knowledge of contagion and contamination as causes of illness. Child Development, 59, 1353–1359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, J. D. (1984). The memory palace of Matteo Ricci. New York: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology. Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man, 20, 73–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (2006a). Conceptual tools for a naturalistic approach to cultural evolution. In Levinson, S. C. & Pierre, J. (Eds.), Evolution and culture: A Fyssen foundation symposium. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (Ed.). (2006b). An evolutionary perspective on testimony and argumentation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 40–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance. Communication and cognition (2nd ed.]. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stoler, A. L. (1991). Sexual affronts and racial frontiers: National identity, ‘mixed bloods’ and the cultural genealogies of Europeans in colonial Southeast Asia. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Szechtman, H., & Woody, E. (2004). Obsessive-compulsive disorder as a disturbance of security motivation. Psychological Review, 111(1), 111–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in inter-group discrimination. Scientific American, 223, 96–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & et al. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. (pp. 19–136). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2001). Origin of autonoesis in episodic memory. In Roediger, H. L. I. & Nairne, J. (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 17–34). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge. Morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In Damon, W. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 863–932). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ursu, S., Stenger, V. A., Shear, M. K., Jones, M. R., & Carter, C. S. (2003). Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Sciences, 14(4), 347–353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301–1303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Erskine, J. A. K. (2003). Voluntary involuntariness: Thought suppression and the regulation of the experience of will. Consciousness & Cognition: An International Journal, 12(4), 684–694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Schneider, D. J. (2003). The white bear story. Psychological Inquiry, 14(3–4), 326–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2000). Arguments and icons. Divergent modes of religiosity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Zalla, T., Verlut, I., Franck, N., Puzenat, D., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Perception of dynamic action in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 128(1), 39–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zohar, A. H., & Felz, L. (2001). Ritualistic behavior in young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(2), 121–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×