Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T03:28:43.064Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - The Warped Forge

from Part II - Interfaces

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2018

Ángel J. Gallego
Affiliation:
Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona
Roger Martin
Affiliation:
Yokohama National University, Japan
Get access

Summary

In this paper we argue that categorization is module independent. Following Uriagereka (1996) we argue that concepts are categorized according to a implicational hierarchy in a scale of structural complexity, expressed in dimensions. Categorization has consequences internal to each module. Concepts exhibit a behavior specific to its category: mass vs. count concepts, for instance, differ in its syntax, but also in the way we interact with them (planning) or in the way we see them. This procedure serves as a base for a porous modular concepticon of our cognition, a kind of bare-phrase structure solution for cognitive interfaces. Furthermore, in this paper we argue against Distributional Morphology for grammatical categories. We show that at least for non derived words cognitive categorization determines the grammatical category of lexical items.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baker, Mark C. 1996. The Polysynthetic Parameter. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Mark C. 2003. Lexical Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron Cohen, S., Leslie, Alan M., and Frith, Uta. 1985. “Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’?Cognition 21: 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berwick, Robert C., and Chomsky, Noam. 2016. Why Only Us: Language and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borer, Hagit. 2004. Structuring Sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bustos, Pablo, Manso, Luis, Bandera, J. P., Romero-Garcés, Adrián, Calderita, Luis, Marfil, Rebeca and Bandera, Antonio. 2015. “A unified internal representation of the outer world for social robotics,” in Reis, L. P., Moreira, A. P., Lima, P. U., Montano, L., and Muñoz-Martínez, V. (eds.), ROBOTS 2015: Second Iberian Robotics Conference. Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 733744.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Embick, David. 2015. “The morpheme: a theoretical introduction.” Ms., University of Pennsylvania.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embick, David, and Marantz, Alec. 2008. “Architecture and blocking.” Linguistic Inquiry 39, 1: 153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, Kenneth, and Keyser, Samuel Jay. 1992. “On argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic relations,” in Hale and Keyser (eds.), The View from Building 20. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 53110.Google Scholar
Harnard, Stevan. 2005. “To cognize is to categorize: cognition is categorization,” in Cohen, Henri, and Lefebvre, Claire (eds.), Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science. Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 2042.Google Scholar
Heinrich, Joseph. 2015. The Secret of Our Success. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, Douglas. 1995. Fluid Concepts and Creative Analogies. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hohwy, Jakob. 2013. The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, Luis. 2012. Indefinite Objects: Scrambling, Choice Functions and Differential Marking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montalbetti, Mario. 1984. “After binding.” Ph.D. thesis, MIT.Google Scholar
Ormazabal, Javier, and Romero, Juan. 2007. “The object agreement constraint.” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 25, 2: 315347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ormazabal, Javier, and Romero, Juan. 2013. “Object clitics, agreement, and dialectal variation.” Probus 25, 2: 301344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uriagereka, Juan. 1996. “Warps: some thoughts on categorization.” Cuadernos de Lingüística del I.U. Ortega y Gasset 4: 138.Google Scholar
Uriagereka, Juan. 1999. “Multiple spell out,” in Epstein, S. D., and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Working Minimalism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 251282.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×