Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T07:09:18.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Intuitive Judgments about Sample Size

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2010

Peter Sedlmeier
Affiliation:
Chemnitz University of Technology, Germany
Klaus Fiedler
Affiliation:
Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg, Germany
Peter Juslin
Affiliation:
Umeå Universitet, Sweden
Get access

Summary

Suppose a state election is coming up in a certain European country and somebody asks you if you would bet on whether the Social Democrats will beat the Christian Democrats again as they did in the last election. Shortly before Election Day, you are presented the results of two opinion polls, based on random samples of either 100 or 1,000 voters. In the smaller sample, the Social Democrats have an advantage of 4 percentage points whereas in the larger sample, the Christian Democrats lead by the same amount. Would you take the bet? You probably would not and I assume that this would be the choice of most people asked this question. As we will see later, your choice would be justified on both empirical and theoretical grounds.

This chapter offers an explanation for how and when the “naïve intuitive statistician” (Fiedler & Juslin, this volume) understands the impact that the size of a sample should have on estimates about population parameters such as proportions or means. I will argue that, in general, people know intuitively that larger (random) samples yield more exact estimates of population means and proportions than smaller samples and that one should have more confidence in these estimates the larger the size of the sample. My main claim is that the size-confidence intuition – an intuition that properly captures the relationship between sample size and accuracy of estimates – governs people's judgments about sample size. This intuition can be regarded as the result of associative learning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Cleveland, W. S. (1979). Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 74, 829–836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1962). The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 145–153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2004). Zum Verständnis von Zufall und Variabilität in empirischen Daten bei Schülern. [School students' understanding of chance and variability in empirical data]Unterrichtswissenschaft, 32, 169–191Google Scholar
Erlick, D. E. (1964). Absolute judgments of discrete quantities randomly distributed over time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 475–482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J.St, B. T., & Dusoir, A. E. (1977). Proportionality and sample size as factors in intuitive statistical judgement. Acta Psychologica, 41, 129–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J.St, B. T., & Pollard, P. (1985). Intuitive statistical inferences about normally distributed data. Acta Psychologica, 60, 57–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1991). The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An information loss account of distinctiveness-based illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 24–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1996). Explaining and simulating judgment biases as an aggregation phenomenon in probabilistic, multiple-cue environments. Psychological Review, 103, 193–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (2000). Beware of samples! A cognitive–ecological sampling approach to judgment biases. Psychological Review, 107, 659–676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiedler, K., Brinkmann, B., Betsch, R., & Wild, B. (2000). A sampling approach to biases in conditional probability judgments: Beyond baserate neglect and statistical format. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 1–20Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Freytag, P., & Plessner, H. (2002). Judgment biases in a simulated classroom – A cognitive-environmental approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 527–561CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbein, E. (1975). The intuitive sources of probabilistic thinking in children. Reidel: Dordrecht-HollandCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haberstroh, S., & Betsch, T. (2002). Online strategies versus memory-based strategies in frequency estimation. In Sedlmeier, P. & Betsch, T. (Eds.), Etc. Frequency processing and cognition (pp. 205–220). Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogarth, R. (2001). Educating intuition. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences. Scientific American, 246, 160–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klayman, J., & Ha, Y.-W. (1989). Hypothesis testing in rule discovery: Strategy, structure and content. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 596–604Google Scholar
Mazzoni, G., & Memon, A. (2003). Imagination can create false autobiographical memories. Psychological Science, 14, 186–188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nisbett, R. E., Krantz, D. H., Jepson, C., & Kunda, Z. (1983). The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakes, M. (1986). Statistical inference: A commentary for the social and behavioral sciences. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Reagan, R. T. (1989). Variations on a seminal demonstration of people's insensitivity to sample size. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 52–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruscio, J. (2003). Comparing Bayes's theorem to frequency-based approaches to teaching Bayesian reasoning. Teaching of Psychology, 30, 325–328Google Scholar
Sedlmeier, P. (1998). The distribution matters: Two types of sample-size tasks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11, 281–3013.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P. (1999). Improving statistical reasoning: Theoretical models and practical implications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P. (2002). Associative learning and frequency judgments: The PASS model. In Sedlmeier, P. & Betsch, T. (Eds.), Etc. Frequency processing and cognition (pp. 137–152). Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P. (2003). Simulation of estimates of relative frequency and probability for serially presented patterns. Unpublished raw data
Sedlmeier, P. (2005). From associations to intuitive judgment and decision making: Implicitly learning from experience. In Betsch, T. & Haberstroh, S. (Eds.), Experience-based decision making (pp. 83–99). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1989). Do studies of statistical power have an effect on the power of studies?Psychological Bulletin, 107, 309–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1997). Intuitions about sample size: The empirical law of large numbers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 33–513.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Was Bernoulli wrong? On intuitions about sample size. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 133–1393.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (2001). Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 380–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedlmeier, P., Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Are judgments of the positional frequencies of letters systematically biased due to availability?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 754–770Google Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Kılınç, B. (2004). The hazards of underspecified models: the case of symmetry in everyday predictions. Psychological Review, 111, 770–780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedlmeier, P., & Köhlers, D. (2001). Wahrscheinlichkeiten im Alltag: Statistik ohne Formeln. [Probabilities in everyday life: statistics without formula]. Braunschweig: Westermann (textbook with CD)Google Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Renkewitz, F. (2004). Attentional processes and judgments about frequency: Does more attention lead to higher estimates? Manuscript submitted for publicationGoogle Scholar
Shanks, D. R. (1995). The psychology of associative learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 105–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wassner, C., Martignon, L., & Sedlmeier, P. (2002). Entscheidungsfindung unter Unsicherheit als fächerübergreifende Kompetenz. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45 (Suppl.), 35–50Google Scholar
Wender, K. F., & Rothkegel, R. (2000). Subitizing and its subprocesses. Psychological Research, 64, 81–92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×