Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-89wxm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-07T21:21:25.835Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

23 - Fear, Greed, and Financial Crises: A Cognitive Neurosciences Perspective

from PART VIII - BEHAVIORAL FINANCE: THE PSYCHOLOGICAL DIMENSION OF SYSTEMIC RISK

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2013

Andrew Lo
Affiliation:
Usa
Jean-Pierre Fouque
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Joseph A. Langsam
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, College Park
Get access

Summary

Abstract Historical accounts of financial crises suggest that fear and greed are the common denominators of these disruptive events: periods of unchecked greed eventually lead to excessive leverage and unsustainable asset-price levels, and the inevitable collapse results in unbridled fear, which must subside before any recovery is possible. The cognitive neurosciences may provide some new insights into this boom/bust pattern through a deeper understanding of the dynamics of emotion and human behavior. In this chapter, I describe some recent research from the neurosciences literature on fear and reward learning, mirror neurons, theory of mind, and the link between emotion and rational behavior. By exploring the neuroscientific basis of cognition and behavior, we may be able to identify more fundamental drivers of financial crises, and improve our models and methods for dealing with them.

Introduction

In March 1933, unemployment in the United States was at an all-time high. Over 4,000 banks had failed during the previous two months. Bread lines stretched around entire blocks in the largest cities. The country was in the grip of the Great Depression. This was the context in which Franklin Delano Roosevelt delivered his first inaugural address to the American people as the 32nd president of the United States. He began his address not by discussing economic conditions, nor by laying out his proposal for the “New Deal”, but with a powerful observation that still resonates today: “So, first of all, let me assert my firm belief that the only thing we have to fear is fear itself – nameless, unreasoning, unjustified terror which paralyzes needed efforts to convert retreat into advance”.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbe, E., Khandani, A., and Lo, A. (2011). Privacy-preserving measures of systemic risk in the financial system. Preprint, MIT Laboratory for Financial Engineering.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30 285–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., and Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50 7–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benartzi, S. and Thaler, R. (2004). Save more tomorrow: Using behavioral economics to increase employee savings. Journal of Political Economy 112 S164–S187.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. (2008). Hierarchical models of behavior and the prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience 12 201–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breiter, Hans C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., and Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 30 619–639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, T. and Lo, A. (2011). The origin of behavior. Quarterly Journal of Finance 1 55–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C. and Dinc, S. (2011). Too many to fail? Evidence of regulatory forbearance when the banking sector is weak. Review of Financial Studies 24 1378–1405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., and Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: How neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature 43 9–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlsson, A., Lindqvist, M., and Magnusson, T. (1957). 3,4-Dihydroxyphenylalanine and 5-hydroxytryptophan as reserpine antagonists. Nature 180 1200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coates, J. and Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a london trading floor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105 6167–6172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. and Einav, L. (2003). The effect of mandatory seat belt laws on driving behavior and traffic fatalities. Review of Economics and Statistics 85 828–843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandall, R. and Graham, J. (1984). Automobile safety regulation and offsetting behavior: Some new empirical estimates. American Economic Review 74 328–331.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R., Tranel, D. and Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behaviour: Theory and preliminary testing. In Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, H.S., Levin, H.M., Eisenberg, and A.L., Benton, (eds). New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 217–229.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (1998). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior”. In Human Emotions: A Reader. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 122–135.Google Scholar
Danziger, S., Levav, J., and Avnaim-Pesso, L. (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 6889–6892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Becker, G. (1997) The Gift of Fear: Survival Signals that Protect us from Violence. Boston: Little Brown and Co.Google Scholar
Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L.Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research 91 176–180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N., Lieberman, M. and Williams, K. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302 290–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elster, J. (1998). Emotions and economic theory. Journal of Economic Literature 36 47–74.Google Scholar
Eslinger, P. and Damasio, A. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology 35 1731–1741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espinosa-Vega, A., Kahn, C., Matta, R., and Solé, J. (2011). Systemic Risk and Optimal Regulatory Architecture. International Monetary Fund Working Paper WP/11]193.CrossRef
Farmer, C., Lund, A., Trempel, R. and Braver, E. (1997). Fatal crashes of passenger vehicles before and after adding antilock braking systems. Accident Analysis and Prevention 29 745–757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finger, S. (1994). Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations into Brain Function. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, J.M. (1974). Recovery after severe injury to the head. In Readings in Behavior, Van der Kloot, W. G., Walcott, C., and Dane, B. (eds). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 291–309.Google Scholar
Huizinga, H. and Laeven, L. (2010). Bank valuation and regulatory forbearance during a financial crisis. European Banking Center Discussion Paper No. 2009–17. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1434359.
Jevons, W. S. (1871). The Theory of Political Economy. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kamstra, M., Kramer, L., and Levi, M. (2003). Winter blues: Seasonal affective disorder (sad) and stock market returns. American Economic Review 93 324–343.Google Scholar
Kapp, B. S., Frysinger, R. C., Gallagher, M., and Haselton, J. R. (1979). Amygdala central nucleus lesions: effects on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit. Physiology and Behavior 23 1109–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keynes, J. M. (1936). The General Theory of Employment Interest and Money. Republished in 1960. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Co.Google Scholar
Kinderman, P., Dunbar, R., and Bentall, R. P. (1998). Theory of mind deficits and causal attributions. British Journal of Psychology 89 191–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kindleberger, C. (1978). Manias, Panics, and Crashes: A History of Financial Crises. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kl'uver, H. and Bucy, P. (1937). “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobotomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology 119 352–353.Google Scholar
Knutson, B. and Bossaerts, P. (2007). Neural antecedents of financial decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience 27 8174–8177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhnen, C. M. and Knutson, B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking, Neuron 47 763–770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Lo, A. (1999). The three Ps of total risk management. Financial Analysts Journal 55 13–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lo, A. (2004). The adaptive markets hypothesis: Market efficiency from an evolutionary perspective. Journal of Portfolio Management 30 15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lo, A. (2005). Reconciling efficient markets with behavioral finance: The adaptive markets hypothesis. Journal of Investment Consulting 7 21–44.Google Scholar
Lo, A. (2010). A complete theory of human behavior. In American Economic Association, Ten Years and Beyond: Economists Answer NSF's Call for Long-Term Research Agendas. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1889318Google Scholar
Lo, A. and MacKinlay, C. (1999). A Non-Random Walk Down Wall Street. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lo, A. and Repin, D. (2002). The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 323–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, A., Repin, D. and Steenbarger, B. (2005). Fear and greed in financial markets: An online clinical study. American Economic Review 95 352–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (2000). Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. American Economic Review 90 426–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madrian, B., and Shea, D. (2001). The power of suggestion: inertia in 401(k) participation and savings behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics 116 1149–1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montague, P. R. and Berns, G.S. (2002). Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron 36 265–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, J., and Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 47 419–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J. and Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …” Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 39 437–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E. and Slovic, P. (2000). The springs of action: Affective and analytical information processing in choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26 1465–1475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ralson, A. (2004). Between a Rock and a Hard Place. New York: Atria Books.Google Scholar
Reinhart, Carmen M., and Rogoff, Kenneth S. (2009). This Time is Different: Eight Centuries of Financial Folly. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and Fabbri-Destro, M. (2010). Mirror neurons, from discovery to autism. Experimental Brain Research 200 223–237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. (1990). A theory of emotion, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. Cognition and Emotion 4 161–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls, E. (1992). Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amygdala. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, J., Aggelton (ed). New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 143–166.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. (1994). A theory of emotion and consciousness, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, M., Gazzaniga (ed). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1091–1106.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. (1999, The Brain and Emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, O. W. (1974, Awakenings. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Saver, J. L. and Damasio, A.R. (1991). Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia 29 1241–1249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoenemann, T., Sheehan, M., and Glotzer, D. (2005). Prefrontal white matter is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nature Neuroscience 8 242–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senior, C. (2006). CDOs explode after growing interest. FT Mandate April.Google Scholar
Shefrin, H. and Thaler, R. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry 26 609–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smaers, J., Steele, J., Case, C., Cowper, A., Amunts, K., and Zilles, K. (2011). Primate prefrontal cortex evolution: Human brains are the extreme of a lateralized ape trend. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 77 67–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobel, R. and Nesbit, T. (2007). Automobile safety regulation and the incentive to drive recklessly: Evidence from NASCAR. Southern Economic Journal 74 71–84.Google Scholar
Tierney, J. (2011). Do you suffer from decision fatigue?New York Times, August 17.Google Scholar
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40 655–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. (2000). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Williams, R. W. and Herrup, K. (1988). The control of neuron number. Annual Review of Neuroscience 11 423–453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, P., He, G., Shapira, N. A., Goodman, W. K., and Liu, Y., 2004). Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. NeuroReport 15 2347–2341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist 35 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist 39 117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×