Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T15:28:43.069Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Color categorization across cultures

from Part IV - Color categorization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Andrew J. Elliot
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Mark D. Fairchild
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Anna Franklin
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agrillo, C., and Roberson, D. R. (2009). Color language and color cognition: Brown and Lenneberg revisited. Visual Cognition, 17, 412–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bachy, R., Dias, J., Alleysson, D., and Bonnardel, V. (2012). Hue discrimination, unique hues and naming. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 29, A60–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baronchelli, A., Gong, T., Puglisi, A., and Loreto, V. (2010). Modeling the emergence of universality in color naming patterns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 2403–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2000). The architecture of the color centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 172–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, B., and Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bird, C. M., Berens, S. C., Horner, A. J., and Franklin, A. (2014). Categorical encoding of color in the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 4590–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., and Korda, N. O. (1984). Discrimination and matching within and between hues measured by reaction times: some implications for categorical perception and levels of information processing. Psychological Research, 46, 207–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boster, J. (1986). Can individuals recapitulate the evolutionary development of color lexicons? Ethnology, 25, 6178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boynton, R. M., and Olson, C. X. (1987). Locating basic colors in the OSA space. Color Research & Application, 12, 94105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, G. J., and Heeger, D. J. (2013). Categorical clustering of the neural representation of color. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 15454–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. M., Lindsey, D. T., and Guckes, K. M. (2011). Color names, color categories, and color-cued visual search: sometimes, color perception is not categorical. Journal of Vision, 11, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., and Lenneberg, E. (1954). A study in language and cognition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 454–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casson, R. W., and Gardner, P. M. (1992). On brightness and color categories: additional data. Current Anthropology, 33, 395–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claidière, N., Jraissati, Y., and Chevallier, C. (2008). A color sorting task reveals the limits of the universalist/relativist dichotomy: color categories can be both language specific and perceptual. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 211–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, A., Holmes, A., Davies, I. R., and Franklin, A. (2010). Color categories affect pre-attentive color perception. Biological Psychology, 85, 275–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collier, G. A., Dorflinger, G. K., Gulick, T. A., Johnson, D. L., McCorkle, C., Meyer, M. A., Wood, D. D., et al. (1976). Language, 52, 884–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conklin, H. C. (1973). Color categorization. American Anthropologist, 75, 931–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropper, S. J., Kvansakul, J. G. S., and Little, D. R. (2013 ). The categorisation of non-categorical colours: a novel paradigm in colour perception. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e59945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czigler, I, Balazs, L., and Pato, L. G. (2004). Visual change detection: event-related potentials are dependent on stimulus location in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 364, 149–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danilova, M. V., and Mollon, J. D. (2012). Foveal color perception: minimal thresholds at a boundary between perceptual categories. Vision Research, 62, 162–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daoutis, C. A., Franklin, A., Riddett, A., Clifford, C., and Davies, I. R. L. (2006a). Categorical effects in children’s color search: a crosslinguistic comparison. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 129.Google Scholar
Daoutis, C. A, Pilling, M., and Davies, I. R. L. (2006b). Categorical effects in visual search for color. Visual Cognition, 14, 217–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, J. (2001). Language and perceptual categories. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 382–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, J., Davies, I., and Roberson, D. (1999). Colour categories in a stone-age tribe. Nature, 398, 203–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, J., and Fagot, J. (2010). Cross-species assessment of the linguistic origins of color categories. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 5, 100–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, J., Fonteneau, E., and Fagot, J. (2008). Local and global processing: observations from a remote culture. Cognition, 108, 702–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidoff, J., Goldstein, J., Tharp, I., Wakui, E., and Fagot, J. (2012). Perceptual and categorical judgements of color similarity. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 871–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, J., and Ostergaard, A. L. (1988). The role of color in categorical judgements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 533–44.Google Scholar
Davidoff, J., and Roberson, D. (2004). Preserved thematic and impaired taxonomic categorisation: a case study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 137–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, I. R. L., and Corbett, G. G. (1997). A cross-cultural study of colour-grouping: evidence for weak linguistic relativity. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 493517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, I. R. L., and Corbett, G. G. (1998). A cross-cultural study of color-grouping: tests of the perceptual-physiology account of color universals. Ethos, 26, 338–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, I. R. L., Sowden, P., Jerrett, D. T., Jerrett, T., and Corbett, G. G. (1998). A cross-cultural study of English and Setswana speakers on a colour triads task: a test of the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Valois, R. L., and de Valois, K. K. (1990). Spatial Vision. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Drivonikou, G. V., Kay, P., Regier, T., Ivry, R. B., Gilbert, A. L., Franklin, A., and Davies, I. R. L. (2007). Further evidence for lateralization of Whorfian effects to the right visual field. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 10971102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46, 621–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonteneau, J., and Davidoff, J. (2007). Neural correlates of color categories. NeuroReport, 18, 1323–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, A., Clifford, A., Williamson, E., and Davies, I. R. L. (2005). Color term knowledge does not affect categorical perception of color in toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 114–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gage, J. (1999). Colour and Meaning. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., and Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 489–94.Google Scholar
Goldstein, J., Davidoff, J., and Roberson, D. (2009). Knowing color terms enhances recognition: further evidence from English and Himba. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 219–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and Language Disturbances. New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 178200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstone, R. L., and Hendrickson, A. T. (2010). Categorical perception. Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 6978.Google ScholarPubMed
Hanley, J. R., and Roberson, D. (2011). Categorical perception effects reflect differences in typicality on within-category trials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 355–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, J. L., Frederick, C. M., Kay, P., and Werner, J. S. (2005). Color naming, lens aging, and grue: what the optics of the aging eye can teach us about color language. Psychological Science, 16, 321–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, S. (1973). Universal aspects of learning color codes: a study in two cultures. Ethos, 1, 175200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S. (1987). Psychophysical and cognitive aspects of categorical perception: a critical overview. In Harnad, S. (ed.), Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition (pp. 125). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
He, X., Witzel, C., Forder, L., Clifford, A., and Franklin, A. (2013). Color categories only affect post-perceptual processes when same- and different-color category colors are equally discriminable. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 31, 322–31.Google Scholar
Heider, E. R., and Olivier, D. C. (1972). The structure of the color space in naming and memory for two languages. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 337–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendrickson, A. T., Carvalho, P. F., and Goldstone, R. L. (2012). Going to extremes: the influence of unsupervised categories on the mental caricaturization of faces and asymmetries in perceptual discrimination. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1662–7). Sapporo, Japan: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Heywood, C. A., Cowey, A., and Newcombe, F. (1991). Chromatic discrimination in a cortically colour blind observer. European Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 802–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, A., Franklin, A., Clifford, A., and Davies, I. (2009). Neuropsychological evidence for categorical perception of color. Brain and Cognition, 69, 426–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., and Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Jameson, K. A., Bimler, D., Dedrick, D., and Roberson, D. (2007). Considering the prevalence of “stimulus error” in color naming research. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 119–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jameson, K. A., and D’Andrade, R. G. (1997). It’s not really red, green, yellow, blue: an inquiry into perceptual color space. InHardin, C. L. and Maffi, L., Color Categories in Thought and Language (pp. 295–319). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jraissati, Y., Wakui, E., Decock, L., and Douven, I. (2012). Constraints on color category formation. International Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 26, 171–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., Berlin, B., Maffi, L., Merrifield, W. R., and Cook, R. (2009). The World Color Survey. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kay, P., and Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 6578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., and Maffi, L. (1999). Color appearance and the emergence and evolution of basic color lexicons. American Anthropologist, 101, 743–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., and McDaniel, C. K. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms. Language, 54, 610–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., and Regier, T. (2006). Language, thought, and color: recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 51–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koida, K., and Komatsu, H. (2007). Effects of task demands on the responses of color-selective neurons in the inferior temporal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 108–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komarova, N. L., Jameson, K. A., and Narens, L. (2007). Evolutionary models of color categorization based on discrimination. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 51, 359–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehni, R. G. (2004). Variability in unique hue selection: a surprising phenomenon. Color Research & Application, 29, 158–62.Google Scholar
Kuehni, R. G. (2007). Nature and culture: an analysis of individual focal color choices in World Color Survey languages. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 151–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuschel, R., and Monberg, T. (1974). “We don’t talk much about colour here”: the semantics of colour naming on Bellona Island. Man, 9, 213–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lantz, D., and Stefflre, V. (1964). Language and cognition revisited. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 472–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, G., Davies, I., and Andrews, C. (1995). Linguistic structure and non-linguistic cognition: English and Russian blues compared. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 5994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazar-Meyn, H. A. (2004). Color naming: “grue” in the Celtic languages of the British Isles. Psychological Science, 15, 288, 291–4.Google ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Yélî Dnye and the theory of basic colors. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 10, 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, H., Luo, M. R., MacDonald, L. W., and Tarrant, W. S. (2001). A cross-cultural color naming study. I. Using an unconstrained method. Color Research & Application, 26, 4060.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsey, D. T., and Brown, A. M. (2002). Naming and the photoxic effects of sunlight on the eye. Psychological Science, 13, 506–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsey, D. T., and Brown, A. M. (2006). Universality of color names. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 16608–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Lindsey, D. T., Brown, A. M., Reijnen, E., Rich, A. N., Kuzmova, Y. I., and Wolfe, J. M. (2010). Color channels, not color appearance or color categories, guide visual search for desaturated color targets. Psychological Science, 21, 1208–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linnell, K. J., Caparos, S., De Fockert, J., and Davidoff, J. (2013). Urbanisation decreases attentional engagement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1232–47.Google Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26, 291312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A., and Schweder, R. A. (1979). Whorf and his critics: linguistic and nonlinguistic influences on color memory. American Anthropologist, 81, 581605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
MacLaury, R. E. (1997). Color and Cognition in Mesoamerica: Constructing Categories as Vantages. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, N. B. (1972). Color and color terminology. Journal of Linguistics, 8, 2133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mo, L., Xu, G., Kay, P., and Tan, L. H. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for the left-lateralized effect of language on preattentive categorical perception of color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 108, 14026–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Munnich, E., and Landau, B. (2003). The effects of spatial language on spatial representation: setting some boundaries. In Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 113–55). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Osborne, R. (2013). Books on Color Since 1500: A History and Bibliography of Color Literature. Boca Raton, FL: Universal-Publishers.Google Scholar
Özgen, E., and Davies, I. R. L. (2002). Acquisition of categorical color perception: a perceptual learning approach to the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 477–93.Google Scholar
Philipona, D. L., and O’Regan, D. K. (2006). Color naming, unique hues, and hue cancellation predicted from singularities in reflectance properties. Visual Neuroscience, 23, 331–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilling, M., and Davies, I. R. L. (2004). Linguistic relativism and color cognition. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 429–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisoni, D. B. (1973). Auditory and phonetic memory codes in the discrimination of consonants and vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 13, 253–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pisoni, D. B., and Tash, J. (1974). Reaction times to comparisons within and across phonetic categories. Perception and Psychophysics, 15, 285–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regier, T., and Kay, P. (2004). Color naming and sunlight: commentary on Lindsey and Brown (2002). Psychological Science, 15, 289–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regier, T., Kay, P., and Cook, R. S. (2005). Focal colors are universal after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 8386–91.Google Scholar
Regier, T., Kay, P., and Khetarpal, N. (2007). Color naming and the shape of color space. Language, 85, 884–92.Google Scholar
Regier, T., Kay, P., and Khetarpal, N. (2009). Color naming reflects optimal divisions of color space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 1436–41.Google Scholar
Rivers, W. H. R. (1905). Observations on the senses of the Todas. British Journal of Psychology, 1, 321–96.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., and Braisby, N. (1999). Similarity and categorisation: neuropsychological evidence for a dissociation in explicit categorisation tasks. Cognition, 71, 142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I., and Shapiro, L. R. (2004). The development of color categories in two languages: a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 554–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I., and Shapiro, L. R. (2005). Color categories: evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 378441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davies, I. R. L., Corbett, G. G., and Vandervyver, M. (2005). Free-sorting of colors across cultures: are there universal grounds for grouping? Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 349–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberson, D., Davies, I., and Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 369–98.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Hanley, J. R., and Pak, H. (2009). Thresholds for color discrimination in English and Korean speakers. Cognition, 112, 82–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. H. (1972). Probabilities, sampling and ethnographic method: the case of Dani colour names. Man, 7, 448–66.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. H. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328–50.Google Scholar
Rosch Heider, E. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch Heider, E., and Olivier, D. C. (1972). The structure of the color space in naming and memory for two languages. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 337–54.Google Scholar
Saunders, B. A. C., and van Brakel, J. (1997). Are there non-trivial constraints on color categorization? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 167–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sauter, D. A., LeGuen, O., and Haun, D. B. M. (2011). Categorical perception of emotional facial expressions does not require lexical categories. Emotion, 11, 1479–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. (1962). The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. Psychometrika, 27, 219–46.Google Scholar
Smallman, H. S., and Boynton, R. M. (1990). Segregation of basic colors in an information display. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 7, 1985–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steels, L., and Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: a case study for color. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 469–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturges, J., and Whitfield, A. (1995). Locating basic colors in the Munsell space. Color Research & Application, 20, 364–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, L. H., Chan, A. H. D., Kay, P., Khong, P.-L., Yip, L. K. C., and Luke, K.-K. (2008). Language affects patterns of brain activation associated with perceptual decision. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 4004–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B., and Kuipers, J. R. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 4567–70.Google ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A., and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Wattenwyl, A., and Zollinger, H. (1979). Color-term salience and neurophysiology of color vision. American Anthropologist, 81, 279–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, M. A. (2011). Adaptation and visual coding. Journal of Vision, 3, 123.Google Scholar
Webster, M. A., and Kay, P. (2012). Color categories and color appearance. Cognition, 122, 375–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, M. A., Webster, S. M., Bharadwaj, S., Verma, R., Jaikumar, J., Madan, G., and Vaithilingham, E. (2002). Variations in normal color vision. III. Unique hues in Indian and United States observers. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 19, 1951–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wierzbicka, A. (2008). Why there are no ‘colour universals’ in language and thought. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 14, 407–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiggett, A. J., and Davies, I. R. L. (2008). The effect of Stroop interference on the categorical perception of color. Memory & Cognition, 36, 231–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., and Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 7780–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Witzel, C., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Is there a lateralised category effect for color? Journal of Vision, 11(12), 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witzel, C., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Categorical sensitivity to color differences. Journal of Vision, 13, 133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodworth, R. S. (1910). The puzzle of color vocabularies. Psychological Bulletin, 7, 325–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiao, Y., Kavanau, C., Bertin, L., and Kaplan, E. (2011). The biological basis of a universal constraint on color naming: cone contrasts and the two-way categorization of colors. PLoS ONE, 6(9), e24994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yasuda, M., Banno,, T., and Komatsu, H. (2010). Color selectivity of neurons in the posterior inferior temporal cortex of the macaque monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1630–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yendrikhovskij, S. N. (2001). A computational model of color categorization. Color Research & Application, 26, 235–8.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yokoi, K., and Uchikawa, K. (2005). Color category influences heterogeneous visual search for color. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 22, 2309–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, K., Mo, L., Kay, P., Kwok, V. P., Ip, T.N., and Tan, L. H. (2010). Newly trained lexical categories produce lateralized categorical perception of color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 107, 9974–8.Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×