Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-rvbq7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T22:29:26.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - It Takes One to Know One Better: Controversy about the Cultural Ingroup Advantage in Communicating Emotion as a Theoretical Rather Than Methodological Issue

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2009

Hillary Anger Elfenbein
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Ursula Hess
Affiliation:
Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
Pierre Philippot
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Get access

Summary

Author's Note

This chapter benefited from the helpful comments of Ursula Hess, Abigail Marsh, and Pierre Philippot, as well as from continuing debate with David Matsumoto. I thank Nalini Ambady for her many contributions over the years. Preparation of this chapter was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Behavioral Science Track Award for Rapid Transition 1R03MH071294-1.

Correspondence can be directed to Hillary Anger Elfenbein at hillary@post.harvard.edu or by post at F-543 Haas School, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720.

As in many areas within psychology, over time the nature-versus-nurture debate about the expression and perception of emotion has gradually given way to a more balanced perspective arguing for the importance of both nature and nurture – neither to the exclusion of the other. This chapter focuses on one recent and controversial area of research on universals and cultural differences in the communication of emotion: the presence of an ingroup advantage, whereby individuals can more easily and accurately understand emotional expressions originating from members of their own cultural group rather than expressions originating from members of a different cultural group.

There has been controversy about how to interpret empirical findings of the ingroup advantage. Although the controversy has often been framed in terms of a methodological debate about the underlying empirical research (Matsumoto, 2002), close examination of the issue reveals its source to be a subtle yet important theoretical divide (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002a, 2003b).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beaupré, M., & Hess, U. (2005). Cross-cultural emotion recognition among Canadian ethnic groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 355–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., Hearn, V., Heider, K., Kudoh, T., & Ton, V. (1997). Matsumoto and Ekman's Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion (JACFEE): Reliability data and cross-national differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971 (Vol. 19, pp. 207–282). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell's mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002a). Is there an ingroup advantage in emotion?Psychological Bulletin, 128, 243–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002b). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003a). Cultural similarity's consequences: A distance perspective on cross-cultural differences in emotion recognition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 92–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003b). Universals and cultural differences in recognizing emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12, 159–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003c). When familiarity breeds accuracy: Cultural exposure and facial emotion recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 276–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M., Leveque, M., & Hess, U. (in press). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion.
Elfenbein, H. A., Foo, M. D., Boldry, J. G., & Tan, H. H. (2005). Dyadic effects in nonverbal communication: A variance partitioning analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 149–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N., Harizuka, S., & Kumar, S. (2002). Cross-cultural patterns in emotion recognition: Highlighting design and analytical techniques. Emotion, 2, 75–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N., Harizuka, S., & Kumar, S. (2004). Hemifacial differences in the ingroup advantage in emotion recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 613–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Marsh, A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the recognition of emotion from the face. In Barrett, L. F. & Salovey, P. (Eds.), The wisdom of feelings: Processes underlying emotional intelligence (pp. 37–59). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hatta, T., & Nachshon, I. (1988). Ear differences in evaluating emotional overtones of unfamiliar speech by Japanese and Israelis. International Journal of Psychology, 23, 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's consequences (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 288–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilbride, J. E., & Yarczower, M. (1983). Ethnic bias in the recognition of facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 27–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markham, R., & Wang, L. (1996). Recognition of emotion by Chinese and Australian children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 616–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (1989). Cultural influences on the perception of emotion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 92–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (2002). Methodological requirements to test a possible ingroup advantage in judging emotions across cultures: Comments on Elfenbein and Ambady and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 236–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Assar, M. (1992). The effects of language on judgments of universal facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16, 85–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Models of emotional intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 396–420). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (2001). Nonverbal receptivity: The diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy (DANVA). In Hall, J. A. & Bernieri, F. J. (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity theory and measurement (pp. 183–198). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U. (1996). Cultural influences on the recognition of emotion by facial expressions: Individualistic or Caucasian cultures?Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sogon, S., & Masutani, M. (1989). Identification of emotion from body movements: A cross-cultural study of Americans and Japanese. Psychological Reports, 65, 35–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, L., & Markham, R. (1999). The development of a series of photographs of Chinese facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 397–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×