Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 2
  • Print publication year: 2011
  • Online publication date: June 2012

6 - The relationship between individual and culture

Summary

The relationship between individual and culture

Cross-cultural psychology has much to be satisfied with. The need to take cultural or ethnic differences into account when studying psychological processes has become widely recognised in mainstream psychology, papers on cross-cultural comparisons are regularly published in psychology books and journals, and courses on intercultural, multicultural, cross-cultural or cultural psychology have become part and parcel of many psychology curricula. With cross-cultural psychology reaching maturity and interest in cross-cultural issues growing, now may be a good time for a reorientation on the relationship between individual and culture (see Chapter 1).

In this chapter I argue that despite all the empirical and methodological advances in our field, our notion of how individuals and culture relate has remained somewhat underdeveloped. Much cross-cultural research seems to take the view of culture as a stable, general factor in the environment that automatically influences the psychological processes of individuals. The individual is seen to deal with culture either through adaptation to culture as context or through internalisation of cultural models and ideas. The view of individuals automatically adapting to or internalising a general cultural context is reinforced by the trend to define cultures in terms of psychological content, by which I mean positions on specific psychological variables such as emotions (shame-cultures and guilt-cultures), notions of the self (interdependent self and independent self) or values (collectivism and individualism).

Related content

Powered by UNSILO
References
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Arends-Tóth, J. V., and Vijver, F. J. R. (2003). Multiculturalism and acculturation: Views of Dutch and Turkish-Dutch. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 249–66.
Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80–114.
Barrett, P. T., Petrides, K. V., Eysenck, S. B. G., and Eysenck, H. J. (1998). The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: An examination of the factorial similarity of P, E, N, and L across 34 countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 805–19.
Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Berry, J. W. (2001). A psychology of immigration. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 615–31.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., and Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (2nd edn). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bond, R., and Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1965) line judgment task. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 111–37.
Breugelmans, S. M., and Cremer, D. (2007). The role of emotions in cross-cultural justice research. In Cremer, D. (ed.), Advances in the psychology of justice and affect (pp. 83–104). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Breugelmans, S. M., and Poortinga, Y. H. (2006). Emotion without a word: Shame and guilt with Rarámuri Indians and rural Javanese. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1111–22.
Brouwers, S. A., Hemert, D. A., Breugelmans, S. M., and Vijver, F. J. R. (2004). A historical analysis of empirical studies published in the Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 251–62.
Campbell, D. T. (1961). The mutual methodological relevance of anthropology and psychology. In Hsu, F. L. (ed.), Psychological anthropology (pp. 333–52). Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
Creighton, M. R. (1990). Revisiting shame and guilt cultures: A forty-year pilgrimage. Ethos, 18, 279–307.
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Diamond, J. M. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: Norton.
Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E., et al. (2009). Individualism–collectivism as descriptive norms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 187–213.
Fontaine, J. R. J., Poortinga, Y. H., Setiadi, B., and Markam, S. S. (2002). Cognitive structure of emotion terms in Indonesia and The Netherlands. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 61–86.
Frijda, N. H., and Jahoda, G. (1966). On the scope and methods of cross-cultural research. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 109–27.
Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialization of shame among young Chinese children. Ethos, 27, 180–209.
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L., and Raver, J. (2006). On the nature and importance of cultural tightness–looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1225–44.
Heine, S. J. (2005). Where is the evidence for pancultural self-enhancement? A reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, & Toguchi. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 531–8.
Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural psychology. New York: Norton.
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?Psychological Review, 106, 766–94.
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., and Greenholtz, J. (2002). What's wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 903–18.
Henrich, N., and Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Gintis, H., Fehr, E., and Camerer, C. (eds.) (2004). Foundations of human sociality: Ethnography and experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hermans, H., and Kempen, H. (1998). Moving cultures: The perilous problems of cultural dichotomies in a globalizing society. American Psychologist, 53, 1111–20.
Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. H. (2001). Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. H., and Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hölldobler, B., and Wilson, E. O. (2008). The superorganism: The beauty, elegance, and strangeness of insect societies. New York: Norton.
Inglehart, R. F. (2000). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and political change in 43 societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Inglehart, R. F. (2008). Changing values among Western publics from 1970 to 2006. West European Politics, 31, 130–46.
Inglehart, R. F., and Welzel, C. (2005). Modernization, cultural change, and democracy: The human development sequence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H. and Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. III, pp. 1–49). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Katz, D., and Allport, F. H. (1931). Students' attitudes. Oxford: Craftsman.
Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kim, H., and Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785–800.
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., and Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in two cultures: A cultural look at New Look. Psychological Science, 14, 201–6.
Kroeber, A., and Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture. New York: Meridian Books.
Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., and Veerdonk, E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365–81.
Masuda, T., and Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically vs. analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922–34.
Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Markus and Kitayama's theory of independent and interdependent self-construals. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 289–310.
Matsumoto, D., and Yoo, S. H. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 234–50.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Del Pilar, G. H., Rolland, J.-P., and Parker, W. D. (1998). Cross-cultural assessment of the five-factor model: The revised NEO personality inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 171–88.
Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 68–74.
Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–60.
Mio, J. S., Hackett, L., and Tumambing, J. (2006). Multicultural psychology: Understanding our diverse communities. Boston, MA: McGraw.
Miyake, K., and Yamazaki, K. (1995). Self-conscious emotions, child rearing, and child psychopathology in Japanese culture. In Tangney, J. P. and Fischer, K. W. (eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 488–504). New York: Guilford Press.
Morris, M. W., and Leung, K. (2000). Justice for all? Progress in research on cultural variation in the psychology of distributive and procedural justice. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 100–132.
Morris, M. W., and Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–71.
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. New York: The Free Press.
Nisbett, R. E., and Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview Press.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., and Chen, S. X. (2009). Connecting and separating mind-sets: Culture as situated cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 217–35.
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., and Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. Hove: Psychology Press.
Poortinga, Y. H. (2003). Coherence of culture and generalizability of data: Two questionable assumptions in cross-cultural psychology. In Berman, J. and Berman, J. (eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspective on the self (vol. 49 of the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation) (pp. 257–305). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards convergence? In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H. and Pandey, J. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. 1, pp. 347–87). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Poortinga, Y. H., and Soudijn, K. A. (2002). Behaviour–culture relationships and ontogenetic development. In Keller, H., Poortinga, Y. H. and Schoelmerich, A. (eds.), Between biology and culture: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 320–40). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Rushton, J. P., and Jensen, A. R. (2005). Thirty years of research on race differences in cognitive ability. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 235–94.
Sam, D. L., and Berry, J. W. (eds.) (2006). Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Scherer, K. R. (1997). Profiles of emotion-antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical predictions across cultures. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 113–50.
Scherer, K. R., and Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–28.
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60–70.
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Vevea, J. L. (2005). Pancultural self-enhancement reloaded: A meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 539–51.
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., and Herskovits, K. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.
Shiraev, E. B., and Levy, D. A. (2007). Cross-cultural psychology: Critical thinking and contemporary applications (3rd edn). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., and Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (2006). Understanding social psychology across cultures. London: Sage.
Sumner, W. G. (1906). Folkways: A study of the sociological importance of usages, manners, customs, mores, and morals. Boston, MA: Ginn.
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin, W. G. and Worchel, S. (eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 94–109). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Toomela, A. (1996). How culture transforms mind: A process of internalization. Culture and Psychology, 2, 285–305.
Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism versus individualism: A reconceptualization of a basic concept in cross-cultural social psychology. In Verma, G. K. and Bagley, C. (eds.), Cross-cultural studies of personality, attitudes and cognition (pp. 60–95). London: Macmillan.
Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51, 407–15.
Triandis, H. C., and Suh, E. M. (2002). Cultural influences on personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 133–60.
Vijver, F. J. R., and Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Vijver, F. J. R., Hemert, D. A., and Poortinga, Y. H. (eds.) (2008). Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vliert, E. (2009). Climate, affluence, and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hemert, D. A., Poortinga, Y. H., and Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). Emotion and culture: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 913–43.
Vandello, J. A., and Cohen, D. (2004). When believing is seeing: Sustaining norms of violence in cultures of honor. In Schaller, M. and Crandall, C. (eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 281–304). New York: Erlbaum.
Verkuyten, M. (2005). The social psychology of ethnic identity. Hove: Psychology Press.
Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Paterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., and Karp, S. (1962). Psychological differentiation. New York: Wiley.
Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., and Schug, J. (2008). Preferences versus strategies as explanations for culture-specific behavior. Psychological Science, 19, 579–84.
Zou, X., Tam, K.-P., Morris, M. W., Lee, S.-L., Lau, I. Y.-M., and Chiu, C.-Y. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 579–97.