Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T14:25:57.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Elephants – Studying Cognition in the African Savannah

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Nereida Bueno-Guerra
Affiliation:
Comillas Pontifical University
Federica Amici
Affiliation:
Universität Leipzig
Get access

Summary

As large brained, slow-maturing, very social animals, elephants offer exciting potential for studies of cognition. In popular literature they are famed for having long memories and being very caring of others, but surprisingly little empirical research into elephant cognitive skill has been conducted. This is due at least in part to the problems inherent to studying such large creatures that cannot be housed in a laboratory. Field studies of elephant cognition are difficult to design, plan and execute, whereas studies of captive elephant can suffer from serious behavioural confounds and pose complicated ethical dilemmas. Here I outline those confounds, so that they may be avoided or at least addressed in future studies of captive elephants, and I attempt to elucidate the process of designing, planning and conducting field-work, so that valid and robust research may be conducted in the future. I argue that ecological validity is key to successfully investigating elephant cognition, and that tests of general or widespread cognitive skills must be adapted to be appropriate for a large-bodied animal with a highly developed auditory sense and an extremely strong, dextrous and still sensitive nose.
Type
Chapter
Information
Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition
A Comparative Guide
, pp. 177 - 198
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

de Silva, S., and Wittemyer, G. (2012). A comparison of social organization in Asian elephants and African savannah elephants. International Journal of Primatology, 33, 11251141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, R. A., Bentley, R. A., and Dunbar, R. I. (2008). Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biology Letters, 4, 748751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. C., and Moss, C. J. (2012). Wild female African elephants (Loxodonta africana) exhibit personality traits of leadership and social integration. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. C., and Moss, C. J. (2014). African elephant play, competence and social complexity. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 144156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Safina, C. (2015). Beyond words: what animals think and feel. New York, NY: Macmillan Press.Google Scholar

References

Archie, E. A., Morrison, T. A., Foley, C. A. H., Moss, C. J., and Alberts, S. C. (2006). Dominance rank relationships among wild female African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Animal Behaviour, 71, 117127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arvidsson, J., Amundin, M., and Laska, M. (2012). Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination test by Asian elephants, Elephas maximus. Physiology and Behavior, 105, 809814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., and Byrne, R. W. (2007). Creative or created: using anecdotes to investigate animal cognition. Methods, 42, 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., Sayialel, K. N., Njiraini, N. W., Moss, C. J., Poole, J. H., and Byrne, R. W. (2007). Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Current Biology, 17, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., Sayialel, K. N., Njiraini, N. W., Poole, J. H., Moss, C. J., and Byrne, R. W. (2008a). African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biology Letters, 4, 3436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., Lee, P. C., Njiraini, N., et al. (2008b). Do elephants show empathy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15, 204225.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A., Handford, R., Lee, P. C., et al. (2010). Why do African elephants (Loxodonta africana) simulate oestrus? An analysis of longitudinal data. PLoS ONE, 5(4), e10052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, S., Bouché, P., Rasmussen, H., Orlando, A., and Douglas-Hamilton, I. (2003). The last Sahelian elephants: ranging behavior, population status and recent history of the desert elephants of Mali. Report by Save the Elephants. Available at http://savetheelephants.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/11/2003Sahelianelephants.pdfGoogle Scholar
Blanc, J. (2008). Loxondonta africana IUCN status report. Retrieved from http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/12392/0Google Scholar
Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 54, 8699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkart, J. M., Schubiger, M. N., and van Schaik, C. P. (2017). The evolution of general intelligence. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 40, e195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., and Bates, L. A. (2007). Sociality, evolution and cognition. Current Biology, 17, 714723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., and Bates, L. A. (2010). Review: primate social cognition: uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron, 65, 815830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., and Bates, L. A. (2011). Elephant cognition: what we know about what elephants know. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 174182). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., and Liska, J. (1993). Tool use by wild and captive elephants. Animal Behaviour, 46, 209219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiyo, P. I., Archie, E. A., Hollister-Smith, J. A., et al. (2011). Association patterns of African elephants in all-male groups: the role of age and genetic relatedness. Animal Behaviour, 81, 10931099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choudhury, A., Lahiri Choudhury, D. K., Desai, A., et al. (2008). Elephas maximus status report. Retrieved from www.iucnredlist.org/details/7140/0.Google Scholar
Clubb, R., and Mason, G. (2002). A review of the welfare of zoo elephants in Europe. Report commissioned by the RSPCA. Available from https://science.rspca.org.uk/sciencegroup/wildlife/reportsandresources/captivityGoogle Scholar
Clubb, R., Rowcliffe, M., Lee, P., Mar, K. U., Moss, C., and Mason, G. J. (2008). Compromised survivorship in zoo elephants. Science, 322, 1649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cozzi, B., Spagnoli, S., and Bruno, L. (2001). An overview of the central nervous system of the elephant through a critical appraisal of the literature published in the XIX and XX centuries. Brain Research Bulletin, 54, 219227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, R., and Plotnik, J. M. (2017). Elephants know when their bodies are obstacles to success in a novel transfer task. Scientific Reports, 7, 46309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Silva, S., and Wittemyer, G. (2012). A comparison of social organization in Asian elephants and African savannah elephants. International Journal of Primatology, 33, 11251141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2016). Are we smart enough to know how smart animals are? New York, NY: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
Douglas-Hamilton, I., Bhalla, S., Wittemyer, G., and Vollrath, F. (2006). Behavioural reactions of elephants towards a dying and deceased matriarch. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 100, 87102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishlock, V., and Breuer, T. (2015). Studying forest elephants. Kerpen: Rettet die Elefanten Afrikas e.V.Google Scholar
Fishlock, V., Caldwell, C., and Lee, P. C. (2016). Elephant resource-use traditions. Animal Cognition, 19, 429433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foerder, P., Galloway, M., Barthel, T., Moore, D. E., and Reiss, D. (2011). Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant. PLoS ONE, 6(8), e23251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, C., PettorelliI, N., and Foley, L. (2008). Severe drought and calf survival in elephants. Biology Letters, 4, 541544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 8687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldenberg, S. Z., Douglas-Hamilton, I., and Wittemyer, G. (2016). Vertical transmission of social roles drives resilience to poaching in elephant networks. Current Biology, 26, 7579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greco, B. J., Brown, T. K., Andrews, J. R. M., Swaisgood, R. R., and Caine, N. G. (2013). Social learning in captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana africana). Animal Cognition, 16, 459469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grubb, P., Groves, C., Dudley, J., and Shoshani, J. (2000). Living African elephants belong to two species: Loxodonta africana (Blumenbach, 1797) and Loxodonta cyclotis (Matschie, 1900). Elephant, 2, 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guldemond, R. A. R., Purdon, A., and VanAarde, R. J. (2017). A systematic review of elephant impact across Africa. PLoS ONE, 12(6), e0178935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakeem, A. Y., Hof, P. R., Sherwood, C. C., Switzer, R. C. III, Rasmussen, L. E. L., and Allman, J. M. (2005). Brain of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana): neuroanatomy from magnetic resonance images. The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology, 287, 11171127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., Hart, L. A., McCoy, M., and Sarath, C. (2001). Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: use and modification of branches for fly switching. Animal Behaviour, 62, 839847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. L., and Hart, L. A. (1994). Fly switching by Asian elephants: tool use to control parasites. Animal Behaviour, 48, 3545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. L., Hart, L. A., and Pinter-Wollman, N. (2008). Large brains and cognition: Where do elephants fit in? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 8698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. (2000). Human-like, population-level specialization in the manufacture of Pandanus tools by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloids. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 267, 403413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahl, M. P., and Armstrong, B. D. (2000). Visual and tactile displays in African elephants, Loxodonta africana: a progress report (1991–1997). Elephant, 2, 1921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langbauer, W. R. (2000). Elephant communication. Zoo Biology, 19, 425445.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. (1987). Allomothering among African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 35, 278291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C., and Moss, C. J. (1995). Statural growth in known-age African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Journal of Zoology, 236, 2941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C., and Moss, C. J. (1999). The social context for learning and behavioural development amond wild African elephants. In Mammalian social learning (pp. 102125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, P. C., and Moss, C. J. (2014). African elephant play, competence and social complexity. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C., Lindsay, W. K., and Moss, C. J. (2011a). Ecological patterns of variability in demographic rates. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 7488). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C., Poole, J. H., Njiraini, N., Sayialel, K. N., and Moss, C. J. (2011b). Male social dynamics: independence and beyond. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 260271). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C., Fishlock, V., Webber, C. E., and Moss, C. J. (2016). The reproductive advantages of a long life: longevity and senescence in wild female African elephants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70, 337345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leggett, K. E. A. (2006). Home range and seasonal movement of elephants in the Kunene Region, northwestern Namibia. African Zoology, 41, 1736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, G. J., and Veasey, J. S. (2010). What do population-level welfare indices suggest about the well-being of zoo elephants? Zoo Biology, 29, 256273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McComb, K., Moss, C., Sayialel, S., and Baker, L. (2000). Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 59, 11031109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McComb, K., Moss, C., Durant, S. M., Baker, L., and Sayialel, S. (2001). Matriarchs act as repositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science, 292, 491494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McComb, K., Baker, L., and Moss, C. (2006). African elephants show high levels of interest in the skulls and ivory of their own species. Biology Letters, 2, 2628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McComb, K., Shannon, G., Durant, S. M., Sayialel, K., Slotow, R., et al. (2011). Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 32703276.Google ScholarPubMed
McComb, K., Shannon, G., Sayialel, K. N., and Moss, C. (2014). Elephants can determine ethnicity, gender, and age from acoustic cues in human voices. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 54335438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, M., Palkopoulou, E., Baleka, S., et al. (2017). Palaeogenomes of Eurasian straight-tusked elephants challenge the current view of elephant evolution. eLife, 6, e25413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. K., Hensman, M. C., Hensman, S., et al. (2015). African elephants (Loxodonta africana) can detect TNT using olfaction: implications for biosensor application. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 171, 177183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mizuno, K., Irie, N., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., and Kutsukake, N. (2016). Asian elephants acquire inaccessible food by blowing. Animal Cognition, 19, 215222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, C. J. (2001). The demography of an African elephant (Loxodonta africana) population in Amboseli, Kenya. Journal of Zoology, 255, 145156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, C. J., and Lee, P. C. (2011a). Female reproductive strategies: individual life histories. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 187204). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, C. J., and Lee, P. C. (2011b). Female social dynamics: fidelity and flexibility. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 205223). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, C. J., and Poole, J. H. (1983). Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In Primate social relationships: an integrated approach (pp. 314325). Oxford: Blackwells.Google Scholar
Nissani, M. (2006). Do Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) apply causal reasoning to tool-use tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen-Smith, N., Kerley, G. I. H., Page, B., Slotow, R., and Van Aarde, R. J. (2006). A scientific perspective on the management of elephants in the Kruger National Park and elsewhere. South African Journal of Science, 102, 389394.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2014). Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) reassure others in distress. PeerJ, 2, e278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plotnik, J. M., Lair, R., Suphachoksahakun, W., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2011). Elephants know when they need a helping trunk in a cooperative task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 51165121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Pokorny, J. J., Keratimanochaya, T., et al. (2013). Visual cues given by humans are not sufficient for Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) to find hidden food. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e61174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Shaw, R. C., Brubaker, D. L., Tiller, L. N., and Clayton, N. S. (2014). Thinking with their trunks: elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude nonrewarding alternatives. Animal Behaviour, 88, 9198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polansky, L., Kilian, W., and Wittemyer, G. (2015). Elucidating the significance of spatial memory on movement decisions by African savannah elephants using state-space models. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20143042.Google ScholarPubMed
Poole, J. H. (1987). Rutting behaviour in African elephants: the phenomenon of musth. Behaviour, 102, 283316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, J. H. (1989a). Announcing intent: the aggressive state of musth in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 37, 140152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, J. H. (1989b). Mate guarding, reproductive success and female choice in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 37, 842849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, J. H., and Granli, P. (2011). Signals, gestures, and behavior of African elephants. In The Amboseli elephants (pp. 125161). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, J. H., and Moss, C. J. (1981). Musth in the African elephant Loxodonta africana. Nature, 292, 830831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S., and Watwood, S. (2005). Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature, 434, 455456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, D. J. (1989). Failure to find self-recognition in Asian elephants (Elephus maximus) in contrast to their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 122131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rensch, B. (1957). The intelligence of elephants. Scientific American, 196, 4449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roca, A. L., Georgiadis, N., Pecon-Slattery, J., and Brien, S. J. O. (2001). Genetic evidence for two species of elephant in Africa. Science, 293, 14731478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C., Call, J., and Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5, 293296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shettleworth, S. J. (1993). Where is the comparison in comparative cognition? Alternative research programs. Psychological Science, 4, 179184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoshani, J., Kupsky, W. J., and Marchant, G. H. (2006). Elephant brain. Part I: gross morphology, functions, comparative anatomy, and evolution. Brain Research Bulletin, 70, 124157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smet, A. F., and Byrne, R. W. (2013). African elephants can use human pointing cues to find hidden food. Current Biology, 23, 20332037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smet, A. F., and Byrne, R. W. (2014a). African elephants (Loxodonta africana) recognize visual attention from face and body orientation. Biology Letters, 10, 20140428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smet, A. F., and Byrne, R. W. (2014b). Interpretation of human pointing by African elephants: generalisation and rationality. Animal Cognition, 17, 13651374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinage, C. (1994). Elephants. London: T and AD Poyser Ltd.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. R. (2010). The challenges of understanding animal minds. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoeger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., et al. (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22, 21442148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sukumar, R. (2006). A brief review of the status, distribution and biology of wild Asian elephants. International Zoo Yearbook, 40, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thuppil, V., and Coss, R. G. (2013). Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard growls according to perceived danger. Biology Letters, 9, 20130518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Aarde, R. J., Jackson, T. P., and Ferreira, S. M. (2006). Conservation science and elephant management in southern Africa. South African Journal of Science, 102, 385388.Google Scholar
Wittemyer, G., Douglas-Hamilton, I., and Getz, W. M. (2005). The socioecology of elephants: analysis of the processes creating multitiered social structures. Animal Behaviour, 69, 13571371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittemyer, G., Okello, J. B. A., Rasmussen, H. B., et al. (2009). Where sociality and relatedness diverge: the genetic basis for hierarchical social organization in African elephants. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 35133521.Google ScholarPubMed
Yokoyama, S., Takenaka, N., Agnew, D. W., and Shoshani, J. (2005). Elephants and human color-blind deuteranopes have identical sets of visual pigments. Genetics, 170, 335344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×