Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T22:52:13.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Evidence for adaptations for female extra-pair mating in humans: thoughts on current status and future directions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2010

Steven W. Gangestad
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Steven M. Platek
Affiliation:
Drexel University, Philadelphia
Todd K. Shackelford
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In the late 1980s and early 1990s, behavioral ecologists were first able to estimate, with good reliability, the paternity of broods in socially monogamous birds. Within a very few years, rates of paternity by social fathers had been estimated within a large number of populations. An amazing pattern emerged from these studies, one that changed fundamentally the way that behavioral ecologists viewed mating in birds. The extra-pair paternity rate – the proportion of offspring not sired by social fathers – was, on average, 10–15% (e.g. Birkhead & Møller, 1995; Petrie & Kempenaers, 1998). Though occasionally in the 1–3% range, estimated extra-pair paternity rates of at least 20% were not uncommon, and they reached 50% in a few populations. A substantial proportion of socially monogamous bird species are clearly not sexually monogamous.

From the perspective of some observers, these surprising empirical findings were the leading edge of a revolutionary “paradigm shift” in behavioral ecology currently taking place, with “the traditional concepts of the choosy, monogamous female and the coadapted gene complex increasingly giving way to the realization that sexual reproduction engenders conflicts [and] promotes polyandry …” (Zeh & Zeh, 2001). Though Trivers' (1972) parental investment theory did not explicitly claim that females in socially monogamous species should be sexually monogamous, it did emphasize the potential reproductive benefits of male multiple mating and not the reasons why females (the sex typically investing more heavily in offspring) might multiply mate. Its arguments were based on reasoning about limitations on offspring number.

Type
Chapter
Information
Female Infidelity and Paternal Uncertainty
Evolutionary Perspectives on Male Anti-Cuckoldry Tactics
, pp. 37 - 57
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D. B., Gold, A. R., and Burt, B. A. (1978). Rise in female-initiated sexual activity at ovulation and its suppression by oral contraceptives. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 1145–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, R. D. and Noonan, K. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care, and human social evolution. In Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, W., eds., Evolutionary Biology and Human Behavior: an Anthropological Perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury, pp. 402–35.Google Scholar
Bancroft, J., Sanders, D., Davidson, D., and Warner, P. (1983). Mood-sexuality, hormones, and the menstrual cycle. III. Sexuality and the role of androgens. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 509–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellis, M. A. and Baker, R. R. (1990). Do females promote sperm competition? Data for humans. Animal Behaviour, 40, 997–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkhead, T. R. and Møller, A. P. (1995). Extrapair copulation and extra-pair paternity in birds. Animal Behaviour, 49, 843–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buller, D. J. (2005). Adapting Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Burt, A. (1995). Perspective: The evolution of fitness. Evolution, 49, 1–8.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2000). Dangerous Passions. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Charlesworth, B. (1990). Mutation-selection balance and the evolutionary advantage of sex and recombination. Genetical Research, 55, 199–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, B. and Hughes, K. A. (1998). The maintenance of genetic variation in life history traits. In Singh, R. S. and Krimbas, C. B., eds., Evolutionary Genetics from Molecules to Morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eberhard, W. G. (2004). Rapid divergent evolution of sexual morphology: Comparative tests of antagonistic coevolution and traditional female choice. Evolution, 58, 1947–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, R. D. and Kutteh, W. H. (1999). Characterization of immunoglobulins and cytokines in human cervical mucus: influence of exogenous and endogenous hormones. Journal of Reproductive Immunology, 42, 93–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G., Burch, R. L., and Platek, S. M. (2002). Does semen have antidepressant properties? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 289–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G., Burch, R. L., Zappieri, M. L., Parvez, R. A., Stockwell, M. L., and Davis, J. A. (2003). The human penis as a semen displacement device. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 277–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W. and Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women's preference for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 262, 727–33.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., and Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in women's sexual interests and their partners' mate retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 975–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., and Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women's preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., and Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005a). Adaptations to ovulation. In Buss, D. M., ed., Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., and Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005b). Female sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle depend on primary partner developmental instability. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B (in press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, A. T., Shackelford, T. K., Weekes-Shackelford, A., et al. (2005). Mate retention, semen displacement, and human sperm competition: a preliminary investigation of tactics to prevent and correct female infidelity. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 749–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grammer, K. (1993). 5-α-Androst-16en-3α-on: a male pheromone? A brief report. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 201–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gravitt, P. E., Hildesheim, A., Herrero, R., et al. (2003). Correlates of IL-10 and IL-12 concentrations in cervical secretions. Journal of Clinical Immunology, 23, 175–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greiling, H. and Buss, D. M. (2000). Women's sexual strategies: the hidden dimension of short-term extra-pair mating. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 929–63.
Haig, D. (1993). Genetic conflicts in human pregnancy. Quarterly Review of Biology, 68, 495–532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, W. D. and Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites. Science, 218, 384–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselton, M. G. and Miller, G. F. (2005). Evidence for ovulatory shifts in attraction to artistic and entrepreneurial excellence. Human Nature (in press).Google Scholar
Hedrick, P. W. and Black, F. L. (1997). HLA and mate selection: no evidence in South Amerindians. American Journal of Human Genetics, 61, 505–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendricks, C., Piccinino, L. J., Udry, J. R., Chimbira, T. H. K. (1987). Peak coital rate coincides with onset of lutenizing hormone surge. Fertility and Sterility, 48, 234–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, B. and Rice, W. R. (1999). Experimental removal of sexual selection reverses intersexual antagonistic coevolution and removes a reproductive load. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96, 5083–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosken, D. J. and Stockley, P. (2004). Sexual selection and genital evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19, 87–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houle, D. (1992). Comparing evolvability and variability of traits. Genetics, 130, 195–204.Google ScholarPubMed
Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1, 13–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hummel, T., Gollisch, R., Wildt, G., and Kobal, G. (1991). Changes in olfactory perception during the menstrual cycle. Experientia, 47, 712–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ihara, Y., Aoki, K., Tokumaga, K., Takahashi, K., and Juji, T. (2000). HLA and human mate choice: tests on Japanese couples. Anthropological Science, 108, 199–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennions, M. D. and Petrie, M. (2000). Why do females mate multiply?: A review of the genetic benefits. Biological Reviews, 75, 21–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., and Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 251–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. and Schaik, C. P. (eds.). (2003). Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kokko, H., Brooks, R., Jennions, M. D., and Morley, J. (2003). The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, 653–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matteo, S. and Rissman, E. F. (1984). Increased sexual activity during the midcycle portion of the human menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 18, 249–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, D. J. and Potts, W. K. (1999). The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility complex genes. American Naturalist, 153, 145–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D., Burt, M., Koyabashi, T., and Murray, L. K. (1999). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically. Nature, 399, 741–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persky, H., Charney, N., Leif, H. I., O'Brien, C. P., Miller, W. R., and Strauss, D. (1978). The relationship of plasma estradiol to sexual behavior in young women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 40, 523–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrie, M. and Kempenaers, B. (1998). Extra-pair paternity in birds: Explaining variation between species and populations. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13, 52–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillsworth, E. G., Haselton, M. G., and Buss, D. M. (2004). Ovulatory shifts in female sexual desire. Journal of Sex Research (in press).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Burch, R. L., Frederick, D. A., Myers, T. E., and Gallup, G. G. (2003). How much paternal resemblance is enough? Sex differences in hypothetical investment decisions but not in the detection of resemblance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 81–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platek, S. M., Raines, D. M., Gallup, G. G.et al. (2004). Reactions to children's faces: Males are more affected by resemblance than females are, and so are their brains. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 394–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomiankowski, A. and Møller, A. P. (1995). A resolution of the lek paradox. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 260, 21–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putz, D. (2005). Menstrual phase and mating context affect women's preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior (in press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regan, P. C. (1996). Rhythms of desire: the association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5, 145–56.Google Scholar
Rice, W. R. (1988). Heritable variation as a prerequisite for adaptive female choice: the effect of mutation-selection balance. Evolution, 42, 817–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, W. R. (1996). Sexually antagonistic male adaptation triggered by experimental arrest of female evolution. Nature, 381, 232–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, W. R. and Holland, B. (1997). The enemies within: intragenomic conflict, interlocus contest evolution (ICE), and the intraspecific Red Queen. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rikowski, A. and Grammer, K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 266, 869–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, S. A., Bromfield, J. J., and Tremellen, K. P. (2003). Seminal ‘priming’ for protection from preeclampsia: a unifying hypothesis. Journal of Reproductive Immunology, 59, 253–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreinerss-Engel, P., Schiavi, R. C., Smith, H., and White, D. (1981). Sexual arousability and the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43(3), 199–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shackelford, T. K., LeBlanc, G. J., Weekes-Shackelford, V. A., Bleske-Rechek, A. L., Euler, H. A., and Hoier, S. (2002). Psychological adaptation to human sperm competition. Evolution and Human Behavior, 12, 123–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornhill, R. and Gangestad, S. W. (1999). The scent of symmetry: a human pheromone that signals fitness? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 175–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCollough, J., and Franklin, M. (2003). MHC, symmetry and body scent attractiveness in men and women (Homo sapiens). Behavioral Ecology (in press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B., ed., Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971. Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
Wedekind, C. and Füri, S. (1997). Body odor preference in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 264, 1471–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wedekind, C., Seebeck, T., Bettens, F., and Paepke, A. J. (1995) MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 260, 245–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, A. J., Baird, D. D., Dunson, D. B., McConnaughey, D. R., Kesner, J. S., and Weinberg, C. R. (2004). On the frequency of intercourse around ovulation: evidence for biological influences. Human Reproduction, 19, 1539–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, G. C. (1992). Natural Selection: Domains, Levels, and Challenges. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zeh, J. A. and Zeh, D. W. (2001) Spontaneous abortion depresses female sexual receptivity in a viviparous arthropod. Animal Behaviour, 62, 427–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×