Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T17:32:28.680Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Experiments in Guardianship Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2014

Maud van Bavel
Affiliation:
VU University Amsterdam
Henk Elffers
Affiliation:
VU University Amsterdam
Brandon C. Welsh
Affiliation:
Northeastern University
Anthony A. Braga
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Gerben J. N. Bruinsma
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

For testing causal models, it is well agreed that experimental research is the gold standard. For example, Farrington (1979) calls experimentation the most conclusive methodology for testing hypotheses. Despite its key role in the prevention of and protection against crimes and renewed interest in the concept, experimental tests of guardianship remain scarce. In their article on guardianship for crime prevention, Hollis-Peel et al. (2011) found that most of the research on guardianship makes use of macrostructural and survey data, relying on cross-sectional and nonobservational approaches. The authors point out that there is a distinct lack of, and subsequently a specific need for, quasi-experimental and experimental designs in guardianship research.

Following the call for more experimental research on guardianship by Hollis-Peel et al. (2011), the aim of this chapter is to explore in more detail if experiments would be useful in guardianship research. We look into experimental research in the realm of guardianship and review the literature. In the course of this chapter we propose to refine the concept of guardianship, distinguishing guardianship before a crime takes place (“preventive guardianship”) from guardianship during or after a crime (“repressive guardianship”). The distinction between phases of guardianship made in this way has, of course, consequences for the way experimental research into guardianship can and should be designed. We observe that experimental work into repressive guardianship, of which a number of examples in the 1970s and 1980s can be found, raises concerns about its validity, whereas experimental work on preventive guardianship is altogether absent.

Type
Chapter
Information
Experimental Criminology
Prospects for Advancing Science and Public Policy
, pp. 90 - 108
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bernasco, W., and Nieuwbeerta, P.. 2005. “How Do Residential Burglars Select Target Areas? A New Approach to the Analysis of Criminal Location Choice.” British Journal of Criminology 45: 296–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braga, A. A. 2006. “Policing Crime Hot Spots.” In Preventing Crime: What Works for Children, Offenders, Victims and Places, edited byWelsh, B. C. and Farrington, D. P., pp. 179–92. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. E., and Felson, M.. 1979. “Social Change and Crime Rate Trends: A Routine Activity Approach.”American Sociological Review, 44(4): 588–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromwell, P., Dunham, R., Akers, R., and Lanza-Kaduce, L.. 1995. “Routine Activities and Social Control in the Aftermath of a Natural Catastrophe. European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research 3(3): 56–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dertke, M. C., Penner, L. A., and Ulrich, K.. 1974. “Observer’s Reporting of Shoplifting as a Function of Thief’s Race and Sex. Journal of Social Psychology 94: 213–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elffers, H., and Visscher, M.. 2002. Bijeffecten van grootschalig politieoptreden. Zien boeven hun kans schoon als de politie de handen elders vol heeft? [Secondary effects of large scale police operations. Do offenders profit when the police are focusing their attention elsewhere?]. NSCR-rapport 2002–1. Leiden, the Netherlands: NSCR.
Farrington, D. P. 1979. “Experiments on Deviance with Special Reference to Dishonesty.” In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, edited byBerkowitz, L., pp. 207–52. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Farrington, D. P., and Knight, B. J.. 1980. “Stealing from a ‘Lost’ Letter: Effects of Victim Characteristics.” Criminal Justice and Behavior, 7(4): 423–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrington, D. P., and Welsh, B. C.. 2006. “Improved Street Lighting.” In Preventing Crime: What Works for Children, Offenders, Victims and Places, edited by Welsh, B.C and Farrington, D. P., pp. 209–26. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
Felson, M. 1995. “Those Who Discourage Crime.” In Crime and Place: Crime Prevention Studies, Vol. 4, edited by Eck, J. E. and Weisburd, D., pp. 53–66. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., Heene, M., Wicher, M. and Kainbacher, M.. 2011. “The Bystander-Effect: A Meta-Analytic Review on Bystander Intervention in Dangerous and Non-dangerous Emergencies.”Psychological Bulletin 137(4): 517–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garofalo, J., and Clark, D.. 1992. “Guardianship and Residential Burglary.” Justice Quarterly, 9(3): 443–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, D., Hartmann, D., Walder, P., and Page, B.. 1973. “Who Reports Shoplifters? A Field Experimental Study.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 25(2): 276–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollis-Peel, M. E., Reynald, D., Van Bavel, M., Elffers, H., and Welsh, B. C.. 2011. “Guardianship for Crime Prevention: A Critical Review of the Literature.” Crime, Law and Social Change, 56: 53–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latané, B., and Darley, J.. 1970. The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn’t He Help?New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Miethe, T., Hughes, M., and McDowall, D.. 1991. “Social Change and Crime Rates: An Evaluation of Alternative Perspectives.”Social Forces 70: 165–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miethe, T. D., and Meier, R. F.. 1994. Crime and Its Social Context: Toward an Integrated Theory of Offenders, Victims and Situations. Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. 1977. The Individual in a Social World. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Moriarty, T. 1975. “Crime, Commitment and the Responsive Bystander: Two Field Experiments.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31(2): 270–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynald, D. M. 2009. “Guardianship in Action: Developing a New Tool for Measurement.” Crime Prevention and Community Safety, 11(1): 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynald, D. M. 2011. Guarding Against Crime: Measuring Guardianship within Routine Activity Theory. Farnham, UK: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Roëll, G., Van Dijk, J. M., and Steinmetz, C. H. D.. 1982. “Interventiegedrag door omstanders: een veldexperiment.” Tijdschrift voor Criminologie, 1: 21–35.Google Scholar
Schwarz, L., Jennings, K., Petrillo, J., and Kidd, R.. 1980. “Role of Commitments in the Decision to Stop a Theft.” Journal of Social Psychology, 110: 183–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S., and Gottlieb, A.. 1976. “Bystanders Reactions to a Violent Theft: Crime in Jerusalem.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34(6): 1188–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takooshian, H., and Bodinger, H.. 1982. “Bystander Indifference to Street Crime.” In Contemporary Criminology, edited bySavitz, L. and Johnston, N., pp. 209–16. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Tewksbury, R., and Mustaine, E. E.. 2003. “College Students’ Lifestyles and Self-protective Behaviors: Further Considerations of the Guardianship Concept in Routine Activity Theory.” Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30(3): 302–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bommel, M., Van Prooijen, J. W., Elffers, H., and Van Lange, P. A. M.. 2012. “Be Aware to Care: Public Self-awareness Leads to a Reversal of the Bystander Effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48(4): 926–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webley, P., Robben, H. S. J., Elffers, H., and Hessing, D. J.. 1991. Tax Evasion: An Experimental Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Welsh, B. C., and Farrington, D. P.. 2006. “Closed-Circuit Television Surveillance.” In Preventing Crime: What Works for Children, Offenders, Victims, and Places, edited by Welsh, B. C. and Farrington, D. P., pp. 193–208. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winkel, F. W. 1981. “Sociopreventie. De rol van omstanders bij de totstandkoming van een delict [Socio-prevention: On the Role of Bystanders in How Crime Occurs].” Tijdschrift voor Criminologie 1: 53–71.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×