Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:15:47.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - The gray mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus): a novel cognitive primate brain aging model

from Part IV - Cheirogaleidae: sensory ecology, communication, and cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2016

Jean-Luc Picq
Affiliation:
Université Paris 8, France
Shawn M. Lehman
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Ute Radespiel
Affiliation:
University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation
Elke Zimmermann
Affiliation:
University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The use of the small cheirogaleid, the gray mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus, which will be referred to as “mouse lemur” throughout this chapter), for laboratory studies of the brain and cognitive aging began in the early 1990s (Bons et al., 1991, 1994; Picq, 1992, 1993; Mestre and Bons, 1993). In the decades that followed, several research groups have become interested in M. murinus and carried out routine assessment of cognitive capacities on various behavioral tasks (see present review). Therefore, the mouse lemur represents an exception among the prosimians, which were largely ignored for a long time by researchers studying cognition. Indeed, when consulting a textbook, such as Primate Cognition (Tomasello and Call, 1997), the following type of remark is commonly encountered: “as usual, we know little about cognitive skills of prosimians …”. In fact, there was a longstanding lack of interest regarding the investigation of cognitive functions in prosimians, which resulted from two major ideas. First, prosimians were generally thought to be untrainable, as they were considered to be nervous, easily distracted, and non-cooperative (Wilkerson and Rumbaugh, 1979). Early attempts to work with these animals were very disappointing. For example, it was reported that “when removed from the home cage, lemurs grew hysterical, lorisoids sulked, and both refused to work” (Jolly, 1964b). Second, prosimians were believed to be less intelligent when compared to other primates. It was claimed that “Old and New World monkeys' intelligence outdistanced that of other mammals, including the prosimian primates” (Jolly, 1966). Two major explanations were proposed to explain the modest cognitive capacity of prosimians. First of all, it was suggested that they have found an ecological niche in which intelligence is not required (i.e., a niche with reduced opportunities for competition between species) (Andrew, 1962; Jolly, 1966). Second, they have been described as primitive, representing “surviving approximations of those ancestral primate forms from whose core components the higher primates evolved” (Wilkerson and Rumbaugh, 1979). Indeed, according to the view of Le Gros Clark (1959), the primate order is composed of a series of forms that are successive approximations of the evolutionary antecedents of man (characterized by high cognitive abilities), with prosimians at the bottom of the scale.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Dwarf and Mouse Lemurs of Madagascar
Biology, Behavior and Conservation Biogeography of the Cheirogaleidae
, pp. 381 - 404
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrew, RJ. 1962. Evolution of intelligence and vocal mimicking: studies of large-brained mammals promise to elucidate some problems of human evolution. Science 137(3530):585–589.Google Scholar
Aujard, F, Perret, M, Vannier, G. 1998. Thermoregulatory responses to variations of photoperiod and ambient temperature in the male lesser mouse lemur: a primitive or an advanced adaptive character?Journal of Comparative Physiology, B 168(7):540–548.Google Scholar
Austad, SN. 1997. Small nonhuman primates as potential models of human aging. ILAR Journal 38(3):142–147.Google Scholar
Austad, SN, Fischer, KE. 2011. The development of small primate models for aging research. ILAR Journal 52(1):78–88.Google Scholar
Bachevalier, J, Landis, LS, Walker, LC, et al. 1991. Aged monkeys exhibit behavioral deficits indicative of widespread cerebral dysfunction. Neurobiology of Aging 12(2):99–111.Google Scholar
Barnes, CA. 1979. Memory deficits associated with senescence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 93(1):74–104.Google Scholar
Bartus, RT, Dean, RL 3rd, Fleming, DL. 1979. Aging in the rhesus monkey: effects on visual discrimination learning and reversal learning. Journal of Gerontology 34(2):209–219.Google Scholar
Bayne, K, Novak, M. 1998. Behavioral disorders. In Bennett, TB, Abee, CR, Henrickson, R (eds.), Nonhuman Primates in Biomedical Research (pp. 485–500). Academic Press, San Diego.
Bons, N, Mestre, N, Petter, A. 1991. Senile plaques and neurofibrillary changes in the brain of an aged lemurian primate, Microcebus murinus. Neurobiology of Aging 13(1):99–105.Google Scholar
Bons, N, Mestre, N, Ritchie, K, et al. 1994. Identification of amyloid beta protein in the brain of the small, short-lived lemurian primate Microcebus murinus. Neurobiology of Aging 15(2):215–220.Google Scholar
Butler, AB, Hodos, W. 1996. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. Wiley-Liss, New York.
Cohen, NJ, Squire, LR. 1980. Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210(4466):207–210.Google Scholar
Cork, LC, Powers, RE, Selkoe, DJ, et al. 1988. Neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques in aged bears. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 47(6):629–641.Google Scholar
Craik, FIM. 1977. Age differences in human memory. In Birren, JE, Schaie, KW (eds.), Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. Van Nostrand-Reinhold, New York.
Dal-Pan, A, Pifferi, F, Marchal, J, Picq, JL, Aujard, F. 2011. Cognitive performances are selectively enhanced during chronic caloric restriction or resveratrol supplementation in a primate. PLoS ONE 6(1).Google Scholar
Davis, HP, Cohen, A, Gandy, M, et al. 1990. Lexical priming deficits as a function of age. Behavioral Neuroscience 104(2):288–297.Google Scholar
Delacourte, A, Sautiere, PE, Wattez, A, et al. 1995. Biochemical characterization of Tau proteins during cerebral aging of the lemurian primate Microcebus murinus. Comptes Rendus de l'Académie des Sciences Série III, Sciences de la Vie 318(1):85–89.Google Scholar
Delatour, B, Guegan, M, Volk, A, Dhenain, M. 2006. In vivo MRI and histological evaluation of brain atrophy in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. Neurobiology of Aging 27(6):835–847.Google Scholar
Dempster, FN. 1992. The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review 12(1):45–75.Google Scholar
Deppe, AM, Wright, PC, Szelistowski, WA. 2009. Object permanence in lemurs. Animal Cognition 12(2):381–388.Google Scholar
Deweer, B, Lehericy, S, Pillon, B, et al. 1995. Memory disorders in probable Alzheimer's disease: the role of hippocampal atrophy as shown with MRI. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 58(5):590–597.Google Scholar
Dhenain, M, Duyckaerts, C, Michot, JL, et al. 1998a. Cerebral T2-weighted signal decrease during aging in the mouse lemur primate reflects iron accumulation. Neurobiology of Aging 19(1):65–69.Google Scholar
Dhenain, M, Michot, J-L, Volk, A, et al. 1998b. Etude préliminaire du vieillissement cognitif des microcèbes murins dans une version spatiale du test de non appariement retardé (Preliminary study of cerebral aging in mouse lemurs by a spatial version of the delayed non-matching to sample task). Primatologie 1:309–331.Google Scholar
Dhenain, M, Michot, JL, Privat, N, et al. 2000. MRI description of cerebral atrophy in mouse lemur primates. Neurobiology of Aging 21(1):81–88.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H. 1999. The hippocampus and mechanisms of declarative memory. Behavioural Brain Research 103(2):123–133.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H, Otto, T, Cohen, NJ. 1992. The hippocampus – what does it do?Behavioral and Neural Biology 57(1):2–36.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H, Otto, T, Cohen, NJ. 1994. Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17(3):449–472.Google Scholar
Ennaceur, A, Delacour, J. 1988. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behavioural Brain Research 31(1):47–59.Google Scholar
Fichtel, C, Kappeler, PM. 2010. Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies: etablishing the lemur baseline. In Kappeler, PM, Silk, JB (eds.), Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals (pp. 395–426). Springer, Heidelberg.
Furuya, Y, Yamamoto, T, Yatsugi, S, Ueki, S. 1988. A new method for studying working memory by using the three-panel runway apparatus in rats. Japanese Journal of Pharmacology 46(2):183–188.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, JD. 1996. Memory systems analyses of mnemonic disorders in aging and age-related diseases. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(24):13534–13540.Google Scholar
Gazzaley, A, Cooney, JW, Rissman, J, D'Esposito, M. 2005. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nature Neuroscience 8(10):1298–1300.Google Scholar
Hohenbrink, P, Dempewolf, S, Zimmermann, E, Mundy, NI, Radespiel, U. 2014. Functional promiscuity in a mammalian chemosensory system: extensive expression of vomeronasal receptors in the main olfactory epithelium of mouse lemurs. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy 8:102.Google Scholar
Huemer, HP, Larcher, C, Czedik-Eysenberg, T, Nowotny, N, Reifinger, M. 2002. Fatal infection of a pet monkey with Human herpesvirus 1. Emerging Infectious Diseases 8(6):639–642.Google Scholar
Jallageas, V, Privat, N, Mestre-Francés, N, Bons, N. 1998. Age-related changes in serotonergic and catecholaminergic brain systems in the lemurian primate Microcebus murinus. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 839:628–630.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. 1964a. Choice of cue in prosimian learning. Animal Behaviour 12:571–577.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. 1964b. Prosimians' manipulation of simple object problems. Animal Behaviour 12:560–570.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. 1966. Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 153(735):501–506.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Zimmermann, E. 2007. First evidence for relocation of stationary food resources during foraging in a strepsirrhine primate (Microcebus murinus). American Journal of Primatology 69(9):1045–1052.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Zimmermann, E. 2011. Do solitary foraging nocturnal mammals plan their routes?Biology Letters 7(4):638–640.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Michel, B, Deputte, B, Verdier, JM. 2004. Odor discrimination assessment with an automated olfactometric method in a prosimian primate, Microcebus murinus. Physiology & Behavior 82(2–3):325–329.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Deputte, B, Verdier, JM. 2006. Age effect on olfactory discrimination in a non-human primate, Microcebus murinus. Neurobiology of Aging 27(7):1045–1049.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Scheumann, M, Zimmermann, E. 2008. Wild mouse lemurs revisit artificial feeding platforms: implications for field experiments on sensory and cognitive abilities in small primates. American Journal of Primatology 70(9):892–896.Google Scholar
Joly, M, Ammersdorfer, S, Schmidtke, D, Zimmermann, E. 2014. Touchscreen-based cognitive tasks reveal age-related impairment in a primate aging model, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). PLoS ONE 9(10):e109393.Google Scholar
Kappeler, PM. 1998. Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories. American Journal of Primatology 46(1):7–33.Google Scholar
Kappeler, PM. 2000. Lemur origins: rafting by groups of hibernators?Folia Primatologica 71(6):422–425.Google Scholar
Kraska, A, Dorieux, O, Picq, JL, et al. 2011. Age-associated cerebral atrophy in mouse lemur primates. Neurobiology of Aging 32(5):894–906.Google Scholar
Languille, S, Blanc, S, Blin, O, et al. 2012. The grey mouse lemur: a non-human primate model for ageing studies. Ageing Research Reviews 11(1):150–162.Google Scholar
Languille, S, Liévin-Bazin, A, Picq, JL, et al. 2015. Deficits of psychomotor and mnesic functions across aging in mouse lemur primates. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8:446.Google Scholar
Clark, WE Le Gros. 1959. The Antecedent of Man. Edinburgh University Press. London.
Lefaux, B, Duprez, R, Tanguy, M, et al. 2004. Nonhuman primates might be highly susceptible to cross-species infectivity by human alpha-herpesviruses. Veterinary Pathology 41(3):302–304.Google Scholar
Lewis, KP, Jaffe, S, Brannon, EM. 2005. Analog number representations in mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz): evidence from a search task. Animal Cognition 8(4):247–252.Google Scholar
Logan, GD. 1985. Executive control of thought and action. Acta Psychologica 60:193–210.Google Scholar
Luhrs, ML, Dammhahn, M, Kappeler, PM, Fichtel, C. 2009. Spatial memory in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Animal Cognition 12(4):599–609.Google Scholar
MacLean, EL, Merritt, DJ, Brannon, EM. 2008. Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Animal Behaviour 76(2):479–486.Google Scholar
Maille, A, Roeder, JJ. 2012. Inferences about the location of food in lemurs (Eulemur macaco and Eulemur fulvus): a comparison with apes and monkeys. Animal Cognition 15(6):1075–1083.Google Scholar
Malkova, L, Mishkin, M, Bachevalier, J. 1995. Long-term effects of selective neonatal temporal lobe lesions on learning and memory in monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience 109(2):212–226.Google Scholar
Martin, RD. 1972. Adaptive radiation and behaviour of the Malagasy lemurs. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 264(862):295–352.Google Scholar
Masters, JC, Wit, MJ de, Lovegrove, BG. 2007. Eyes wide shut: can hypometabolism really explain the primate colonization of Madagascar?Journal of Biogeography 34(1):21–37.Google Scholar
McLay, RN, Freeman, SM, Harlan, RE, Kastin, AJ, Zadina, JE. 1999. Tests used to assess the cognitive abilities of aged rats: their relation to each other and to hippocampal morphology and neurotrophin expression. Gerontology 45(3):143–155.Google Scholar
Mestre, N, Bons, N. 1993. Age-related cytological changes and neuronal loss in basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in Microcebus murinus (lemurian, primate). NeuroDegeneration 2:25–32.Google Scholar
Mestre-Francés, N, Silhol, S, Bons, N. 1996. Evolution of β-amyloid deposits in the cerebral cortex of Microcebus murinus lemurian primate. Alzheimer Research 2:19–28.Google Scholar
Mishkin, M, Delacour, J. 1975. An analysis of short-term visual memory in the monkey. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1(4):326–334.Google Scholar
Montgomery, SH, Mundy, NI. 2013. Parallel episodes of phyletic dwarfism in callitrichid and cheirogaleid primates. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 26(4):810–819.Google Scholar
Norman, DA, Shallice, T. 1986. Attention in action: willed and automatic control of behavior. In Davidson, RJ, Schwartz, GE, Shapiro, D (eds.), Consciouness and Self-Regulation: Advance in Research and Theory (pp. 1–18). Plenum, New York.
Pariente, GF. 1979. The role of vision in prosimian behavior. In Doyle, GA, Martin, RD (eds.), The Study of Prosimian Behavior (pp. 411–459). Academic Press, New York.
Perret, M. 1982. Stress-effects in Microcebus murinus. Folia Primatologica 39(1–2):63–114.Google Scholar
Perret, M. 2005. Relationship between urinary estrogen levels before conception and sex ratio at birth in a primate, the gray mouse lemur. Human Reproduction 20(6):1504–1510.Google Scholar
Picq, JL. 1992. Aging and social behaviour in captivity in Microcebus murinus. Folia Primatologica 59(4):217–220.Google Scholar
Picq, JL. 1993. Radial maze performance in young and aged grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Primates 24:223–226.Google Scholar
Picq, JL. 1995. Effects of aging upon recent memory in Microcebus murinus. Aging 7(1):17–22.Google Scholar
Picq, JL. 2007. Aging affects executive functions and memory in mouse lemur primates. Experimental Gerontology 42(3):223–232.Google Scholar
Picq, JL, Dhenain, M. 1998. Reaction to new objects in young and aged grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Journal of Experimental Psychology 51:337–348.Google Scholar
Picq, JL, Aujard, F, Volk, A, Dhenain, M. 2012. Age-related cerebral atrophy in nonhuman primates predicts cognitive impairments. Neurobiology of Aging 33(6):1096–1109.Google Scholar
Piep, M, Radespiel, U, Zimmerman, E, Schmidt, S, Siemers, BM. 2008. The sensory basis of prey detection in captive-born grey mouse lemurs. Animal Behaviour 75(3):871–878.Google Scholar
Rapp, PR, Amaral, DG. 1992. Individual differences in the cognitive and neurobiological consequences of normal aging. Trends in Neurosciences 15(9):340–345.Google Scholar
Santos, LR, Mahajan, N, Barnes, JL. 2005. How prosimian primates represent tools: experiments with two lemur species (Eulemur fulvus and Lemur catta). Journal of Comparative Psychology 119(4):394–403.Google Scholar
Scheumann, M, Rabesandratana, A, Zimmerman, E. 2007. Predation, communication, and cognition in lemurs. In Gursky-Doyen, SL, Nekaris, KAI (eds.), Primate Anti-Predator Strategies (pp. 100–126). Springer, New York.
Scheumann, M, Joly-Radko, M, Leliveld, L, Zimmermann, E. 2011. Does body posture influence hand preference in an ancestral primate model?BMC Evolutionary Biology 11:52.Google Scholar
Schilling, A. 1979. Olfactory communication in prosimians. In Doyle, GA, Martin, RD (eds.), The Study of Prosimian Behavior (pp. 461–538). Academic Press, New-York.
Schilling, A. 2000. Sensory organs and communication in Microcebus murinus. Primatologie 3:85–143.Google Scholar
Schilling, A. 2013. Cognitive capacities of captive gray mouse lemurs as evidenced by object manipulation. In Masters, J, Gamba, M, Génin, F (eds.), Leaping Ahead: Advances in Prosimian Biology (pp. 331–340). Springer, New York.
Schülke, O, Ostner, J. 2005. Big times for dwarfs: social organisation, sexual selection, and cooperation in the Cheirogaleidae. Evolutionary Anthropology 14:170–185.Google Scholar
Shepherd, SV, Platt, ML. 2008. Spontaneous social orienting and gaze following in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Animal Cognition 11(1):13–20.Google Scholar
Sherry, DF, Schacter, DL. 1987. The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psychological Review 94(4):439–454.Google Scholar
Sherwood, CC, Gordon, AD, Allen, JS, et al. 2011. Aging of the cerebral cortex differs between humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108(32):13029–13034.Google Scholar
Squire, LR. 1992. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review 99(2):195–231.Google Scholar
Squire, LR. 2004. Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 82(3):171–177.Google Scholar
Squire, LR, Zola-Morgan, S. 1991. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 253(5026):1380–1386.Google Scholar
Stoub, TR, Rogalski, EJ, Leurgans, S, Bennett, DA, deToledo-Morrell, L. 2010. Rate of entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and mild AD: relation to memory function. Neurobiology of Aging 31(7):1089–1098.Google Scholar
Struble, RG, Price, DL Jr, Cork, LC, Price, DL. 1985. Senile plaques in cortex of aged normal monkeys. Brain Research 361(1–2):267–275.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M, Call, J. 1997. Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press, New York.
Trouche, SG, Maurice, T, Rouland, S, Verdier, JM, Mestre-Francés, N. 2010. The three-panel runway maze adapted to Microcebus murinus reveals age-related differences in memory and perseverance performances. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 94(1):100–106.Google Scholar
Valdois, S, Joanette, Y, Poissant, A, Ska, B, Dehaut, F. 1990. Heterogeneity in the cognitive profile of normal elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 12(4):587–596.Google Scholar
Vinot, N, Jouin, M, Lhomme-Duchadeuil, A, et al. 2011. Omega-3 fatty acids from fish oil lower anxiety, improve cognitive functions and reduce spontaneous locomotor activity in a non-human primate. PLoS ONE 6(6):e20491.Google Scholar
Wilkerson, J, Rumbaugh, DM. 1979. Learning and intelligence in prosimians. In Doyle, GA, Martin, RD (eds.), The Study of Prosimian Behavior (pp. 207–246). Academic Press, New York.
Wisniewski, H, Johnson, AB, Raine, CS, Kay, WJ, Terry, RD. 1970. Senile plaques and cerebral amyloidosis in aged dogs. A histochemical and ultrastructural study. Laboratory Investigation 23(3):287–296.Google Scholar
Ylikoski, R, Ylikoski, A, Keskivaara, P, et al. 1999. Heterogeneity of cognitive profiles in aging: successful aging, normal aging, and individuals at risk for cognitive decline. European Journal of Neurology 6(6):645–652.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×