Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T23:08:47.298Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Creativity Has No Dark Side

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Mark A. Runco
Affiliation:
University of Georgia, Athens
David H. Cropley
Affiliation:
University of South Australia
Arthur J. Cropley
Affiliation:
University of Hamburg
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University at San Bernardino
Mark A. Runco
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Creativity does not have a dark side. Creative products and efforts can be malevolent, but that is apparent in their impact and is not an inherent quality of creativity nor a requisite trait in the creative personality. Claiming that there is a dark side to creativity is much like arguing that hammers are evil because they can be used to dismantle as well as construct things. Creativity is indeed in some ways a tool of humanity, but of course that is merely a metaphor and, as such, only imperfectly applicable. The important point is that the process that underlies all creative things is not moral or immoral, ethical or unethical, good or evil. It is essentially blind. Like a tool, it can be applied in many different ways, some of which are benevolent and some of which are unethical and immoral, but to understand creativity it is best to be parsimonious and leave out what is extraneous, and that includes all possible effects. This chapter develops this view of parsimonious creativity and describes the ostensible dark side as a function of values and decisions that are ancillary to actual creative work.

There is no denying that creative talents have in the past been used in highly unfortunate ways. Many famous examples of this have been described by McLaren (1993), Stein (1993), and the authors of others chapters in this volume.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aries, P. (1973). Centuries of childhood. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Barron, F. (1993). Controlled weirdness. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 182–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. (1999). Mad genius. In M. A. Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity. San Diego, CA: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1989/1934). Patterns of culture. New York: Mariner Books.Google Scholar
Bergson, H. (2002/1946). The creative mind. New York: Citadel.Google Scholar
Bower, R. (1999). Dangerous minds: Eminently creative people who have spent time in jail. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 3–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. 1962. The conditions of creativity. In J. Bruner (Ed.), On knowing: Essays for the left hand. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 105–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollinger, S., Burke, P A., & Gump, N. W. (2007). Creativity and values. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 91–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1991). Bully for Brontosaurus. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Gruber, H. E. (1993). Creativity in the moral domain: Ought implies can implies create. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1968). Creativity, intelligence and their educational implications. San Diego, CA: EDITS/Knapp.Google Scholar
Gutbezahl, J. & Averill, R. J. (1996). Individual differences in emotional creativity as manifested in words and pictures. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 327–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K., Runco, M. A., Dorinan, C., & Freeland, D. C. (1997). Influential factors in artists' lives and themes in their art work. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 221–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasof, J. (1995). Explaining creativity: The attributional perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 311–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasof, J., Chen, C., Himsel, A. & Greenberger, E. (2007). Values and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 105–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1987). The development of moral judgment and moral action. In L. Kohlberg (Ed.), Child psychology and childhood education: A cognitive developmental view. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 352–367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemons, G. (2005). When the horse drinks: Enhancing everyday creativity using elements of improvisation. Creativity Research Journal,17, 25–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, M. G. (2007). A look at Wernher von Braun, patriarch of U.S. rocketry, Nazi past and all. Review of Von Braun: Dreamer of Space, Engineer of War, by Michael J. Neufeld. Los Angeles Times Book Review. Available at www.latimes.com/features/books/la-bk-lord23sep23,0,4789920.story?coll= la-books-headlines; accessed September 23, 2007.
Lumsden, C. J. & Findlay, S. C. (1988). Evolution of the creative mind. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 75–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1973). Creativity in self-actualizing people. In A. Rothenberg and C. R. Hausman (Eds.), The creative question (pp. 86–92). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Maugh, T. H. (2009). Venus figurine sheds light on origins of art by early humans. Los Angeles Times Science. Available at www.latimes.com/news/nationworld/nation/la–sci–venus14–2009may14,0,181830.story. Accessed on May 14, 2009.
Mednick, S.A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 222–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaren, R. B. (1993). The dark side of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 137–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milgram, R. M. & Rabkin, L. (1980). Developmental test of Mednick's associative hierarchies of original thinking. Developmental Psychology, 16, 157–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norlander, T., Nordmarker, A., & Archer, T. (1998). Effects of alcohol and frustration on experimental graffiti. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 201–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordmarker, A., Norlander, T., & Archer, T. (2000). The effects of alcohol intake and induced frustration upon art vandalism. Social Behavior and Personality, 28, 15–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, R. (1990). Everyday creativity, eminent creativity, and health: Afterview for CRJ issues on creativity and health. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 300–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rickards, T. & deCock, C. (in press). Understanding organizational creativity: Towards a multiparadigmatic approach. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity research handbook (vol. 2). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Rogers, C. R. (1954/1959). Toward a theory of creativity. In H. H. Anderson (Ed.), Creativity and its cultivation (pp. 69–82). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, E. & Winner, E. (1988). The art of children's drawings. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, A. 1990. Creativity, mental health, and alcoholism. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 179–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1988). Creativity research: Originality, utility, and integration. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1989). Parents' and teachers' ratings of the creativity of children. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 73–83.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (Ed.). (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1993). Moral creativity: Intentional and unconventional. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 17–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1994). Conclusions concerning problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 272–290). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1995). Insight for creativity, expression for impact. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 377–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1996). Creativity and development: Recommendations. New Directions for Child Development, 72, 87–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2003). Education for creative potential. Scandinavian Journal of Education, 47, 317–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2007). A hierarchical framework for the study of creativity. New Horizons in Education, 55(3), 1–9.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2010). Parsimonious creativity and its measurement. In E. Villalba (Ed.), Measuring creativity: Proceedings of European Council meeting on creativity and innovation (pp. 393–405). Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (Ed.). (In press-a). Divergent and creative thinking. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Runco, M. A. (in press-b). Divergent thinking. In Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, J. (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Runco, M. A. & Albert, R. S. (In press). Creativity research: A history. In R. J. Sternberg, & J. Kaufman (Eds.), Handbook of creativity research. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Runco, M.A. & Basadur, M. (1993). Assessing ideational and evaluative skills and creative styles and attitudes. Creativity and Innovation Management, 2, 166–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., Johnson, D., & Gaynor, J. R. (1997). The judgmental bases of creativity and implications for the study of gifted youth. In A. Fishkin, B. Cramond, & P. Olszewski–Kubilius (Eds.), Creativity in youth: Research and methods. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Kaufman, J. C., Halladay, L. R., & Cole, J. C. (in press). Change in reputation as index of genius and eminence. Historical Methods.
Runco, M. A. & Shaw, M. P. (1994). Conclusions concerning creativity and affect. In M. P. Shaw & M. A. Runco (Eds.), Creativity and affect (pp. 261–270). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative ideas. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 295–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Vega, L. (1990). Evaluating the creativity of children's ideas. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 439–452.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2008). The creative process in Picasso's Guernica sketches: Monotonic improvements versus nonmonotonic variants. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 329–344.Google Scholar
Spooner, M. T. (2008). Commentary on malevolent creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 128–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, M. I. (1993). Moral issues facing intermediaries between creators and the public. Creativity Research Journal, 6. 197–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1995). Why fly?Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1996). Presymbolic processes in scientific creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 163–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×