Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-tsvsl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-30T23:57:17.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2015

Vyvyan Evans
Affiliation:
Bangor University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Crucible of Language
How Language and Mind Create Meaning
, pp. 334 - 355
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A., Schubotz, R.I. and Cramon, D.Y.von. (2008). Thinking about the future versus the past in personal and non-personal contexts. Brain Research, 1233: 106–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aitchison, J. (1996). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Antón, S.C. (2003). Natural history of Homo erectus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122: 126–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and Experience. Baltimore, MA: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. (2012). How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arsuaga, J.L., Martínez, L., Lorenzo, C., Gracia, A., Muñoz, A., Alonso, O. and Gallego, J.. (1999). The human cranial remains from Gran Dolina Lower Pleisticene site (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 37/3–4: 431–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arterberry, M.E. and Bornstein, M.H.. (2001). Three-month-old infants’ categorization of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80: 333–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkins, B.T.S. (1987). Semantic-ID tags: Corpus evidence for dictionary senses. In The Uses of Large Text Databases: Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the New OED Centre, pp. 17–36. University of Waterloo, Canada.Google Scholar
Bak, T.H., O'Donovan, D.G., Xuereb, J.H., Boniface, S. and Hodges, J.R.. (2001). Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neuron disease-dementia-aphasia syndrome. Brain, 124: 103–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Yosef, O. and Belfer-Cohen, A.. (2001). From Africa to Eurasia – early dispersals. Quaternary International, 75: 19–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. (1992). Cognitive Psychology: An Overview for Cognitive Scientists. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22/4: 577–609.Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L.W. (2005). Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9: 309–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L.W., Santo, A., Simmons, W.K. and Wilson, C.D.. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In Vega, M.De, Glenberg, A.M. and Graesser, A.C. (Eds.), Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning, pp. 245–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bayle, P., Macchiarelli, R., Trinkaus, E., Duarte, C., Mazurier, A. and Zilhão, J.. (2010). Dental maturational sequence and dental tissue proportions in the early Upper Paleolithic child from Abrigodo Lagar Velho, Portugal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107: 1338–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behme, C. (2014). A Galilean science of language. Journal of Linguistics, 50/3: 671–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthenthal, B. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In Granrud, C. (Ed.), Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy, pp. 175–214. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C., Friederici, A.D., Chomsky, N. and Bolhuis, J.J.. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17: 89–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and Species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2002). From proto-language to language. In Crow, T. (Ed.)., The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, pp. 103–20. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94: 115–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. and Calvin, W.H.. (2000). Lingua ex Machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1911). The Mind of Primitive Man. London: Macmillan.
Boden, M.A. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. London: Weidenfield and Nicholson.Google Scholar
Boden, M.A. (1994a). Précis of The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17/3: 519–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M.A. (1994b). Creativity: A framework for research. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 17/3: 558–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M.A. (1995). Modelling creativity: Reply to reviewers. Journal ofArtificial Intelligence, 79: 161–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M.A. (1996). What is creativity? In Boden, M.A. (Ed.), Dimensions of Creativity, pp. 75–118. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H.. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H.. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Tai National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78: 547–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H.. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatology, 54: 86–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75/1: 1–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulenger, V., Mechtou, L., Thobois, S., Broussolle, E., Jeannerod, M. and Nazir, T.A.. (2008). Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns. Neuropsychologia, 46: 743–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., Sutikna, T., Morwood, M.J., Soejono, R.P., Jatmiko, E., Saptomo, W. and Rokus, A.D.. (2004). A new small-bodied hominin from the Late Pleistocene of Flores, Indonesia. Nature, 431: 1055–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1976). From communication to language – a psychological perspective. Cognition, 3: 255–87.Google Scholar
Buccino, G., Riggio, T.L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G.. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24: 355–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Büchel, C., Price, C. and Friston, K.. (1998). A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway. Nature, 394: 274–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhusi, C.V. and Meck, W.H.. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. National Review of Neuroscience, 6/10: 755–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z.G. and Connell, L.. (2012). Space-time interdependence and sensory modalities: Time affects space in the hand but not in the eye. In Miyake, N., Peebles, D. and Cooper, R.P. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 168–73. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Cai, Z. G. and Connell, L.. (2015). Space-time interdependence: Evidence against asymmetric mapping between time and space. Cognition, 136: 268–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2007). The Gestural Communications of Apes and Monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12/5: 187–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S.S. (1986). Estimation of empty time. Human Neurobiology, 5, 205–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Cartmill, M. and Smith, F.H.. (2009). The Human Lineage. London: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Carretero, J.M., Rodríguez, L., García-González, R., Arsuaga, J.L., Gómez-Olivencia, A., Lorenzo, C., Bonmatí, A., Gracia, A. and Martínez, I.. (2012). Stature estimation from complete long bones in the Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sima de los Huesos, Sierra de Atapuerca (Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 62/2: 242–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. (2010). Space for thinking. In Evans, V. and Chilton, P. (Eds.), Language, Cognition, and Space: State of the Art and New Directions, pp. 453–78. London: Equinox Publishing.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. and Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106: 579–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D.L. and Seyfarth, R.M.. (1990). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (1998). STRAIGHT: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9/2: 107–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cisek, P. (1999). Beyond the computer metaphor: Behaviour as interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6/11–12: 125–42.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, J., de Heinzelin, J., Schick, K., Hart, W., White, T., WoldeGabriel, G., Walter, R., Suwa, G., Asfaw, B. and Vrba, E.. (1994). African Homo erectus: Old radiometric ages and young Oldowan assemblages in the middle Awash Valley, Ethiopia. Science, 264/5167: 1907–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. (2002). From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. (2008). Not the last word. Book review of The First Word: The Search for the Origins of Language by Christine Kenneally in American Scientist, Jan-Feb, Scientist's Nightstand, available online at www.americanscientist.org/bookshelf/pub/not-the-last-word
Coulson, S. (2000). Semantic Leaps: Frame-Shifting and Conceptual Blending in Meaning Construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coveney, P. and Highfield, R.. (1991). The Arrow of Time: The Quest to Solve Science's Greatest Mystery. London: Flamingo.Google Scholar
Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. (1998). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2009). Words as constructions. In Evans, V. and Pourcel, S. (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 201–24. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dahan, D. and Tanenhaus, M.K.. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Evidence from immediate effects of verb-based constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30: 498–513.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1995). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species (). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2010). The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution. London: Black Swan.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Deecke, V. (2012). Tool-use in the brown bear (Ursus arctos). Animal Cognition, 15/4: 725–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1989). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Backwell, L., Villa, P. and colleagues. (2012). Early evidence of San material culture represented by organic artifacts from Border Cave, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 10.1073/pnas.1204213109.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F. and Soressi, M.. (2002). Systematic use of manganese pigment by the Pech-de-l'Azé Neandertals: Implications for the origin of behavioural modernity. Journal of Human Evolution, 42: A13.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M.. (2009). Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origin of language debate. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 16–40. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DiMaggio, E.N., Campisano, C.J., Rowan, J., Dupont-Nivet, G., Deino, A.L., Bibi, F., Lewis, M.E., Souron, A., Garello, D., Werdelin, L., Reed, K.E. and Arrowsmith, J. Ramón. (2015). Late Pliocene fossiliferous sedimentary record and the environmental context of early Homo from Afar, Ethiopia. Science, 347/6228: 1355–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuBrul, E. (1977). Origins of the speech apparatus and its reconstruction in fossils. Brain and Language, 4: 365–81.Google Scholar
Dediu, D. and Levinson, S.C.. (2013). On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4: 397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, R.I.M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Evans, V. (2004). The Structure of Time: Language, Meaning and Temporal Cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2009). How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models and Meaning Construction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2010). Figurative language understanding in LCCM Theory. Cognitive Linguistics 21/4: 601–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2013). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2014). The Language Myth: Why Language is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2015). What's in a concept? Analogue versus parametric concepts in LCCM Theory. In Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (Eds.), The Conceptual Mind: New Directions in the Study of Concepts, pp. 251–90. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Evans, V. and Green, M.. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology, 46/4: 621–646.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2009). Don't Sleep, There are Snakes: Life and Language in the Amazonian Jungle. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2012). Language: The Cultural Tool. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1975). Comparative anatomy of the larynx in man and the chimpanzee: Implications for language in Neanderthal. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 43: 123–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2000). Compression and global insight. Cognitive Linguistics. 11/3-4: 283–304.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind's Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books.
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2008). The origin of language as a product of the evolution of double-scope blending. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31/5: 520–1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W.T. (2009a). Musical Protolanguage: Darwin's Theory of Language Evolution Revisited. Available online at http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=1136.
Fitch, W.T. (2009b). Fossil cues to the evolution of speech. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 112–34. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Flaherty, M. (1999). A Watched Pot: How We Experience Time. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Floss, M. (2003). Did they meet or not. Observations on Châtelperronian and Aurignacian settlement patterns in eastern France. In Zilhão, J. and d'Errico, F. (Eds.), The Chronology of the Aurignacian and of the Transitional Technocomplexes, pp. 273–88. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Português de Arqueologia.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1981). The current status of the innateness controversy. In Representations, pp. 257–316. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1995). The Elm and Expert: Mentalese and its Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1998). Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (2008). LOT2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35: 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N.H., Lloyd, S. and Sinha, C.. (1980). Infant search tasks reveal early concepts of containment and canonical usage of objects. Cognition, 8: 243–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye, D., Rawling, P., Moore, C. and Myers, I.. (1983). Object–person discrimination at 3 and 10 months. Developmental Psychology, 19: 303–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galton, A. (2011). Time flies but space doesn't: Limits to the spatialization of time. Journal of Pragmatics, 43: 695–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamble, C. and Marshall, G.. (2001). The shape of handaxes, the structure of the Acheulian world. In Milliken, S. and Cook, J.D. (Eds.), A Very Remote Period Indeed. Papers on the Palaeolithic presented to Derek Roe, pp. 19–27. Oxford: Oxbow.Google Scholar
Gavins, J. and Steen, G.. (2003). Cognitive Poetics in Practice. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniger, M. (2006 ).Leon Festinger: Lunch with Leon. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1: 88–94.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (2009). Theories of Lexical Semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai, M. and Boroditsky, L.. (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17/5: 537–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.W. (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. (1986). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Verb Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalizations in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gomes, C.M. and Boesch, C.. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. ONE, 4/4: e5116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, C. (2003). Conversation and Brain Damage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A.M. and Kaschak, M. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9/3: 558–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997a). Foundations of Meaning: Primary Metaphors and Primary Scenes. Doctoral thesis, Linguistics Dept, University of California, Berkeley (available from UMI Dissertation Services: www.il.proquest.com/umi/dissertations/).Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997b). THEORIES ARE BUILDINGS revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8/4: 267–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grady, J. (1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In Gibbs, R.W. and Steen, G. (Eds.), Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 79–100. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grady, J. and Johnson, C.. (2000). Converging evidence for the notions of ‘subscene’ and ‘primary scene’, Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 123–36. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Gräfenhaim, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M.. (2009). Young children's understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology, 45/5: 1430–43.Google Scholar
Green, R.E., Krause, J., Briggs, A.W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U. and Kircher, M.. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328: 710–722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D.J. and Alemseged, Z.. (2012). Australopithecus afarensis scapular ontogeny, function, and the role of climbing in human evolution. Science, 338/6106: 514–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, R. (1998). Eye and Brain (). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2006). Corpus-based methods and cognitive semantics: The many meanings of to run. In Gries, S. Th. and Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis, pp. 57–99. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haley, D. (Ed.) (1986). Sea otter. Marine Mammals of Eastern North Pacific and Arctic Waters (). Seattle, WA: Pacific Search Press.Google Scholar
Hampe, B. (2005). From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42: 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B.J., Hart, L.A., McCory, M. and Sarath, C.R.. (2001). Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: Use and modification of branches for fly switching. Animal Behaviour 62/5: 839–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, J.B. (1974). A last look at Cook's Guugu-Yimidhirr wordlist. Oceania, 44/3: 216–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawks, J. (2013). Neandertal Ancestry and Founder Effects. John Hawks Weblog. Available at: http://johnhawks.net/weblog/topics/introgression.
Hebb, D.O. (2002). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory (new edition). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B., Claudi, U. and Hünnemeyer, F.. (1991). Grammaticalization: A Conceptual Framework. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T.. (2007). The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hespos, S.J. and Baillargeon, R.. (2001). Reasoning about containment events in very young infants. Cognition, 78: 207–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Houghton, P. (1993). Neandertal supralaryngeal vocal tract. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 90: 139–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hublin, J.-J. (2009). Out of Africa: Modern human origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106: 16022–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Hulme, M. (2008). The conquering of climate: Discourses of fear and their dissolution. The Geographical Journal, 174/1: 5–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurford, J. (2007). The Origins of Meaning. Language in the Light of Evolution I. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. (2012). The Origins of Grammar. Language in the Light of Evolution II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., Beattie, B., Leon, A.C., and Stern, E.. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 96: 10456–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2010). Your theory of language evolution depends on your theory of language. In Larson, R., Déprez, V. and Yamakido, H. (Eds.), The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives, pp. 63–72. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1942). Six Lectures on Sound and Meaning. Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jelinek, E. (1995). Quantification in Straits Salish. In Bach, E., Jelinek, E., Kratzer, A. and Partee, B. (Eds.), Quantification in Natural Languages, pp. 487–540. New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Johanson, D.C. and Edey, M.A.. (1981). Lucy: The Beginnings of Humankind. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14: 201–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, C. (1999). Metaphor vs. conflation in the acquisition of polysemy: The case ofsee. In Hiraga, M., Sinha, C. and Wilcox, S. (Eds.), Cultural, Typological and Psychological Perspectives in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 155–69. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Joormens, J., d' Errico, F., Wesselingh, F.P. and colleagues. (2014). Homo erectus at Trinil on Java used shells for tool production and engraving. Nature, 10.1038/nature13962.
Julesz, B. (1981). Textons: The elements of texture perception and their interactions. Nature, 290: 91–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Julian, R. (2005). A John Dee Chronology, 1509–1609. Renaissance Man: The Reconstructed Libraries of European Scholars: 1450–1700 Series One: The Books and Manuscripts of John Dee, 1527–1608. Marlborough, Wiltshire: Adam Matthew Publications.Google Scholar
Kellman, P.J. and Spelke, E.S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15: 483–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendon, A. (1991). Some considerations for a theory of language origins. Man, 26/2: 199–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kingdon, J. (2004). Lowly Origin: Where, When, and Why Our Ancestors First Stood Up. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Klatzky, R.L., Pellegrino, J.W., McCloskey, B.P and Doherty, S.. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgements. Journal of Memory and Language, 28: 56–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R.G. (2009). The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Origins (). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kojima, S. (1990). Comparison of auditory functions in the chimpanzee and human. Folia Primatology, 55: 62–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Koops, K., McGrew, W.C. and Matsuzawa, T.. (2010). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) use cleavers and anvils to fracture Treculia africana fruits? Preliminary data on a new form of percussive technology. Primates, 51/2: 175–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kövescses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kranjec, A., Lehet, M. and Chatterjee, A.. (2013). Space and time are mutually contagious in sound. In Knauff, M., Pauen, M., Sebanz, N. and Wachsmuth, I. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 829–33, http://cognitivesciencesociety.org/conference_past.html
Kubovy, M. (1988). Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time::vision:audition analogy?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14/2: 318–20.Google Scholar
Lafon, J.-C. (1968). Auditory basis of phonetics. In Malmberg, B. (Ed.), Manual of Phonetics, pp. 76–104. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image schemas?Cognitive Linguistics, 1/1: 39–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1991). Metaphor and War: The Metaphor System Used to Justify War in the Gulf. An open letter to the Internet.
Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (second edition), pp. 202–51. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2004). Don't Think of an Elephant: Know Your Values and Frame the Debate. White River Junction, VM: Chelsea Green.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In Gibbs, R.W. (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, pp. 17–38. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Turner, M.. (1987). More Than Cool Reason: A Field-guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lalueza-Fox, C., Rosas, A., Estalrrich, A., Gigli, E., Campos, P.F. and García-Tabernero, A.. (2010). Genetic evidence for patrilocal mating behaviour among Neandertal groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108: 250–3.Google ScholarPubMed
Langacker, R. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar (Vol. I). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R.W. (2013). Essentials of Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langone, J. (2000). The Mystery of Time: Humanity's Quest for Order and Measure. Washington DC: National Geographic Press.Google Scholar
Larsen, C. (2003). Equality for the sexes in human evolution? Early hominid sexual dimorphism and implications for mating systems and social behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100/16: 9103–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leakey, M.G., Spoor, F., Brown, F.H., Gathogo, P.N., Kiarie, C., Leakey, L.N. and McDougallet, I.. (2001). New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature, 410: 433–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leakey, R.E. (1981). The Making of Mankind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J.E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46: 209–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, N., Mikesell, L., Joaquin, A.D., Mates, A.W. and Schumann, J.H.. (2009). The Interactional Instinct: The Evolution and Acquisition of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1992). A review of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing. Early Development and Parenting, 1: 59–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Poidevin, R. (2003). Travels in Four Dimensions: The Enigmas of Space and Time. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A.M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11: 173–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A.M. (1984). Infant perception of a manual pick-up event. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2: 19–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cultural Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C. (2006). On the human ‘interaction engine’. In Enfield, N.J. and Levinson, S.C. (Eds.). Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction, pp. 39–69. Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D.E. (2001). Another face in our family tree. Nature, 410: 419–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, P. (2015). Review of The Science of Language: Interviews with James McGilvray, by Chomsky, Noam. Modern Language Review, 110/1: 222–4.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. and Crelin, E.. (1971). On the speech of Neanderthal man. Linguistic Inquiry, 2: 203–22.Google Scholar
Linden, S.J. (2003). The Alchemy Reader: from Hermes Trismegistus to Isaac Newton. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E., Schooler, J.W., Boone, S.M. and Kline, D.. (1987). Time went by so slowly: Overestimation of event duration by males and females. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1/1: 3–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lozano, M., Mosquera, M., Castro, J.De, Bermúdez, M., Arsuaga, J.L. and Carbonell, E.. (2009). Right handedness of Homo heidelbergensis from Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Spain) 500,000 years ago. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30/5: 369–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J.A. (1992). Language Diversity and Thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lurz, R. (2011). Mindreading Animals: The Debate about What Animals Know, and Other Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntosh, J.J. and Anstey, P.. (2014). Robert Boyle. In Zalta, E.N. (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2014/entries/boyle/.
Mandler, J.M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3: 113–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J.M. (1992). How to build a baby II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99: 567–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, J.M. (1996). Preverbal representation and language. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M.A., Nadel, L. and Garrett, M.F. (Eds.), Language and Space, pp. 365–84. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J.M. (2004). The Foundations of Mind: Origins of Conceptual Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A. and Chao, L.L.. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11: 194–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez, I., Quam, R.M., Rosa, M., Jarabo, P., Lorenzo, C. and Arsuaga, J.L.. (2008). Auditory capacities of human fossils: A new approach to the origin of speech. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 123: 3606–3606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez, I., Rosa, M., Arsuaga, J.-L., Jarabo, P., Quam, R. and Lorenzo, C.. (2004). Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierrade Atapuerca in Spain. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences, USA, 101: 9976–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPherron, S.P., Alemseged, Z., Marean, C., Wynn, J.G., Reed, D., Geraads, D., Bobe, R. and Béarat, H.. (2011). Tool-marked bones from before the oldowan change the paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108/21: e116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M.. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198: 75–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mithen, S. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Science and Religion. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Miracle, A.W. Jr. and de Dios Yapita Moya, J.. (1981). Time and space in Aymara. In Hardman, M. J.. (ed), The Aymara Language and Its Social and Cultural Context. Gainesville, FL: University Presses of Florida, 33–56.Google Scholar
Moore, B.C. (1977). Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. Baltimore, MA: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Moore, K.E. (2006). Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cognitive Linguistics, 17/2: 199–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, K.E. (2013). The Spatial Language of Time: Metaphor, Metonymy and Frames of Reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mounier, A., Marchal, F. and Condemi, S.. (2009). Is Homo heidelbergensis a distinct species? New insight on the Mauer mandible. Journal of Human Evolution, 56/3: 219–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, M. (1861). The theoretical stage, and the origin of language. Lecture 9 from Lectures on the Science of Language. Reprinted in Harris, R. (Ed.), 1996, The Origin of Language, pp. 7–41. Bristol, Avon: Thoemmes Press.Google Scholar
Nerlich, B., Evans, V. and Koteyko, N.. (2011). Low carbon diet: Reducing the complexities of climate change to human scale. Language and Cognition, 3/1: 45–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, W.R. and Principle, L.M.. (2002). Alchemy Tried in the Fire. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., Motz, B. and Teuscher, U.. (2006). Time after time: The psychological reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21: 133–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. and Sweetser, E.. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30: 401–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paget, R. (1930). Human Speech: Some Observations, Experiments, and Conclusions as to the Nature, Origin, Purpose and Possible Improvement of Human Speech. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pansini, R. and de Ruiter, J.R.. (2011). Observation of tool use and modification for apparent hygiene purposes in a mandrill. Behavioural Processes 88/1: 53–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, A. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100: 1–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettitt, P.B. (2002).The Neanderthal dead: Exploring mortuary variability in Middle Palaeolithic Eurasia. Before Farming, 1: 1–19.Google Scholar
Peterson, D. (2011). The Moral Lives of Animals. London: Bloomsbury Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). The Language and Thought of the Child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pierce, C.S. (1903). A syllabus of certain topics of logic. EP, 2: 272–3.Google Scholar
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2005). Gestural communication of apes. Gesture, 5/1–2: 41–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W.W. Norton and co.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (2002). Emotions and Life: Perspectives from Psychology, Biology, and Evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Pöppel, E. (1994). Temporal mechanisms in perception. In Sporns, O. and Tononi, G. (Eds.), Selectionism and the Brain: International Review of Neurobiology, 37: 185–201.Google ScholarPubMed
Pöppel, E. (2009). Pre-semantically defined temporal windows for cognitive processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364: 1887–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pruetz, J. and Bertolani, P.. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17: 412–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22: 253–79.Google ScholarPubMed
Raichlen, D.A., Gordon, A.D., Harcourt-Smith, W.E.H., Foster, A.A. and Haas, Wm.R. Jr. (2010). Laetoli footprints preserve earliest direct evidence of human-like bipedal biomechanics. PLoS ONE, 5/3: e9769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rak, Y., Ginzburg, A. and Geffen, E.. (2007). Gorilla-like anatomy on Australopithecus afarensis mandibles suggests Au. Afarensis link to robust australopiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104/16: 6568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratliff, F., and Hartline, H.K.. (1974). Studies on Excitation and Inhibition in the Retina. New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. (1990). Archaeology and Language: The Puzzle of Indo-European Origins. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. (2008). Prehistory: The Making of the Human Mind. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Reynolds, S. and Gallagher, A.. (2012). African Genesis: Perspectives on Hominin Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roebroeks, W. and Villa, P.. (2011). On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108: 5209–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Lloyd, B. and Rosch, E. (Eds.), Cognition and Categorization, pp. 27–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. (1921). Language: The Scientific Study of Speech. NY: Harcourt Brace and company.Google Scholar
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I. and Padden, C.. (2011). The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29/2: 503–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Aronoff, M.. (2005). The emergence of grammar in a new sign language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102/7: 2661–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandra, D. and Rice, S.. (1995). Network analyses of prepositional meaning: mirroring whose mind – the linguist's or the language user's?Cognitive Linguistics, 6/1: 89–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. de. (1916). Course in General Linguistics. London: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L., Addis, D.R. and Buckner, R.L.. (2007). The prospective brain: Remembering the past to imagine the future. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8: 657–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmelz, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2012). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108/7: 3077–9.Google Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2000). English Abstract Nouns as Conceptual Shells: From Corpus to Cognition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senut, B., Pickford, M., Gommery, D., Mein, P., Cheboi, K. and Coppens, Y. (2001). First hominid from the Miocene (Lukeino Formation, Kenya). Comptes Rendus de l'Academie des Sciences, Series IIA – Earth and Planetary Science 332/2: 137–44.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L. and Marler, P.. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210: 801–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, L. (2010). Embodied Cognition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shipman, P. (2001). The Man who Found the Missing Link: The Extraordinary Life of Eugène Dubois. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K. (1999). Epistemology of Space and Time. Kwansei, Japan: Kwansei Gakuin University Press.Google Scholar
Shumaker, R.W., Walkup, K.R. and Beck, B.B.. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Baltimore, MA: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Siffre, M. (1964). Beyond Time. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Sinha, C. (1982). Representational development and the structure of action. In Butterworth, P. and Light, G. (Eds.), Social Cognition: Studies of the Development of Understanding, pp. 137–62. Brighton: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Sinha, C. (1988). Language and Representation: A Socio-Naturalistic Approach to Human Development. Hove: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Sinha, C., da Silva Sinha, V., Zinken, J. and Sampaio, W.. (2011). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3/1: 137–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, B. (2009). Energy use by Eem Neanderthals. Journal of Archaeological Science, 36: 2201–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. and Wilson, D.. (1996). Relevance: Communication and Cognition (second edition). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spivey, M., Tyler, M., Richardson, D. and Young, E.. (2000). Eye movements during comprehension of spoken scene descriptions. In Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 487–92. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spoor, F., Leakey, M.G., Gathogo, P.N., Brown, F.H., Antón, S.C., McDougall, I., Kiarie, C., Manthi, F.K. and Leakey, L.N.. (2007). Implications of new early Homo fossils from Ileret, east of Lake Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 448/7154: 688–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanfield, R.A. and Zwaan, R.A.. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12: 153–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. (2011). Rationality and the Reflective Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, J.T. Jr. and Susman, R.L.. (1983). The locomotor anatomy of Australopithecus afarensis. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 60/3: 279–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stockwell, P. (2002). Cognitive Poetics: An Introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1988). Grammaticalization and semantic bleaching. In Axmaker, S., Jaisser, A. and Singmaster, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 389–405. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics (two volumes). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tattersall, T. (2012). Masters of the Planet: The Search for Our Human Origins. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. (2002). Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. (2003). Linguistic Categorization (third edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. (2012). The Mental Corpus: How Language is Represented in the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, L. and Zwaan, R. A.. (2009). Action in cognition: The case of language. Language and Cognition, 1/1: 45–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B. and Kuipers, J-R.. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 106/11: 4567–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). The Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In Gelfand, M. (Ed.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (pp. 5–51). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J.. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. (1998). The Origins of Humankind. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication, pp. 321–47. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turing, A.M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59: 433–60.Google Scholar
Turner, F. and Pöppel, E.. (1983). The neural lyre: Poetic meter, the brain and time. Poetry, 142/5: 277–309.Google Scholar
Turner, M. (1996). The Literary Mind: The Origins of Thought and Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Turner, M. (2014). The Origin of Ideas: Blending, Creativity and the Human Spark. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tyler, A. (2012). Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Learning: Theoretical Basics and Experimental Evidence. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tyler, A. and Evans, V.. (2001). Reconsidering prepositional polysemy networks: The case of over. Language, 77/4: 724–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A. and Evans, V.. (2003). The Semantics of English Prepositions: Spatial Scenes, Embodied Experience, and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungar, P. (2004). Dental topography and diets of Australopithecus afarensis and earlyHomo. Journal of Human Evolution, 46: 605–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Van Schaik, C., Fox, E. and Sitompul, A.. (1996). Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften, 83: 186–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Vandeloise, C. (1994). Methodology and analyses of the preposition in. Cognitive Linguistics, 5/2: 157–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, F. (1999). Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6/2-3: 111–40.Google Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E. and Rosch, E.. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Verri, G., Barkai, R., Bordeanu, V., Gopher, A., Hass, M. and Kaufman, A.. (2004). Flint mining in prehistory recorded by in situ-produced cosmogenic10Be. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101: 7880–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M. and Kousta, S.. (2009). Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition, 1/2: 219–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villa, P. and Roebroeks, W.. (2014). Neanderthal demise: An archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS ONE, 9/4: e96424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, J. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and ‘the game’. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18: 323–40.Google Scholar
Watts, I. (2009). Red ochre, body painting, and language: Interpreting the Blombos ochre. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 62–92. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Watts, D. and Mitani, J.C.. (2002). Hunting and meat sharing by Chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 23: 1–28.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1923 [1938]). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt II, inPsycologische Forschung, 4, 301–350. Published as: Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms, in Ellis, W. (1938). A source book of Gestalt psychology, pp. 71–88. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
White, T.D., Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., Haile-Selassie, Y., Lovejoy, C.O., Suwa, G. and WoldeGabriel, G.. (2009). Ardipithecus ramidus and the paleobiology of early hominids. Science, 326 /5949: 75–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, T.D., Suwa, G., Simpson, S. and Asfaw, B.. (2000). Jaws and teeth of Australopithecus afarensis from Maka, Middle Awash, Ethiopia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1111: 45–68.Google Scholar
Whitehead, H. (2003). Sperm Whales: Social Evolution in the Ocean. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Whorf, B.L. (1956). Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P., and Coslett, H.B.. (2010). The image of time: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. Neuroimage 49: 1728–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wildman, D.E., Uddin, M., Guozhen, L., Grossman, L.I. and Goodman, M.. (2003). Implications of Natural Selection in Shaping 99.4% Nonsynonymous DNA Identity between Humans and Chimpanzees: Enlarging Genus Homo. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100/12: 7181–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, J.Schoville, B.J., Brown, K.S. and Chazan, M.. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338/6109: 942–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J.A. (2012). Lewis Carroll and the private life of words. Review of English Studies, 64/266: 651–71.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358: 749–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1: 35–60.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2001). The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor: A Perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L. (2001). Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D.E., Badal-Garcia, E., d'Errico, F., Daniel, F., and Dayet, L.. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107: 1023–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilhão, J. and Trinkaus, E.. (2003). Social implications. In ‘portrait of the artist as a child'. In Zilhão, J. and Trinkaus, E. (Eds.), The Gravettian Human Skeleton From the Abrigo Do Lagar Velho and its Archaeological Context, pp. 519–41. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Português de Arqueologia.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R.A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B.H. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, pp. 35–62. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R.A. and Yaxley, R.H.. (2003). Hemispheric differences in semantic-relatedness judgments. Cognition, 87: 79–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Crucible of Language
  • Online publication: 05 November 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316403631.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Crucible of Language
  • Online publication: 05 November 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316403631.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Crucible of Language
  • Online publication: 05 November 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316403631.014
Available formats
×