Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:17:02.186Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Reviewing recent empirical findings on creativity and mental illness

from Part I - Creativity and mental illness: the state of the field

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2014

Melanie L. Beaussart
Affiliation:
Psychology researcher and writer
Arielle E. White
Affiliation:
San Francisco State University
Adam Pullaro
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Given that the entire book is devoted to creativity and mental illness, we view this chapter as a chance to highlight key work on the topic and raise some of the questions that will be addressed later in these pages. The historical antecedents of the debate have already been discussed (Becker, this volume), and the previous chapter highlighted the extensive historiometric studies conducted on this topic. Here, therefore, we will primarily focus on empirical research. Many of the studies discussed here are by scholars who have also contributed to this volume; similarly, other chapters have covered specific topics in great detail. We will only briefly allude to this work and instead send interested readers to the relevant parts of this book.

Conceptions of creativity

The connection between creativity and mental illness has been widely debated, both in creativity research and across many other disciplines. One of the biggest sources of variability and conflicting results is the problem of definitions: What exactly do we mean by creativity? Standard definitions of creativity often specify novelty and task-appropriateness (Amabile, 1996; Barron, 1955). Some also include a third concept, such as whether something is surprising (Simonton, 2013) or of high quality (Sternberg et al., 2003). Yet such broad conceptions invariably raise more questions than they answer. In order to interpret empirical results cohesively, there must be a way in which studies of creativity and mental illness define creativity. Certainly, such studies do not consistently highlight how they operationally define creativity (similar to most papers on creativity; see Plucker et al., 2004). Beyond definitions, there are many theories on how to measure creativity, from divergent thinking to remote associations to consensual assessment (Kaufman et al., 2012). Countless further dimensions may be derived from these measures, such as fluency, flexibility, unusual responses, originality, novelty, functionality, aesthetics, or remoteness of associations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A. and Windmann, S. (2008). Selective information processing advantages in creative cognition as a function of schizotypy. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1), 1–6.Google Scholar
Akinola, M. and Mendes, W. B. (2008). The dark side of creativity: Biological vulnerability and negative emotions lead to greater artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(12), 1677–1686.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Anderson, S. (2008). The urge to end it all. New York Times, July 6. , last accessed March 15, 2014.
Andreasen, N. C. (1987). Creativity and mental illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(10), 1288–1292.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. and Glick, I. D. (1988). Bipolar affective disorder and creativity: Implications and clinical management. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29(3), 207–217.Google Scholar
Avila, M., Thaker, G. and Adami, H. (2001). Genetic epidemiology and schizophrenia: A study of reproductive fitness. Schizophrenia Research, 47(2), 233–241.Google Scholar
Barbee, J. G. (1998). Mixed symptoms and syndromes of anxiety and depression: Diagnostic, prognostic, and etiologic issues. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 10(1), 15–29.Google Scholar
Barron, F. (1955). The disposition toward originality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 478.Google Scholar
Bassett, A. S., Bury, A., Hodgkinson, K. A. and Honer, W. G. (1996). Reproductive fitness in familial schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 21(3), 151–160.Google Scholar
Batey, M. and Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(8), 816–821.Google Scholar
Beaussart, M. L., Kaufman, S. B. and Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Creative activity, personality, mental illness, and short-term mating success. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(3), 151–167.Google Scholar
Caplan, P. J. (1996). They say you’re crazy: How the world’s most powerful psychiatrists decide who’s normal. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.
Carlsson, I. (2002). Anxiety and flexibility of defense related to high or low creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 14(3–4), 341–349.Google Scholar
Carlsson, I., Wendt, P. E. and Risberg, J. (2000). On the neurobiology of creativity: Differences in frontal activity between high and low creative subjects. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 873–885.Google Scholar
de Souza, L. C., Volle, E., Bertoux, M., Czernecki, V., Funkiewiez, A., Allali, G. and Dubois, B. (2010). Poor creativity in frontotemporal dementia: A window into the neural bases of the creative mind. Neuropsychologia, 48(13), 3733–3742.Google Scholar
Djikic, M., Oatley, K. and Peterson, J. B. (2006). The bitter-sweet labor of emoting: The linguistic comparison of writers and physicists. Creativity Research Journal, 18(2), 191–197.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290–309.Google Scholar
Fink, A., Slamar-Halbedl, M., Unterrainer, H. F. and Weiss, E. M. (2012). Creativity: Genius, madness, or a combination of both? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 11.Google Scholar
Fisher, J. E., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Koven, N. S., Miller, G. A. and Heller, W. (2004). Neuropsychological evidence for dimensional schizotypy: Implications for creativity and psychopathology. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(1), 24–31.Google Scholar
Forgeard, M. (2008). Linguistic styles of eminent writers suffering from unipolar and bipolar mood disorder. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1), 81–92.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K. and Manfield, J. (2008). Personality, hypomania, intelligence and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(5), 1060–1069.Google Scholar
Galvez, J. F., Thommi, S. and Ghaemi, S. N. (2011). Positive aspects of mental illness: A review in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 128(3), 185–190.Google Scholar
Ghadirian, A., Gregoire, P. and Kosmidis, H. (2001). Creativity and the evolution of psychopathologies. Creativity Research Journal, 13(2), 145–148.Google Scholar
Greenberg, N. (2004). The beast at play: The neuroethology of creativity. In Clements, R. and Fiorentino, L. (Eds.), The child’s right to play: A global approach (pp. 309–327). Westport, CT: Praeger Press.
Heston, L. L. (1966). Psychiatric disorders in foster home reared children of schizophrenic mothers. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112(489), 819–825.Google Scholar
Jamison, K. R. (1989). Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 52(2), 125–134.Google Scholar
Jamison, K. R. (1993). Touched with fire: Manic depressive illness and the artistic temperament. New York: Free Press.
Karimi, Z., Windmann, S., Güntürkün, O. and Abraham, A. (2007). Insight problem solving in individuals with high versus low schizotypy. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(2), 473–480.Google Scholar
Karlsson, J. L. (1984). Creative intelligence in relatives of mental patients. Hereditas, 100(1), 83–86.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. and Baer, J. E. (2002). I bask in dreams of suicide: Mental illness, poetry, and women. Review of General Psychology, 6(3), 271–286.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. and Baer, J. E. (2005). Creativity across domains: Faces of the muse. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kaufman, J. C., Bromley, M. L. and Cole, J. C. (2006). Insane, poetic, lovable: Creativity and endorsement of the “mad genius” stereotype. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 26(1), 149–161.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Plucker, J. A. and Russell, C. M. (2012). Identifying and assessing creativity as a component of giftedness. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 30(1), 60–73.Google Scholar
Kéri, S. (2009). Genes for psychosis and creativity: A promoter polymorphism of the neuregulin 1 gene is related to creativity in people with high intellectual achievement. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1070–1073.Google Scholar
Kwapil, T. R., Barrantes-Vidal, N. and Silvia, P. J. (2008). The dimensional structure of the Wisconsin schizotypy scales: Factor identification and construct validity. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(3), 444–457.Google Scholar
Kyaga, S., Landén, M., Boman, M., Hultman, C. M., Långström, N. and Lichtenstein, P. (2012). Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. Journal of Psychiatric Research. doi:
Kyaga, S., Lichtenstein, P., Boman, M., Hultman, C., Långström, N. and Landén, M. (2011). Creativity and mental disorder: Family study of 300,000 people with severe mental disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199(5), 373–379.Google Scholar
Liu, A., Werner, K., Roy, S., Trojanowski, J. Q., Morgan-Kane, U., Miller, B. L. and Rankin, K. P. (2009). A case study of an emerging visual artist with frontotemporal lobar degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurocase, 15(3), 235–247.Google Scholar
Lloyd-Evans, R., Batey, M. and Furnham, A. (2006). Bipolar disorder and creativity: Investigating a possible link. Advances in Psychology Research, 40, 111–142.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory (Vol. 9). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Midorikawa, A., Fukutake, T. and Kawamura, M. (2008). Dementia and painting in patients from different cultural backgrounds. European Neurology, 60(5), 224–229.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L. and Hou, C. E. (2004). Portraits of artists: Emergence of visual creativity in dementia. Archives of Neurology, 61(6), 842.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L., Boone, K., Cummings, J. L., Read, S. L. and Mishkin, F. (2000). Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176(5), 458–463.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L., Cummings, J., Mishkin, F., Boone, K., Prince, F., Ponton, M. and Cotman, C. (1998). Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 51(4), 978–982.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (1998). How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution. In Crawford, C. and Krebs, D. (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 87–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual selection shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.
Miller, G. F. and Tal, I. R. (2007). Schizotypy versus openness and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1), 317–324.Google Scholar
Murray, G. and Johnson, S. L. (2010). The clinical significance of creativity in bipolar disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 721–732.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61(6), 622.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. and Clegg, H. (2006). Schizotypy, creativity and mating success in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1586), 611–615.Google Scholar
Palmiero, M., Di Giacomo, D. and Passafiume, D. (2012). Creativity and dementia: A review. Cognitive Processing, 13(3), 193–209.Google Scholar
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W. and Carson, S. (2002). Openness and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1137–1147.Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A. and Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 83–96.Google Scholar
Rankin, K. P., Liu, A. A., Howard, S., Slama, H., Hou, C. E., Shuster, K. and Miller, B. L. (2007). A case-controlled study of altered visual art production in Alzheimer’s and FTLD. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology: Official Journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology, 20(1), 48.Google Scholar
Rawlings, D. and Locarnini, A. (2008). Dimensional schizotypy, autism, and unusual word associations in artists and scientists. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(2), 465–471.Google Scholar
Richards, R. and Kinney, D. K. (1990). Mood swings and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 3(3), 202–217.Google Scholar
Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Benet, M. and Merzel, A. P. (1988). Assessing everyday creativity: Characteristics of the Lifetime Creativity Scales and validation with three large samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 476–485.Google Scholar
Rodrigue, A. L. and Perkins, D. R. (2012). Divergent thinking abilities across the schizophrenic spectrum and other psychological correlates. Creativity Research Journal, 24(2–3), 163–168.Google Scholar
Romo, M. and Alfonso, V. (2003). Implicit theories of Spanish painters. Creativity Research Journal, 15(4), 409–415.Google Scholar
Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity and madness: New findings and old stereotypes. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Rothenberg, A. (1995). Creativity and mental illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(5), 815–816.Google Scholar
Sass, L. A. and Schuldberg, D. (2001). Introduction to the special issue: Creativity and the schizophrenia spectrum. Creativity Research Journal, 13(1), 1–4.Google Scholar
Schermer, J. A., Johnson, A. M., Vernon, P. A. and Jang, K. L. (2011). The relationship between personality and self-report abilities: A behaviour-genetic analysis. Journal of Individual Differences, 32(1), 47–53.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, J. (2003). Issues in creativity and madness. Part three: Who cares? Ethical Human Sciences and Services, 5(2), 149–152.Google Scholar
Schott, G. (2012). Pictures as a neurological tool: Lessons from enhanced and emergent artistry in brain disease. Brain, 135(6), 1947–1963.Google Scholar
Schuldberg, D. (2001). Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13(1), 5–16.Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, S., Adler, N., Aharon-Peretz, J., Perry, D. and Mayseless, N. (2011). The origins of originality: The neural bases of creative thinking and originality. Neuropsychologia, 49(2), 178–185.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. and Kimbrel, N. A. (2010). A dimensional analysis of creativity and mental illness: Do anxiety and depression symptoms predict creative cognition, creative accomplishments, and creative self-concepts? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 2–10.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2012). Creative genius as a personality phenomenon: Definitions, methods, findings, and issues. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(9), 691–706.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2013). What is a creative idea? Little-c versus Big-c creativity. In K. Thomas and J. Chan (Eds.), Handbook of research on creativity (pp. 69–83). Cheltenham, UK, and Northampton, USA: Edward Elgar Publishing.Google Scholar
Soeiro-de-Souza, M. G., Dias, V. V., Bio, D. S., Post, R. M. and Moreno, R. A. (2011). Creativity and executive function across manic, mixed and depressive episodes in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 135(1), 292–297.Google Scholar
Spaniol, S. (2001). Art and mental illness: Where is the link? The Arts in Psychotherapy, 28(4), 221–231.Google Scholar
Staltaro, S. O. (2003). Contemporary American poets, poetry writing, and depression. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Alliant International University at Fresno.
Stirman, S. W. and Pennebaker, J. W. (2001). Word use in the poetry of suicidal and nonsuicidal poets. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 517–522.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C. and Pretz, J. E. (2003). A propulsion model of creative leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 14(4), 455–473.Google Scholar
Strong, C. M., Nowakowska, C., Santosa, C. M., Wang, P. W., Kraemer, H. C. and Ketter, T. A. (2007). Temperament–creativity relationships in mood disorder patients, healthy controls and highly creative individuals. Journal of Affective Disorders, 100(1), 41–48.Google Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R. and Kawashima, R. (2011). Failing to deactivate: The association between brain activity during a working memory task and creativity. Neuroimage, 55(2), 681–687.Google Scholar
Thomas, K. M. and Duke, M. (2007). Depressed writing: Cognitive distortions in the works of depressed and nondepressed poets and writers. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1(4), 204.Google Scholar
Vellante, M., Zucca, G., Preti, A., Sisti, D., Rocchi, M. B. L., Akiskal, K. K. and Akiskal, H. S. (2011). Creativity and affective temperaments in non-clinical professional artists: An empirical psychometric investigation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 135(1), 28–36.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Joormann, J. and Khan, R. (2005). Why we sing the blues: The relation between self-reflective rumination, mood, and creativity. Emotion, 5(2), 226–232.Google Scholar
Widiger, T. A. and Lowe, J. R. (2007). Five-factor model assessment of personality disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 89(1), 16–29.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×