Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T15:36:17.200Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - The challenge of trying to make a difference using media messages

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2009

Sharon Dunwoody
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Susanne C. Moser
Affiliation:
National Center for Atmospheric Research, Boulder
Lisa Dilling
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Get access

Summary

When social problems erupt, one classic response of governments and organizations is to wade in with an information campaign. From automobile seat belts to AIDS to recycling, policy-makers wage war on our inappropriate behaviors with newspaper stories, brochures, and public service announcements.

The goals are often noble ones, the dollars spent gargantuan, and the outcomes all too predictable: Messages seem to change the behaviors of some people some of the time but have almost no discernible impact on most people most of the time (McGuire, 1986; Hornik, 1989).

The situation has so discouraged policy-makers in the past that the pattern was given its own, dismal label: minimal effects (for perhaps the earliest articulation of this name, see Lazarsfeld, Berelson, and Gaudet, 1944). If media messages have no impact, policy-makers opined, why bother?

Today's communication scholars would agree with yesterday's policy-makers that media messages are often poor catalysts for behavior change, but many would disagree with that “minimal effects” label. Mediated messages can have pronounced effects, they would suggest, just not the ones envisioned by those who design them.

As countries around the globe face the prospect of encouraging massive behavior changes in order to try to prevent or, at the least, cope with climate change, it will be tempting to resort to information campaigns. I would urge us to succumb to that temptation; after all, media campaigns offer dramatic economies of scale by reaching large audiences at relatively low cost with potentially useful information.

Type
Chapter
Information
Creating a Climate for Change
Communicating Climate Change and Facilitating Social Change
, pp. 89 - 104
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arctic Climate Impacts Assessment (ACIA) (2004). Impact of a Warming Arctic: Arctic Climate Impact Assessment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bostrom, A., Fischhoff, B., and Morgan, M. G. (1992). Characterizing mental models of hazardous processes: A methodology and an application to radon. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 4, 85–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., et al. (1994). What do people know about global climate change?: 1. Mental models. Risk Analysis, 14, 6, 959–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaffee, S. H., Zhao, X., and Leshner, G. (1994). Political knowledge and the campaign media of 1992. Communication Research, 21, 3, 305–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. (1993). Influence: Science and Practice, 3rd edn. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Corbett, J. B. and Durfee, J. L. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26, 2, 128–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crane, V., Nicholson, H., Chen, M., et al. (1994). Informal Science Learning: What the Research Says About Television, Science Museums, and Community-Based Projects. Ephrata, PA: Science Press.Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H. and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Falk, J. H. and Dierking, L. D. (2000). Learning from Museums: Visitor Experience and the Making of Meaning. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, S. M., Dunwoody, S., and Rogers, C. L. (1999). Communicating Uncertainty: Media Coverage of New and Controversial Science. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., et al. (2002). Growing up with television: Cultivation processes. In Media Effects, 2nd edn., eds. Bryant, J. and Zillmann, D.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 43–67.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Czerlinski, J., and Martignon, L. (2002). How good are fast and frugal heuristics? In Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, eds. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 559–81.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. and Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilovich, T. (1991). How We Know What Isn't So. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D. (eds.) (2002). Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., Dybro, T., et al. (2000). The relationship of communication to risk perceptions and preventive behavior related to lead in tap water. Health Communication, 12, 1, 81–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, R. J., Neuwirth, K., Dunwoody, S., et al. (2004). Information sufficiency and risk communication. Media Psychology, 6, 23–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornik, R. (1989). The knowledge–behavior gap in public information campaigns: A development communication view. In Information Campaigns: Balancing Social Values and Social Change, ed. Sandman, C. T.. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 113–38.Google Scholar
Kahlor, L., Dunwoody, S., and Griffin, R. J. (2004). Predicting knowledge complexity in the wake of an environmental risk. Science Communication, 26, 1, 5–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment, eds. Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 49–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., and Gaudet, H. (1944). The People's Choice: How the Voter Makes Up His Mind in a Presidential Campaign. New York: Duell, Sloan and Pearce.Google Scholar
Liu, J. T. and Smith, V. K. (1990). Risk communication and attitude change: Taiwan's national debate over nuclear power. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 3, 332–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAlister, A., Ramirez, A., Galavotti, C., et al. (1981). Antismoking campaigns: Progress in the application of social learning theory. In Public Communication Campaigns, 2nd edn., eds. Rice, R. E. and Atkin, C. K.. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 291–307.Google Scholar
McCombs, M. E. and Bell, T. (1996). The agenda-setting role of mass communication. In An Integrated Approach to Communication Theory and Research, eds. Salwen, M. and Stacks, D.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 93–110.Google Scholar
McGuire, W. J. (1986). The myth of massive media impact: Savagings and salvagings. Public Communication and Behavior, 1, 173–257.Google Scholar
McLeod, J. M. and Becker, L. B. (1981). The uses and gratifications approach. In Handbook of Political Communication, eds. Nimmo, D. D. and Sanders, K. R.. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 67–97.Google Scholar
Miller, J. D. (1998). The measurement of civic scientific literacy. Public Understanding of Science, 7, 203–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., et al. (2001). Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal Influence: How Perceptions of Mass Collectives Affect Political Attitudes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R., Bordiga, E., Crandall, R., and Reed, H. (1982). Popular induction: Information is not necessarily informative. In Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, eds. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 102–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. and Ross, L. (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Peterson, A. V., Kealey, K. A., Mann, S. L., et al. (2000). Hutchinson Smoking Prevention Project: long-term randomized trial in school-based tobacco use prevention – results on smoking. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 92, 24, 1979–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, R. E., Priester, J. R., and Brinol, P. (2002). Mass media attitude change: implications of the Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. In Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research, eds. Bryant, J. and Zillmann, D.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 155–98.Google Scholar
Phillips, D. P., Kanter, E. J., Bednarczyk, B., et al. (1991). Importance of the lay press in the transmission of medical knowledge to the scientific community. The New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 1180–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, V. (1992). Public Opinion. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J. P. and Levy, M. R. (1986). The Main Source: Learning from Television News. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. L. (1999). The importance of understanding audiences. In Communicating Uncertainty, eds. Friedman, S. M., Dunwoody, S., and Rogers, C. L.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 179–200.Google Scholar
Sears, D. O. and Chaffee, S. H. (1979). Uses and effects of the 1976 debates: An overview of empirical studies. In The Great Debates: Carter vs. Ford, 1976, ed. Kraus, D.. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, pp. 223–61.Google Scholar
Shanahan, J., Morgan, M., and Stenbjerre, M. (1997). Green or brown? Television's cultivation of environmental concern. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 41, 305–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, J. and McComas, K. (1999). Nature Stories: Depictions of the Environment and Their Effects. Kresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
The Biodiversity Project (1998). Engaging the Public on Biodiversity: A Road Map for Education and Communication Strategies. Madison, WI: The Biodiversity Project. Available at www.biodiversityproject.org/roadmap.htm.
The Biodiversity Project (2002). Ethics for a Small Planet: A Communications Handbook on the Ethical and Theological Reasons for Protecting Biodiversity. Madison, WI: Biodiversity Project.
Tichenor, P. J., Donohue, G. A., and Olien, C. N. (1980). Community Conflict & the Press.Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
Trumbo, C. (1995). Longitudinal modeling of public issues: An application of the agenda-setting process to the issue of global warming. Journalism & Mass Communication Monographs, No. 152.Google Scholar
Trumbo, C. W., Dunwoody, S., and Griffin, R. J. (1998). Journalists, cognition, and the presentation of an epidemiologic study. Science Communication, 19, 3, 238–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T. R. and Cook, F. L. (1984). The mass media and judgments of risk: Distinguishing impact on personal and societal level judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 693–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, D. and Brosius, H.-B. (2000). Exemplification in Communication: The Influence of Case Reports on the Perception of Issues. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×