Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:17:02.734Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preface

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2009

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Professor of Psychology and Education, Yale; Director of the PACE Center, Yale
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University in Bloomington, Illinois
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

COGNITION AND INTELLIGENCE

How did the study of cognition and intelligence get started? Although some psychologists in the nineteenth century were interested in cognitive processing (e.g., Donders, 1868/1869), the connection between information processing and intelligence seems first to have been explicitly drawn by Charles Spearman (1923), the same individual known for initiating serious psychometric theorizing about intelligence with his theory of the general factor of intelligence (Spearman, 1927).

Spearman (1923) proposed what he believed to be three fundamental qualitative principles of cognition. The first, apprehension of experience, is what today might be called the encoding of stimuli (see Sternberg, 1977). It involves perceiving the stimuli and their properties. The second principle, eduction of relations, is what today might be labeled inference. It is the inferring of a relation between two or more concepts. The third principle, eduction of correlates, is what today might be called application. It is the application of an inferred rule to a new situation.

Spearman was not the only early psychologist interested in the relationship between cognition and intelligence. Thorndike et al. (1926) proposed a quite similar theory based on Thorndike's theory of learning. According to this theory, learned connections are what underlie individual differences in intelligence. Some early researchers tried to integrate cognition and biology in studying intelligence. For example, the Russian psychologist Alexander Luria (1973, 1980) believed that the brain is a highly differentiated system whose parts are responsible for different aspects of a unified whole.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Intelligence
Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind
, pp. vii - xii
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brown, A. L., & French, A. L. (1979). The zone of potential development: Implications for intelligence testing in the year 2000. In R. J. Sternberg & D. K. Detterman (Eds.), Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement (pp. 217–235). Norwood, NJ: AblexCrossRef
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Ability and task difficulty in cognitive psychology. Educational Researcher, 10, 11–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1868/1869). Over de snelheid van psychische processen. Onderzoekingen gedaan in het Physiologisch Laboratorium der Utrechtsche Hoogeschool. Tweede reeks, II, 92–120Google Scholar
Feuerstein, R. (1980). Instrumental enrichment: An intervention program for cognitive modifiability. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press
Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Dynamic testing. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 75–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological Review, 85, 109–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1980). Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449–474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, E., Frost, N., & Lunneborg, C. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 7, pp. 87–122). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Hunt, E. B., Lunneborg, C., & Lewis, J. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 194–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1979). g: Outmoded theory of unconquered frontier? Creative Science and Technology, 2, 16–29Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence. Heidelberg: Springer-VerlagCrossRef
Keating, D. P., & Bobbit, B. (1978). Individual and developmental differences in cognitive processing components of mental ability. Child Development, 49, 155–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books
Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed., revised & expanded). New York: Basic Books
Naglieri, J. A., & Das, J. P. (1990). Planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive cognitive processes as a model for intelligence. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 8, 303–337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naglieri, J. A., & Das, J. P. (1997). Cognitive assessment system. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1979). Cognitive correlates and components in the analysis of individual differences. In R. J. Sternberg & D. K. Detterman (Eds.), Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement (pp. 61–88). Norwood, NJ: AblexCrossRef
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1980). Components of inductive reasoning. In R. E. Snow, P. A. Federico, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Aptitude, learning, and instruction: Cognitive process analyses of aptitude (Vol. 1, pp. 177–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1982). Analyzing aptitudes for learning: Inductive reasoning. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace
Piaget, J. (1928). Judgment and reasoning in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press
Piaget, J. (1972). The psychology of intelligence. Totowa, NJ: Littlefield Adams
Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392–409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royer, F. L. (1971). Information processing of visual figures in the digit symbol substitution task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 335–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of ‘intelligence’ and the principles of cognition (2nd ed.). London: Macmillan. (1923 edition reprinted in 1973 by Arno Press, New York.)
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Sternberg, R. J. (1980). Factor theories of intelligence are all right almost. Educational Researcher, 9, 6–13, 18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1982). Natural, unnatural, and supernatural concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 451–488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Components of human intelligence. Cognition, 15, 1–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (1994). Encyclopedia of human intelligence. New York: Macmillan
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2000). Handbook of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J., & Gardner, M. K. (1983). Unities in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 80–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 4, 421–457Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L., Bregman, E. D., Cobb, M. V., & Woodyard, E. I. (1926). The measurement of intelligence. New York: Teachers College
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Preface
    • By Robert J. Sternberg, Professor of Psychology and Education, Yale; Director of the PACE Center, Yale, Jean E. Pretz, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University in Bloomington, Illinois
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Yale University, Connecticut, Jean E. Pretz, Yale University, Connecticut
  • Book: Cognition and Intelligence
  • Online publication: 23 November 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511607073.001
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Preface
    • By Robert J. Sternberg, Professor of Psychology and Education, Yale; Director of the PACE Center, Yale, Jean E. Pretz, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University in Bloomington, Illinois
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Yale University, Connecticut, Jean E. Pretz, Yale University, Connecticut
  • Book: Cognition and Intelligence
  • Online publication: 23 November 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511607073.001
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Preface
    • By Robert J. Sternberg, Professor of Psychology and Education, Yale; Director of the PACE Center, Yale, Jean E. Pretz, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University in Bloomington, Illinois
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Yale University, Connecticut, Jean E. Pretz, Yale University, Connecticut
  • Book: Cognition and Intelligence
  • Online publication: 23 November 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511607073.001
Available formats
×