Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T17:21:51.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Mental Chronometry and the Unification of Differential Psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2009

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Mental chronometry is the measurement of cognitive speed. It is the actual time taken to process information of different types and degrees of complexity. The basic measurements are an individual's response time (RT) to a visual or auditory stimulus that calls for a particular response, choice, or decision.

Since at least the time of Sir Francis Galton (1822–1911), the father of differential psychology, it has been hypothesized by him and many others that mental speed is a major aspect of general intelligence. What we now know for sure is that RT can be a highly precise, reliable, and sensitive measure of individual differences. Its relationship to other psychological and ecological variables, however, is a complex affair just recently being explored.

Research on RT has a venerable history. Not only was it the earliest measurement technique used in empirical psychology, but also its scientific use as a measure of individual differences preceded the beginning of experimental psychology by at least half a century. The first published research on RT appeared in astronomy journals. Time, as measured by the earth's rotation with reference to a star's moment of transit across a hairline in the lens of a telescope, had to be measured as accurately as possible. In 1796 it was accidentally discovered by the Astronomer Royal at the Greenwich Observatory that astronomers showed individual differences in RT to the star's transit across the hairline.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Intelligence
Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind
, pp. 26 - 50
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press
Baumeister, A. A. (1998). Intelligence and the “personal equation.”Intelligence, 26, 255–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinley, J. F. (1965). Cognitive sets, speed and accuracy in the elderly. In A. T. Welford & J. E. Birren (Eds.), Behavior, aging, and the nervous system (pp. 114–149). New York: Springer-Verlag
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Caryl, P. G., Deary, I. J., Jensen, A. R., Neubauer, A. C., & Vickers, D. (1999). Information processing approaches to intelligence: Progress and prospects. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. de Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 181–219). Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg University Press
Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67–83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerella, J., & Hale, S. (1994). The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span. Acta Psychologica, 86, 109–197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, S. J., Carlson, J. S., & Jensen, A. R. (1985). Speed of information processing in academically gifted youths. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 621–629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (2000a). Looking down on human intelligence: From psychometrics to the brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
Deary, I. J. (2000b). Simple information processing and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 267–284). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzong, H., Ahmed, A., Newell, F. N., & Emslie, H. (2000). A neural basis for intelligence. Science, 289, 457–460CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groen, G. J., & Parkman, J. M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychological Review, 79, 329–343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, S. (1990). A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed. Child Development, 61, 653–663CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, S., & Jansen, J. (1994). Global processing-time coefficients characterize individual and group differences in cognitive speed. Psychological Science, 5, 384–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. R. (1942). Tabulation of the probabilities for the ratio of the mean square successive difference to the variance. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 13, 207–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1982a). The chronometry of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 93–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Jensen, A. R. (1982b). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 93–132). New York: Springer
Jensen, A. R. (1985). Methodological and statistical techniques for the chronometric study of mental abilities. In C. R. Reynolds & V. L. Wilson (Eds.), Methodological and statistical advances in the study of individual differences (pp. 51–116). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Jensen, A. R. (1987a). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 101–175). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Jensen, A. R. (1987b). The g beyond factor analysis. In R. R. Ronning, J. A. Glover, J. C. Conoley, & J. C. Witt (Eds.), The influence of cognitive psychology on testing (pp. 87–142). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Jensen, A. R. (1992a). The importance of intraindividual variability in reaction time. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 869–882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1992b). The relation between information processing time and right/wrong responses. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 290–292Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1993). Why is reaction time correlated with psychometric g? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 9–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1994). Reaction time. In R. J. Corsini (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology, 2nd ed. (Vol. 3, pp. 282–285). New York: Wiley
Jensen, A. R. (1998a). The g factor. Westport, CT: Praeger
Jensen, A. R. (1998b). The suppressed relationship between IQ and the reaction time slope parameter of the Hick function. Intelligence, 26, 43–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R., Larson, G. E., & Paul, S. M. (1988). Psychometric g and mental processing speed on a semantic verification test. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 243–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1991a). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1991b). Development of processing speed in childhood and adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 23, 151–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kranzler, J. H. (1992). A test of Larson and Alderton's (1990) worst performance rule of reaction time variability. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 255–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C. (1993). Aptitude testing inspired by information processing: A test of the four-sources model. Journal of General Psychology, 120, 375–405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, G. E., & Alderton, D. L. (1990). Reaction time variability and intelligence: A “worst performance” analysis of individual differences. Intelligence, 14, 309–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Myerson, J., Zheng, Y., Hale, S, Jenkins, L., & Widaman, K. (2003). Difference engines: Mathematical models of diversity in speeded cognitive performance. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 262–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, A. C. (1997). The mental speed approach to the assessment of intelligence. In J. S. Carlson, J. Kingma, & W. Tomic (Eds.), Advances in cognition and educational practice: Reflections on the concept of intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 149–173). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press Inc.
O'Hara, R., Sommer, B., & Morgan, K. (2001). Reaction time but not performance on cognitive tasks identifies individuals at risk for Alzheimer's disease: A preliminary report. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Paul, S. M. (1984). Speed of information processing: The Semantic Verification Test and general mental ability. Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley
Rabbitt, P. (1996). Do individual differences in speed reflect “global” or “local” differences in mental abilities? Intelligence, 22, 69–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1998). Relation of successive percentiles of reaction time distributions to cognitive variables and adult age. Intelligence, 26, 153–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., Erp, T., Poutanen, V.-P., Huttunen, M., Longqvist, J., Standertkjold-Nordenstam, C.-G., Kaprio, J., Khaledy, M., Dail, R., Zoumalan, C. I., & Toga, A. W. (2001). Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature: Neuroscience, 4(No. 12), 1253–1258Google ScholarPubMed
Vernon, P. A. (Ed.) (1987). Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 101–175). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Neumann, J. (1941). Distribution of the ratio of the mean square successive difference to the variance. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 14, 378–388Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×