Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-v5vhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T21:29:09.191Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Animal models and cancer-related symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2009

Christina A. Meyers
Affiliation:
University of Texas, M. D. Anderson Cancer Center
James R. Perry
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Animal models have frequently been useful in developing treatments for a variety of diseases. Their use permits researchers to ask questions about mechanisms that would be difficult or unethical in humans, and also permits the testing of potential treatments. Models that work in rodents are usually preferred because we know much about their physiology and behavior, and because rodents are relatively inexpensive. Moreover, a host of experimental manipulations have been developed for use in these species. Work with primates that may be more valid is substantially more expensive, and the numbers of subjects that can be used are normally very limited. The development of animal models for behavioral symptoms presents a special challenge, because although many such models and tests exist, they address poorly the symptoms that are of most concern to cancer patients. This chapter will provide a selective overview of animal models and tests, and provide examples of what has been achieved in other areas. The limitations of the use of the non-human animal models and tests will also be addressed.

It is important at the outset to note the distinction between an animal model and an animal test. A model is a procedure used to induce a state in the animal that resembles the disease under study. In this context, a test is a procedure that reveals symptoms that resemble aspects of the human disease. For example, in the case of depression, most animal models have relied on chronic stress paradigms.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Cancer , pp. 270 - 278
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alkon, DL, Amaral, DG, Bear, MFet al. (1991). Learning and memory. Brain Res Rev 16: 193–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anisman, H, Zacharko, RM (1982). Depression: the predisposing influence of stress. Behav Brain Sci 5: 89–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayada, K, Tadano, T, Endo, Y (2002). Gnawing behavior of a mouse in a narrow cylinder: a simple system for the study of muscle activity, fatigue, and stress. Physiol Behav 77: 161–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, RJ, Yudko, E, Dulloog, L, Blanchard, DC (2001). Defense changes in stress nonresponsive subordinate males in a visible burrow system. Physiol Behav 72: 635–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, DC, Griebel, G, Blanchard, RJ (2003). The Mouse Defense Test Battery: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Eur J Pharmacol 463: 97–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsini, F, Meli, A (1988). Is the forced swimming test a suitable model for revealing antidepressant activity? Psychopharmacology 94: 147–160.CrossRef
Carroll, BJ (1978). Neuroendocrine function in psychiatric disorders. In Psychopharmacology: A Generation of Progress (pp. 487–497). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Chance, W, Meyenfeldt, M, Fischer, J (1983). Changes in brain amines associated with cancer anorexia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 7: 471–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chance, WT, Cao, L, Nelson, JL, Foley-Nelson, T, Fischer, JE (1988). Reversal of neurochemical aberrations after tumor resection in rats. Am J Surg 155: 124–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, CC, DeLaHunt, M, Hu, S, Close, K, Peterson, PK (1992). Immunologically mediated fatigue: a murine model. Clin Immunol Immunopathol 64: 161–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chermat, R, Thierry, B, Steru, L, Simon, P (1986). Adaptation of the tail suspension test to the rat. J Pharmacol 17: 348–350.Google ScholarPubMed
Chuluyan, HC, Wolcott, RM, Chervenak, R, Dunn, AJ (2000). Catecholamine, indoleamine and corticosteroid responses in mice bearing tumors. Neuroimmunomodulation 8: 107–113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawley, JN (2000). What's Wrong with My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice (pp. 1–368). Wilmington, DE: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
Cryan, JF, Mombereau, C (2004). In search of a depressed mouse: utility of models for studying depression-related behavior in genetically modified mice. Mol Psychiatry 9: 1050–1062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cryan, JF, Markou, A, Lucki, I (2002). Assessing antidepressant activity in rodents: recent developments and future needs. Trends Pharmacol Sci 23: 238–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalakas, MC, Mock, V, Hawkins, MJ (1998). Fatigue: definitions, mechanisms, and paradigms for study. Semin Oncol 25: S48–S53.Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, JM, Weaver, JA, Kohut, ML, Colbert, LH, Ghaffar, A, Mayer, EP (1997). Immune system activation and fatigue during treadmill running: role of interferon. Med Sci Sports Exerc 30: 863–868.Google Scholar
Dunn, AJ, Berridge, CW (1990). Physiological and behavioral responses to corticotropin-releasing factor administration: is CRF a mediator of anxiety or stress responses?Brain Res Rev 15: 71–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, AJ, Swiergiel, AH (2004). Interferon effects on behavior, corticosterone, catecholamines and indoleamines in mice and rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 29[Suppl. 1]: S120.Google Scholar
Dunn, AJ, Swiergiel, AH (2005). Effects of interleukin-1 and endotoxin in the forced swim and tail suspension tests in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 81: 688–693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, AJ, Swiergiel, AH, Cork, R, Newman, RA (2004). Behavior, neurochemical and endocrine responses to leukemia in mice. In 2004 Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner (Vol. 462.13). Washington DC: Society for Neuroscience.Google Scholar
Dunn, AL, Crnic, LS (1993). Repeated injections of interferon-α A/D in Balb/c mice: behavioral effects. Brain Behav Immun 7: 104–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geller, A, Robustelli, F, Jarvik, ME (1970). Incubation and the Kamin effect. J Exp Psychol 85: 61–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaus, A, Crow, R, Hammond, S (1996). A qualitative study to explore the concept of fatigue/tiredness in cancer patients and in healthy individuals. Support Care Cancer 4: 82–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haug, M, Whalen, RE (1999). Animal Models of Human Emotion and Cognition (p. 341). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, RJ (1981). Animal models and human depressive disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 5: 231–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaur, G, Kulkarni, SK (2000). Comparative study of antidepressants and herbal psychotropic drugs in a mouse model of chronic fatigue. J Chronic Fatigue Syndr 6: 23–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, JP, Wrynn, AS, Leonard, BE (1997). The olfactory bulbectomized rat as a model of depression: an update. Pharmacol Ther 74: 299–316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kent, S, Bluthé, R-M, Kelley, KW, Dantzer, R (1992). Sickness behavior as a new target for drug development. Trends Pharmacol Sci 13: 24–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, SF, Seligman, MEP (1976). Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol 1: 3–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, K, Christmas, D, Swan, J, Sorrell, E (2005). Animal models of depression: navigating through the clinical fog. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 503–513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKinney, WT, Bunney, WE (1969). Animal models of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 21: 240–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, R (1984). Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 11: 47–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olton, DS (1987). The radial arm maze as a tool in behavioral pharmacology. Physiol Behav 40: 793–797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ottenweller, JE, Natelson, BH, Gause, WCet al. (1998). Mouse running activity is lowered by Brucella abortus treatment: a potential model to study chronic fatigue. Physiol Behav 63: 795–801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overmier, JB, Seligman, MEP (1967). Effects of inescapable shock on subsequent escape and avoidance responding. J Comp Physiol Psychol 63: 28–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD (2000). Animal models of depression: utility for transgenic research. Rev Neurosci 11: 53–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Bertin, A, Jalfre, M (1977a). Behavioural despair in mice: a primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacodyn 229: 327–336.Google ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Le Pichon, M, Jalfre, M (1977b). Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Nature 266: 730–732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Anton, G, Blavet, N, Jalfre, M (1978). Behavioural despair in rats: a new model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Eur J Pharmacol 47: 379–391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachar, EJ (1967). Corticosteroids in depressive illness: II. A longitudinal psychoendocrine study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 17: 554–567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandman, CA, Beckwith, BE, Gittis, MM (1974). Melanocyte-stimulating hormone (MSH) and overtraining effects on extradimensional shift (EDS) learning. Physiol Behav 13: 1631–1666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandman, CA, George, JM, Nolan, JD, Riezen, H, Kastin, AJ (1975). Enhancement of attention in man with ACTH/MSH4–10. Physiol Behav 15: 427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schimanski, , Nguyen, PV (2004). Multidisciplinary approaches for investigating the mechanisms of hippocampus-dependent memory: a focus on inbred mouse strains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28: 463–483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, BA, Klopper, JH, Yager, RD, Gordon, JW (1976). Paradoxical response to amphetamine in developing rats treated with 6-hydroxydopamine. Nature 261: 153–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, BA, Fletcher, JM, Shaywitz, SE (2001). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Treat Options Neurol 3: 229–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, C, Leonard, BE (2005). The olfactory bulbectomized rat as a model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 627–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takagi, T, Saito, H, Lee, C-H, Hayashi, T (1972). Pharmacological studies on fatigue I. Jpn J Pharmacol 22: 17–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Praag, HM (2004). Can stress cause depression?Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 28(5): 891–907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vegas, O, Beitia, G, Sanchez-Martin, JR, Arregi, A, Azpiroz, A (2004). Behavioral and neurochemical responses in mice bearing tumors submitted to social stress. Behav Brain Res 155: 125–134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, JM, Kilts, CD (1998). Animal models of depression and schizophrenia. In Schatzberg, AF, Nemeroff, CB (eds.) The American Psychiatric Press Textbook of Psychopharmacology (2nd edn.) (pp. 89–131). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Whishaw, IQ, Kolb, B (2005). The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat. A Handbook with Tests. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Willner, P (1984). The validity of animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 83: 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willner, P (1997). Validity, reliability and utility of the chronic mild stress model of depression: a 10-year review and evaluation. Psychopharmacology 134: 319–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willner, P (2005). Chronic mild stress (CMS) revisited: consistency and behavioural-neurobiological concordance in the effects of CMS. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 52: 90–110.Google ScholarPubMed
Wong, ML, Kling, MA, Munson, PJet al. (2000). Pronounced and sustained central hypernoradrenergic function in major depression with melancholic features: relation to hypercortisolism and corticotropin-releasing hormone. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97: 325–330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×