Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nmvwc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T23:51:05.159Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Patterns of maternal behavior among infants at risk for abuse: relations with infant attachment behavior and infant development at 12 months of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2010

Get access

Summary

Although the organization and development of mother-infant affective exchange during the first year has received a great deal of attention from developmental researchers, the majority of the work to date has focused on the development of methodologies for describing the construct of sensitive, responsive mothering (Bakeman and Brown, 1977; Belsky, Taylor, and Rovine; 1984; Kaye and Fogel, 1980; Tronick, Als, and Brazelton, 1980). However, the emphasis on assessment of the components of sensitivity has not been matched by an equally careful description of the behaviors characterizing insensitive patterns of mothering. The implicit assumption underlying most previous work has been that maternal insensitivity is adequately defined simply as the absence or opposite of maternal sensitivity. Thus, maternal sensitivity and insensitivity are regarded as quantitatively related along a single theoretical dimension. Support for this conceptualization comes not only from our natural language system regarding the usage of terms like sensitivity but also from factor-analytic studies of mother-infant interaction. These studies have shown that a strong first factor, accounting for 50 percent of the variance or more, reliably emerges and is defined by behaviors such as verbal interaction, maternal responsiveness, maternal positive affect and maternal stimulation (Belsky, Rovine and Taylor, 1984; Clarke-Stewart, 1973). This primary maternal factor is in turn related to positive infant affect and higher developmental scores (e.g., Clarke-Stewart, 1973). The often replicated relationship between maternal verbal responsiveness and high infant mental developmental scores has probably acted to fix attention even more strongly on this general “good mother” construct (e.g., Carew, 1980; Elardo, Bradley, and Caldwell, 1977).

Type
Chapter
Information
Child Maltreatment
Theory and Research on the Causes and Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect
, pp. 464 - 493
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×