Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T18:39:42.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Psychology

from PART II - THE DISCIPLINES IN WESTERN EUROPE AND NORTH AMERICA SINCE ABOUT 1880

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2008

Theodore M. Porter
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Dorothy Ross
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University
Get access

Summary

Psychology occupies a peculiar place among the sciences, suspended between methodological orientations derived from the physical and biological sciences and a subject matter that extends into the social and human sciences. The struggle to create a science of both subjectivity and behavior, and the related effort to develop professional practices utilizing that science’s results, provide interesting examples of both the reach and the limits of such scientific ideals as objectivity, measurability, repeatability, and cumulative knowledge acquisition. In addition, psychologists’ struggles to live by such ideals while competing with others to fulfill multiple public demands for their services illuminate both the formative impact of science on modern life, and the effects of technocratic hopes on science.

The aim of this chapter is to sketch the results of a broad shift in the historiography of psychology over the past twenty years, from the achievements of important figures and the history of psychological systems and theories to the social and cultural relations of psychological thought and practice. In the process, I hope to bring out the interrelationships of psychological research and societal practices both with one another and with prevailing cultural values and institutions in different times and places, while at the same time attempting to bring out certain common threads in this varied narrative.

One of those common threads is that the history of psychology has been a continuous struggle by multiple participants to occupy and define a sharply contested, but never clearly bounded, discursive and practical field. The emergence and institutionalization of both the discipline and the profession called “psychology” are often portrayed as acts of liberation from philosophy or medicine, but these efforts to establish scientific and professional autonomy have never completely succeeded.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ash, Mitchell G.Cultural Contexts and Scientific Change in Psychology: Kurt Lewin in Iowa,” American Psychologist, 47 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ash, Mitchell G.Emigré Psychologists after 1933: The Cultural Coding of Scientific and Professional Practices,” in Forced Migration and Scientific Change: Emigré German-Speaking Scientists and Scholars after 1933, ed. Ash, Mitchell G. and Söllner, Alfons (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ash, Mitchell G.Kurt Gottschldt and Psychological Research in Nazi and Socialist Germany,” in Science under Socialism: East Germany in Comparative Perspective, ed. Macrakis, Kristie and Hoffmann, Dieter (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1999).Google Scholar
Ash, Mitchell G.Psychology in Twentieth-Century Germany: Science and Profession,” in German Professions, 1800–1950, ed. Cocks, Geoffrey and Jarausch, Konrad H. (New York: Oxford University Press, 1990).Google Scholar
Ash, Mitchell G.Gestalt Psychology in German Culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the Quest for Objectivity (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Bazerman, CharlesCodifying the Social Scientific Style: The A.P.A. ‘Publication Manual’ as a Behaviorist Rhetoric,” in The Rhetoric of the Human Sciences: Language and Argument in Scholarship and Public Affairs, ed. Nelson, John S., McCloskey, Donald, and Megill, Allen (Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Benetka, GerhardPsychologie in Wien: Sozial- und Theoriegeschichte des Wiener Psychologischen Instituts, 1922–1938 (Vienna: Wiener Universitätsverlag, 1995).Google Scholar
Benjamin, Ludy T. Jr., A History of Psychology: Original Sources and Contemporary Research (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1988)Google Scholar
Binet, Alfred and Henri, Victor, “La Psychologie individuelle,” L’Année Psychologique, 2 (1895).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloor, DavidWhatever Happened to ‘Social Constructiveness’?,” in Bartlett, Culture and Cognition, ed. Saito, Akiko (London: Psychology Press, 2000).Google Scholar
Brentano, FranzPsychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt (Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot, 1874)Google Scholar
Bringmann, Wolfgang G. and Ungerer, Gustav, “Experimental versus Educational Psychology: Wilhelm Wundt’s Letters to Ernst Meumann,” Psychological Research, 42 (1980).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, AdrianWas macht den psychologischen Expertenstatus aus?,” Psychologie und Geschichte, 2 (1991).Google Scholar
Brooks, John I. IIIPhilosophy and Psychology at the Sorbonne, 1885–1913,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 29 (1993)3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bühler, KarlDie Krise der Psychologie (Jena: Fischer, 1927).Google Scholar
Burnham, John C.How Superstition Won and Science Lost: Popularizing Science and Health in the United States (New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Busse, StefanGab es eine DDR-Psychologie?,” Psychologie und Geschichte, 5 (1993).Google Scholar
Campbell, JoanJoy in Work, German Work: The National Debate, 1880–1945 (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capshew, James H.Psychologists on the March: Science, Practice and Professional Identity in America, 1929–1969 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999), esp. chaps. 3–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroy, Jacqueline and Plas, Regine, “The Origins of French Experimental Psychology: Experiment and Experimentalism,” History of the Human Sciences, 9:1 (1996).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, JohnArmy Alpha, Army Brass and the Search for Army Intelligence,” Isis, 84 (1993).Google Scholar
Cartwright, DorwinLewinian Theory as a Contemporary Systematic Framework,” in Psychology: A Study of a Science, vol. 4: General Systematic Formulations, ed. Koch, Sigmund (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959).Google Scholar
Chapman, Paul D.Schools as Sorters: Lewis M. Terman, Applied Psychology and the Intelligence Testing Movement, 1890–1930 (New York: New York University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Chimisso, CristinaThe Mind and the Faculties: The Controversy over Primitive Mentality and the Struggle for Disciplinary Space at the Inter-war Sorbonne,” History of the Human Sciences, 13 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coon, Deborah J.Standardizing the Subject: Experimental Psychologists, Introspection, and the Quest for a Technoscientific Ideal,” Technology and Culture, 34 (1993).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coon, Deborah J.Testing the Limits of Sense and Science: American Experimental Psychologists Combat Spiritualism, 1880–1920,” American Psychologist, 47 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cravens, HamiltonBefore Head Start: The Iowa Station and America’s Children (Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press, 1993).Google Scholar
Cronbach, LeeThe Two Disciplines of Scientific Psychology,” American Psychologist, 12 (1957).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, Kurt, Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, Kurt, Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found Its Language (London: Sage, 1997).Google Scholar
Danziger, KurtThe Project of an Experimental Social Psychology: Historical Perspectives,” Science in Context, 5 (1992).Google Scholar
Daston, LorraineObjectivity and the Escape from Perspective,” Social Studies of Science, 22 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daston, LorraineThe Naturalized Female Intellect,” Science in Context, 5 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daston, LorraineThe Theory of Will and the Science of Mind,” in The Problematic Science: Psychology in Nineteenth-Century Thought, ed. Woodward, William R. and Ash, Mitchell G. (New York: Praeger, 1982).Google Scholar
Dehue, TrudyChanging the Rules: Psychology in the Netherlands, 1900–1945 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Eckard, Geroge ed., Völkerpsychologie – Versuch einer Neuentdeckung (Weinheim: Beltz Psychologie-Verl.-Union, 1997).Google Scholar
Elias, NorbertThe Civilizing Process (1939), 2 vols, trans. Jephcott, E. (New York: Urizen, 1978)Google Scholar
Furumoto, Laurel, “The New History of Psychology,” in The G. Stanley Hall Lecture Series, vol. 9, ed. Cohen, Ira S. (Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1989).Google Scholar
Furumoto, LaurelOn the Margins: Women and the Professionalization of Psychology in the United States 1890–1940,” in Psychology in Twentieth-Century Thought and Society, ed. Ash, Mitchell G. and Woodward, William R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Gardner, HowardThe Mind’s New Science (1985) (New York: Basic Books, 1996).Google Scholar
Garvey, Charles R.List of American Psychological Laboratories,” Psychological Bulletin, 26 (1929)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geuter, UlfriedThe Professionalization of Psychology in Nazi Germany (1984), trans. Holmes, Richard (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992).Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, GerdFrom Tools to Theories: Discovery in Cognitive Psychology,” Science in Context, 5 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, Steven J.The Mismeasure of Man (New York: Norton, 1981), chap. 6Google Scholar
Gundlach, HorstFaktor Mensch im Krieg: Der Eintritt der Psychologie und Psychotechnik in den Krieg,” Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, 19 (1996).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gundlach, HorstZur Verwendung physiologischer Analogien bei der Entstehung der experimentellen Psychologie,” Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, 12 (1989).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, AnneMedicine, Mind and the Double Brain (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1987)Google Scholar
Harrington, AnneReenchanted Science: Holism and German Science from Wilhelm II to Hitler (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1996).Google Scholar
Hatfield, Gary, “Psychology as a Natural Science in the Eighteenth Century,” Revue de Synthese, 115 (1994).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, Gary, “Wundt and Psychology as Science: Disciplinary Transformations,” Perspectives on Science, 5 (1997)Google Scholar
Hearnshaw, Leslie S., A Short History of British Psychology, 1840–1940 (London: Methuen, 1964)Google Scholar
Hearnshaw, L. S.Cyril Burt, Psychologist (Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press, 1979)Google Scholar
Heidbreder, EdnaSeven Psychologies (New York: Century, 1933).Google Scholar
Herman, EllenThe Romance of American Psychology (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1996).Google Scholar
Hilgard, Ernest R., Psychology in America: A Historical Survey (New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1987).Google Scholar
Hornstein, Gail A.The Return of the Repressed: Psychology’s Problematic Relations with Psychoanalysis, 1909–1960,” American Psychologist, 47 (1992)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, SiegfriedZur Herausbildung von Praxisfeldern der Psychologie bis 1933,” in Geschichte der deutschen Psychologie im 20. Jahrhundert, ed. Ash, Mitchell G. and Geuter, Ulfried (Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag, 1985)Google Scholar
John Mackintosh, Nicolas ed., Cyril Burt: Fraud or Framed? (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joynson, Robert B.The Burt Affair (London: Routledge, 1989)Google Scholar
Juttemann, Gerd ed., Die Geschichtlichkeit des Seelischen: Der historische Zugang zum Gegenstand der Psychologie (Weinheim: Psychologie Verl. Union, 1986)Google Scholar
Kline, RonaldConstructing ‘Technology’ as ‘Applied Science’: Public Rhetoric of Science and Engineering in the United States, 1880–1945,” Isis, 86 (1995)Google Scholar
Klix, FriedhartInformation und Verhalten (Berlin: Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften, 1966).Google Scholar
Kusch, MartinPsychologism (London: Routledge, 1995).Google Scholar
Lazarus, Moritz and Steinthal, Haim, eds., Zeitschrift für Völkerpsychologie und Sprachwissenschaft, 20 vols. (Berlin: Dümmler, 1860–90).Google Scholar
Leahey, Thomas H., A History of Psychology, 3rd ed. (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, 1992)Google Scholar
Leary, David E.Telling Likely Stories: The Rhetoric of the New Psychology, 1880–1920,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 23 (1987)3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewin, Kurt, Lippitt, Ronald and White, Robert K., “Patterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally Created ‘Social Climates,’Journal of Social Psychology, 10 (1939).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, MarilynWundt, Spiritism, and the Assumptions of Science,” in Wundt Studies, ed. Bringmann, Wolgang and Tweney, Ryan D. (Toronto:C. J. Hogrefe, 1980)Google Scholar
Meehl, Paul E.Clinical versus Statistical Prediction (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1954).Google Scholar
Morawski, Jill G. and Hornstein, Gail A., “Quandary of the Quacks: The Struggle for Expert Knowledge in American Psychology, 1890–1940,” in The Estate of Social Knowledge, ed. , JoAnne Brown and Keuren, David K. (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991)Google Scholar
Morawski, Jill G., ed. The Rise of Experimentation in American Psychology (New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Morawski, Jill G.Self-Regard and Other-Regard: Reflexive Practices in American Psychology, 1890–1940,” Science in Context, 5 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mucchielli, Laurent, “Aux origines de la psychologie universitaire en France (1870–1900): enjeux intellectuels, contexte politique, réseaux et stratégies d’alliance autor de la ‘Revue Philosophique’ de Théodule Ribot,” Annals of Science, 55 (1998).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mucchielli, LaurentAux origines de la nouvelle histoire en France: l’évolution intellectuelle et la formation du champ des sciences sociales (1880–1930),” Revue de synthèse, 1 (1995).Google Scholar
Nitzschke, Bernd ed., Freud und die akademische Psychologie: Beiträge zu einer historischen Kontroverse (Munich: Psychologie-Verlag-Union, 1989)Google Scholar
O’Donnell, John M.The Origins of Behaviorism: American Psychology, 1870–1920 (New York: New York University Press, 1985)Google Scholar
Oppenheim, JanetThe Other World: Spiritualism and Psychical Research in England, 1850–1914 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1985)Google Scholar
Pandora, Katherine A.Rebels within the Ranks: Psychologists’ Critique of Scientific Authority and Democratic Realities in New Deal America (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1997).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parot, Francoise and Richelle, Marc, Introduction a la Psychologie. Histoire et méthodes, 4th ed. (Paris: Presses Universitaires de Paris, 1998).Google Scholar
Peters, R. S.Brett’s History of Psychology, ed. and abr. (London: Allen and Unwin, 1962).Google Scholar
Porter, Roy ed., Rewriting the Self: Histories from the Renaissance to the Present (London: Routledge, 1997).Google Scholar
Rabinbach, AnsonThe Human Motor: Energy, Fatigue and the Origins of Modernity (New York: Basic Books, 1990).Google Scholar
Richards, Graham “‘To Know Our Fellow Men to Do Them Good’: American Psychology’s Enduring Moral Project,” History of the Human Sciences, 8:3 (1995).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, GrahamBritain on the Couch: The Popularization of Psychoanalysis in Britain 1918–1940,” Science in Context, 13 (2000).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, Robert J.Darwin and the Emergence of Evolutionary Concepts of Mind and Behavior (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Rose, Nikolas, The Psychological Complex: Psychology, Politics and Society in England, 1839–1939 (London: Routledge, 1985).Google Scholar
Rose, NikolasGoverning the Soul (London: Routledge, 1990)Google Scholar
Rose, NikolasInventing Our Selves: Psychology, Power and Personhood (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, DorothyG. Stanley Hall: The Psychologist as Prophet (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1972)Google Scholar
Rossiter, MargaretWhich Science? Which Women?,” Osiris, 2nd ser., 12 (1997).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samelson, FranzAuthoritarianism from Berlin to Berkeley: On Social Psychology and History,” Journal of Social Issues, 42 (1986).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samelson, FranzOrganizing for the Kingdom of Behavior: Academic Battles and Organizational Policies in the Twenties,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 21 (1985)3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, William H.After Binet: French Intelligence Testing, 1900–1950,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 28 (1992).3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Roger, The Fontana History of the Human Sciences (London: Fontana, 1997)Google Scholar
Smith, LaurenceBehaviorism and Logical Positivism: A Reassesment of the Alliance (Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1986).Google Scholar
Smith, RogerInhibition: History and Meaning in the Sciences of Mind and Brain (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1992).Google Scholar
Sokal, Michael M., ed., An Education in Psychology: James McKeen Cattell’s Journal and Letters from Germany and England, 1880–1888 (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1981)Google Scholar
Sokal, Michael M.Origins and Early Years of the American Psychological Association, 1890–1906,” American Psychologist, 47 (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokal, Michael M.The Gestalt Psychologists in Behaviorist America,” American Historical Review, 89 (1984).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokal, Michael M. ed., Psychological Testing and American Society (New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1987)Google Scholar
Staeuble, Irmingard‘Psychological Man’ and Human Subjectivity in Historical Perspective,” History of the Human Sciences, 4 (1991)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, WilliamDie psychologische Arbeit des 19. Jahrhunderts,” Zeitschrift für pädagogische Psychologie, 2 (1900).Google Scholar
Stout, G. F.Analytic Psychology, 2 vols. (London: Allen and Unwin, 1909)Google Scholar
Tolman, Edward C.Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men (New York: Century, 1932).Google Scholar
van Ginneken, JappCrowds, Psychology and Politics, 1871–1899 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992).Google Scholar
van Strien, Pieter J.The American ‘Colonization’ of Northwest European Social Psychology after World War II,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 33 (1997).3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidal, FernandoPiaget before Piaget (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1994).Google Scholar
von Mayrhauser, RichardThe Practical Language of American Intellect,” History of the Human Sciences, 4 (1991)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wise, Norton M. ed., The Values of Precision (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Wolf, Theta, Alfred Binet (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1974)Google Scholar
Woodworth, Robert S.Contemporary Schools of Psychology (New York: Ronald Press, 1931)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wooldridge, AdrianMeasuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England, c. 1860–c. 1990 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1994).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zenderland, LeilaMeasuring Minds: Henry Herbert Goddard and the Origins of American Mental Testing (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998).Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Psychology
  • Edited by Theodore M. Porter, University of California, Los Angeles, Dorothy Ross, The Johns Hopkins University
  • Book: The Cambridge History of Science
  • Online publication: 28 March 2008
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CHOL9780521594424.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Psychology
  • Edited by Theodore M. Porter, University of California, Los Angeles, Dorothy Ross, The Johns Hopkins University
  • Book: The Cambridge History of Science
  • Online publication: 28 March 2008
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CHOL9780521594424.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Psychology
  • Edited by Theodore M. Porter, University of California, Los Angeles, Dorothy Ross, The Johns Hopkins University
  • Book: The Cambridge History of Science
  • Online publication: 28 March 2008
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CHOL9780521594424.016
Available formats
×