Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2014
  • Online publication date: March 2015

PART III - Objects and media

  • Edited by Pablo P. L. Tinio, Montclair State University, New Jersey, Jeffrey K. Smith, University of Otago, New Zealand
  • Publisher: Cambridge University Press
  • pp 219-478

References

Arnheim, R. (1966). Toward a psychology of art. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception. Berkeley, CA: University of Chicago Press.
Arnheim, R. (1988). The power of the center. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Augustin, M. D., Leder, H., Hutzler, F., and Carbon, C. C. (2008). Style follows content: On the microgenesis of art perception. Acta Psychologica, 128, 127–38.
Augustin, M. D., Defranceschi, B., Fuchs, H., Carbon, C. C., and Hutzler, F. (2011). The neural time course of art perception: An ERP study on the processing of style versus content in art. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2,071–81.
Bouleau, C. (1980). The painter’s secret geometry. New York: Hacker Books.
Cooper, J., and Silvia, P. (2009). Opposing art: Rejection as an action tendency of hostile aesthetic emotions. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27, 109–26.
Cupchik, G. C. (2007). A critical reflection on Arnheim’s Gestalt theory of aesthetics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 16–24.
Cupchik, G., Vartainian, O., Crawley, A., and Mikulis, D. (2009). Viewing artworks: Contributions of cognitive and perceptual facilitation to aesthetic experience. Brain and Cognition, 70, 84–91.
Currie, G. (1985). The authentic and the aesthetic. American Philosophical Quarterly, 22, 153–60.
Cutting, J. (2006). Impressionism and its canon. Nanham, MD: University Press of America.
Firstov, V., Firstov, V., Voloshinov, A., and Locher, P. (2007). The colorimetric barycenter of paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 25, 209–17.
Forsythe, A., Nadal, M., Sheehy, N., Cela-Conde, C., and Sawey, M. (2011). Predicting beauty: Fractal dimension and visual complexity in art. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 49–70.
Hubard, O. (2007). Originals and reproductions: The influence of presentation format in adolescents’ responses to a Renaissance painting. Studies in Art Education, 48, 247–64.
Jacobsen, T. (2006). Bridging the arts and sciences: A framework for the psychology of aesthetics. Leonardo, 39, 155–62.
Johnson, M., Muday, J., and Schirillo, J. (2010). When viewing variations in paintings by Mondrian, aesthetic preferences correlate with pupil size. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 161–67.
Kandinsky, V. (1926/1979). Point and line to plane. New York: Dover.
Kapoula, Z., Adenis, M. S., , T. T., Yang, Q., and Lipede, G. (2011). Pictorial depth increases body sway. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 186–93.
Kuchinke, L., Trapp, S., Jacobs., A., and Leder, H. (2009). Pupillary responses in art appreciation:Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 156–63.
Latto, R. (1995). The brain of the beholder. In R. Gregory, J. Harris, P. Heard and D. Rose (Eds.), The artful eye (pp. 66–94). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Latto, R., Brian, D., and Kelly, B. (2000). An oblique effect in aesthetics: Homage to Mondrian (1872–1944). Perception, 29, 981–87.
Latto, R., and Russell-Duff, K. (2002). An oblique effect in the selection of line orientation by twentieth century painters. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 20, 49–60.
Linnett, C., Morris, R., Dunlap, W., and Fritchie, C. (1991). Differences in color balance depending upon mode of comparison. Journal of General Psychology, 111, 271–83.
Locher, P. (2003). An empirical investigation of the visual rightness theory of picture perception. Acta Psychologica, 114, 147–64.
Locher, P., Smith, J., and Smith, L. (2001). The influence of presentation format and viewer training in the visual arts on the perception of pictorial and aesthetic qualities of paintings. Perception, 30, 449–65.
Locher, P., and Dolese, M. (2004). A comparison of the perceived pictorial and aesthetic qualities of original paintings and their postcard images. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 22, 129–42.
Locher, P., Overbeeke, K., and Stappers, P. (2005). Spatial balance of color triads in the abstract art of Piet Mondrian. Perception, 34, 169–89.
Locher, P. J. (2012). Empirical investigation of an aesthetic experience with art. In A. Shimamura and S. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience (pp. 163–88). New York: Oxford University Press.
Mastandrea, S., Bartoli, S., and Bove, G. (2009). Preferences for ancient and modern art museums: Visitor experiences and personality characteristics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 164–68.
McManus, I., Stöver, K., and Kim, D. (2011). Arnheim’s Gestalt theory of visual balance: Examining the compositional structure of art photographs and abstract images. I-Perception, 2, 615–47.
Munsell, A. H. (1905). A color notation. Boston, MA: GeorgeH. Ellis Co.
Nadal, M., Munar, E., Marty, G., and Cela-Conde, C. (2010). Visual complexity and beauty appreciation: Explaining the divergence of results. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 28, 173–91.
Ngo, D., and Byrne, W. (2001). Application of an aesthetic evaluation model to data entry screens. Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 149–85.
Nodine, C., Locher, P., and Krupinski, E. (1993). The role of formal art training on the perception and aesthetic judgment of art compositions. Leonardo, 26, 219–27.
Palmer, S., Schloss, K., and Sammartino, J. (2012). Hidden knowledge in aesthetic judgments. In A. Shimamura and S. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience (pp. 189–222). New York: Oxford University Press.
Pinto, P., Linhares, J., Carvalhal, J., and Nascimento, S. (2006). Psychophysical estimation of the best illumination for appreciation of Renaissance paintings. Visual Neuroscience, 23, 669–74.
Plumhoff, J., and Schrillo, J. (2009). Mondrian, eye-movements, and the oblique effect. Perception, 38, 719–32.
Polzella, D., Hammar, S., and Hinkle, C. (2005). The effects of color on viewers’ ratings of paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 23, 153–63.
Reber, R. (2012). Processing fluency, aesthetic pleasure, and culturally shared taste. In A. Shimamura and S. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience (pp. 223–49). New York: Oxford University Press.
Silvia, P. (2012). Human emotions and aesthetic experience: An overview of empirical aesthetics. In A. Shimamura and S. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience (pp. 250–75). New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, L., and Smith, J. (2006). The nature and growth of aesthetic fluency. In P. Locher, C. Martindale and L. Dorfman (Eds.), New directions in aesthetics, creativity, and the arts (pp. 47–58). Amityville, NY: Baywood.
Stangos, N. (1994). Concepts of modern art: From Fauvism to Postmodernism. London: Thames & Hudson Ltd.
Tinio, P.P L., and Leder, H. (2009). Natural scenes are indeed preferred, but image quality might have the last word. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 52–56.
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., Podsiadlo, J., Overbay, S., and Borkum, J. (2005). The link between composition and balance in masterworks vs. paintings of lower artistic quality. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 493–503.
Watanabe, S. (2001). Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: Picture discrimination in pigeons and humans. Animal Cognition, 4, 147–51.
Watanabe, S., Sakamoto, J., and Wakita, M. (1995). Pigeon’s discrimination of paintings by Monet and Picasso. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 165–74.
Wiesmann, M., and Ishai, A. (2010). Training facilitates object recognition in cubist paintings. Frontiers in Human Science, 14, 1–7.
Wilner, E. (2000). The gilded edge. San Francisco, CA: Chronicle Books.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–36.
Wölfflin, H. (1950). Principles of art history: The problem of the development of style in later art. New York: Dover Publications, Inc.
Wright-Smith, R. (1980). Picture framing. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company.

References

Adams, A. (1983). Examples: The making of 40 photographs. New York: Little, Brown and Company.
Amirshahi, S. A., Redies, C., and Denzler, C. (2012). Assessment of the rule of thirds in photographs taken by amateurs. Poster presented at the 1st Visual Science of Art Conference, September1–2, 2012. Alghero, Italy.
Arce-Lopera, C., Masuda, T., Kimura, A., Wada, Y., and Okajima, K. (2012). Luminance distribution modifies the perceived freshness of strawberries. i-Perception, 3, 338–55.
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. The new version. Berkeley, CA: University of California.
Augustin, M. D., Leder, H., Hutzler, F., and Carbon, C.-C. (2008). Style follows content: On the microgenesis of art perception. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 127–38.
Baetens, J. (2010). Is a photograph worth a thousand films?Visual Studies, 24(2), 143–48.
Benjamin, W. (1999). Illuminations. London: Pimlico.
Bouleau, C. (1963). The painter’s secret geometry: A study of composition in art. London: Thames & Hudson.
Bourdieu, P. (2012). Picturing Algeria. New York: Columbia University Press.
Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In J.A. Coan and J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Alvarez, G. A., and Oliva, A. (2008). Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105(38), 14,325–29.
Broadbent, S. (1980). Simulating the ley hunter. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, A, 143, 109–40.
Brusius, M. (2010). Impreciseness in Julia Margaret Cameron’s portrait photographs. History of Photography, 34(4), 342–55.
Burger, W., and Burge, M. J. (2009). Principles of digital image processing: Fundamental techniques. London: Springer-Verlag.
Burgin, V. (1982). Introduction, and Looking at photographs. In V. Burgin (Ed.), Thinking photography (pp. 1–14, 142–53). Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Burke, E. (1757). A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and beautiful. London: R. and J. Dodsley.
Cartier-Bresson, H. (1999). The mind’s eye: Writings on photography and photographs. New York: Aperture.
Cartier-Bresson, H. (2003). De qui s’agit-il?Paris: Gallimard.
Cartier-Bresson, H. (2006). Scrap Book: Photographs 1932–1946. London: Thames & Hudson.
Charrier, P. (2012). On Diane Arbus: Establishing a revisionist framework of analysis. History of Photography, 36(4), 422–38.
Chubb, C., Econopouly, J., and Landy, M. S. (1994). Histogram contrast analysis and the visual segregation of IID textures. Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science and Vision, 11(9), 2,350–74.
Chubb, C., Landy, M. S., and Econopouly, J. (2004). A visual mechanism tuned to black. Vision Research, 44, 3,223–32.
Chubb, C., Nam, J.-H.,Bindman, D. R., and Sperling, G. (2007). The three dimensions of human visual sensitivity to first-order contrast statistics. Vision Research, 47, 2,237–48.
Cooper, E. A., Piazza, E. A., and Banks, M. S. (2012). The perceptual basis of common photographic practice. Journal of Vision, 12(5), Article 8, 1–14.
Cutting, J. E., DeLong, J. E., and Nothelfer, C. E. (2010). Attention and the evolution of Hollywood film. Psychological Science, 21(2), 432–39.
Dror, R. O., Willsky, A. S., and Adelson, E. H. (2004). Statistical characterisation of real-world illumination. Journal of Vision, 4(9), Article 11, 821–837.
Dyer, G. (2005). The ongoing moment. London: Little, Brown.
Edwards, S. (2006). Photography: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Einhaüser, W., Spain, M., and Perona, P. (2008). Objects predict fixations better than early saliency. Journal of Vision, 8(14), Article 18, 1–26.
Fei-Fei, L., Iyer, A., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2007). What do we perceive in a glance at a real-world scene?Journal of Vision, 7(1), Article 10, 1–29.
Fleming, R. W., Torralba, A., and Adelson, E. H. (2004). Specular reflections and the perception of shape. Journal of Vision, 4, 798–820.
Flusser, V. (2000). Towards a philosophy of photography [originally published as Für eine Philosophie der Photographie (1983)]. London: Reaktion Books.
Freeman, M. (2007). The photographer’s eye: Composition and design for better digital photos. Lewes: ILEX.
Gardner, J. S., Nothelfer, C., and Palmer, S. (2008). Exploring aesthetic principles of spatial composition through stock photography. Journal of Vision, 8(6), Article 337, 1. Poster available from: http://socrates.berkeley.edu/%7Eplab/posters/VSS2008.pdf
Goldman, A. (2001). The aesthetic. In B. Gaut and D. M. Lopes (Eds.), The Routledge companion to aesthetics (pp. 181–92). London: Routledge.
Gombrich, E. H. (1979). The sense of order. London: Phaidon Press.
Gombrich, E. H. (2006). Concerning “The Science of Art.”Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 17.
Hagen, M. A. (1986). Varieties of realism: Geometries of representational art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heinrich, S. P., and Bach, M. (2010). Less is more: Subjective detailedness depends on stimulus size. Journal of Vision, 10(2), 1–10.
Hicks, R. W. (2005). Composition. In R. Lenman (Ed.), The Oxford companion to the photograph (pp. 135–36). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40, 1,489–1506.
Jacobs, S. (2010). The history and aesthetics of the classical film still. History of Photography, 34(4), 373–86.
Jeffrey, I. (1997). The Photography Book. London: Phaidon.
Ke, Y., Tang, X., and Jing, F. (2006). The design of high-level features for photo quality assessment. Proc.IEEE Conf.Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition ( CVPR), 1, 419–26.
Kim, J., Marlow, P. J., and Anderson, B. L. (2012). The dark side of gloss. Nature Neuroscience, 15(11), 1,590–95.
Kubovy, M. (1985). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Latto, R., and Harper, B. (2007). The non-realistic nature of photography: Further reasons why Turner was wrong. Leonardo, 40(3), 243–47.
Levinson, J. (2003). Philosophical aesthetics: An overview. In J. Levinson (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of aesthetics (pp. 3–24). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lubben, K. (2011). Magnum contact sheets. London: Thames & Hudson.
Luo, Y., and Tang, X. (2008). Photo and video quality evaluation: Focusing on the subject. Computer Vision-ECCV, 2008, 386–99.
Maynard, P. (2001). Photography. In B. Gaut and D. M. Lopes (Eds.), The Routledge companion to aesthetics (pp. 477–90). London: Routledge.
McManus, I. C. (2005). Symmetry and asymmetry in aesthetics and the arts. European Review, 13(Supp. 2), 157–80.
McManus, I. C., and Humphrey, N. K. (1973). Turning the left cheek. Nature, 243, 271–72.
McManus, I. C., and Kitson, C. M. (1995). Compositional geometry in pictures. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 13, 73–94.
McManus, I. C., and Weatherby, P. (1997). The golden section and the aesthetics of form and composition: A cognitive model. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 15, 209–32.
McManus, I. C., and Thomas, P. (2007). Eye-centring in portraits: A theoretical and empirical evaluation. Perception, 36, 167–82.
McManus, I. C., Cook, R., and Hunt, A. (2010). Beyond the Golden Section and normative aesthetics: Why do individuals differ so much in their aesthetic preferences for rectangles?Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 113–26.
McManus, I. C., Stöver, K., and Kim, D. (2011). Arnheim’s Gestalt theory of visual balance: Examining the compositional structure of art photographs and abstract images. i-Perception, 2(6), 615–47.
McManus, I. C., Zhou, A., I’Anson, S., Waterfield, L., Stöver, K., and Cook, R. (2011). The psychometrics of photographic cropping: The influence of colour, meaning, and expertise. Perception, 40, 332–57.
Meese, T. S., Baker, D. H., and Summers, R. J. (2012). Fake tilt shift miniaturization causes negative d-prime for detecting reality. Predicting Perceptions: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Apperance, Edinburgh, April 2012 (61).
Metzger, W. (1953). Gesetze des Sehens (2nd edition). Frankfurt am Main: Verlag von Waldemar Kramer.
Metzger, W. (2006). Laws of seeing (Trans. L. Spillmann, S. Lehar, M. Stromeyer and M. Wertheimer). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Moholy-Nagy, L. (1936). From pigment to light. Telhor: The International Review New Vision (Brno), 1, 32–36.
Moorthy, A. K., and Bovik, A. C. (2011). Visual quality measures: What does the future hold?International Journal of Multimedia Tools and Applications, 51(2), 675–96.
Palmer, S. E., Gardner, J. S., and Wickens, T. D. (2008). Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects of position and direction on framing single objects. Spatial Vision, 21(3–5), 421–49.
Parsons, S. (2009). Sontag’s lament: Emotion, ethics, and photography. Photography and Culture, 2(3), 289–302.
Peréz-González, C. (2012a). Lateral organisation in nineteenth-century studio photographs is influenced by the direction of writing: A comparison of Iranian and Spanish photographs. Laterality, 17(5), 515–32.
Peréz-González, C. (2012b). Local portraiture: Through the lens of the 19th-century Iranian photographers. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
Pinto, N., Cox, D. D., and DiCarlo, J. J. (2012). Why is real-world visual object recognition hard?PLoS Computational Biology, 4(1), e27.
Pirenne, M. H. (1970). Optics, painting and photography. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Prosser, J. (2010). Click! Photography changes everything. Photography and Culture, 3(1), 119–22.
Radonjic, A., Allred, S. R., Gilchrist, A. L., and Brainard, D. H. (2011). The dynamic range of human lightness perception. Current Biology, 21(22), 1,931–36.
Ramachandran, V. S., and Hirstein, W. (1999). The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 15–51.
Reinhard, E., Stark, M., Shirley, P., and Ferwerda, J. (2002). Photographic tone reproduction for digital images. ACM Transactions on Graphics (TOG) – Proceedings of ACM SIGGRAPH 2002, 21(3), 267–76.
Riding, A. (2003). An instinct for decisive moments: A show and a Foundation honor Cartier-Bresson. New York Times, May 27.
Ross, D. W. (1907). A theory of pure design: Harmony, balance, rhythm. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Schapiro, M. (1994). Theory and philosophy of art: Style, artist, and society. Selected papers. New York: George Braziller.
Silvera, D. H., Josephs, R. A., and Giesler, R. B. (2002). Bigger is better: The influence of physical size on aesthetic preference judgements. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 189–202.
Smith, C. Z. (1985). Audience reception of Diane Arbus’ photographs. Journal of American Culture, 8(1), 19–28.
Smith, J. K., and Smith, L. F. (2001). Spending time on art. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 19(2), 229–36.
Smith, J. T. (1797). Remarks on rural scenery; with twenty etchings of cottages, from nature; and some observations and precepts relative to the pictoresque. London: Nathaniel Smith and I.T. Smith.
Smith, T. J., Levin, D., and Cutting, J. E. (2012). A window on reality: Perceving edited moving images. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 107–13.
Snyder, J., and Allen, N. W. (1975). Photography, vision and representation. Critical Inquiry, 2(1), 143–69.
Sontag, S. (1979). On photography. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Sontag, S. (2004). Regarding the pain of others. London: Penguin.
Standing, L. (1973). Learning 10,000 pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 207–22.
Stones, M. J., and Bygate, M. (2009). Emotive factors that make photographs memorable. Photography and Culture, 2(2), 119–34.
Suitner, C., and McManus, I. C. (2012). Aesthetic asymmetries, spatial agency, and art history: A social psychological perspective. In T. W. Schubert and A. Maass (Eds.), Spatial dimensions of social thought (pp. 277–301). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Szarkowski, J. (1976). Introduction. InWilliam Eggleston’s guide. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thomas, B. (1969). Geometry in pictorial composition. Newcastle: Oriel Press.
Tinio, P. P. L., and Leder, H. (2009). Natural scenes are indeed preferred, but image quality might have the last word. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 52–56.
Tinio, P. P. L., Leder, H., and Strasser, M. (2010). Image quality and the aesthetic judgement of photographs: Contrast, sharpness, and grain teased apart and put together. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 5(2), 165–76.
Tinio, P. P. L., and Leder, H. (2013). The means to art’s end: Styles, creative devices, and the challenge of art. In A. S. Bristol, O. Vartanian and J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The neuroscience of creativity (pp. 273–98). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tkacik, G., Prentice, J. S., Victor, J. D., and Balsubramanian, V. (2010). Local statistics in natural scenes predict the saliency of synthetic textures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107(42), 18,149–54.
Troscianko, T., Meese, T. S., and Hinde, S. (2012). Perception while watching movies: Effects of physical screen size and scene type. i-Perception, 3, 414–45.
Tyler, C. W. (1998, April 30). Painters centre one eye in portraits. Nature, 392, 877.
Vishwanath, D., and Blaser, E. (2010). Retinal blur and the perception of egocentric distance. Journal of Vision, 10(10), Article 26, 1–16.
Wallis, T. S. A., and Bex, P. J. (2012). Image correlates of crowding in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 12(7), Article 6, 1–19.
Wichmann, F. A., Drewes, J., Rosas, P., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2010). Animal detection in natural scenes: Critical features revisited. Journal of Vision, 10(4), Article 6, 1–27.
Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiest, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., and Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling lower-level image properties: The SHINE toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 671–84.
Zakia, R. D. (2012). Perception and imaging: Photography – A way of seeing. Burlington, MA: Focal Press.
Zakia, R. D., and Page, D. A. (2011). Photographic composition: A visual guide. Burlington, MA: Focal Press.

References

Ball, P. (2010). The music instinct: How music works and why we can’t do without it. London: The Bodley Head.
Barsalou, L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–609.
Barsalou, L.W., Simmons, W.K., Barbey, A.K., and Wilson, C.D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. TRENDS in Cognitive Science, 7, 84–91.
Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration. Science, 153, 25–33.
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Bermudez, J. L., Marcel, A., and Eilan, N. (Eds.) (1995). The body and the self. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Bhatt, R. S., and Quinn, P. C. (2011). How does learning impact development in infancy? The case of perceptual organization. Infancy, 16, 2–38.
Biederman, I., and Vessel, E. A. (2006). Perceptual pleasure and the brain. American Scientist, May–June, 249–55.
Blijlevens, J., Gemser, G., and Mugge, R. (2012). The importance of being “well-placed”: The influence of context on perceived typicality and esthetic appraisal of produce appearance. Acta Psychologica, 139, 178–86.
Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in the brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98, 11,818–23.
Bloom, P. (2010). How pleasure works: The new science of why we like what we like. London: The Bodley Head.
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–89.
Boselie, F., and Leeuwenberg, E. (1985). Birkhoff revisited: Beauty as a function of effect and means. American Journal of Psychology, 98, 1–39.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. I: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Brattico, E., Brattico, P., and Jacobsen, T. (2009). The origins of the aesthetic enjoyment of music – A review of the literature. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 15–37.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organisation of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bullough, E. (1912). “Psychical distance” as a factor in art and an aesthetic principle. British Journal of Psychology, 5, 87–118.
Chang, H. H., and Chen, S. W. (2009). Consumer perception of interface quality, security, and loyalty in electronic commerce. Information and Management, 46, 411–17.
Cialdini, R. B., Wosinska, W., Barrett, D. W., Butner, J., and Gornik-Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures: The differential influence of social proof and commitment/consistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1,242–53.
Cox, D., and Cox, A. D. (2002). Beyond first impressions: The effects of repeated exposure on consumer liking of visually complex and simple product designs. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 119–30.
Crease, R. P. (2003). The prism and the pendulum: The ten most beautiful experiments in science. New York: Random House.
Crilly, N. (2011). Do users know what designers are up to? Product experience and the inference of persuasive intentions. International Journal of Design, 5, 1–15.
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.
Desmet, P. M. A., and Hekkert, P. (2007). Framework of product experience. International Journal of Design, 1, 57–66.
Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Berkley Publishing Group.
Diefenbach, S., and Hassenzahl, M. (2011). The dilemma of the hedonic – appreciated, but hard to justify. Interacting with Computers, 23, 461–72.
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–90.
Distel, H., Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Martínez-Gómez, M., Schicker, I., Kobayakawa, T., Saito, S., and Hudson, R. (1999). Perception of everyday odors – Correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgement. Chemical Senses, 24, 191–99.
Dunne, A. (1999). Hertzian tales: Electronic products, aesthetic experience and critical design. London: RCA CRD Research publications.
Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., and Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109–28.
Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns vs. novel ones. Science, 146, 668–70.
Fechner, G. T. (1876). Vorschule der Ästhetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.
Forgas, J. P. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Summary and integration. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 387–406). Paris: Cambridge University Press.
Freitas, A. L., Azizian, A., Travers, S., and Berry, S. A. (2005). The evaluative connotation of processing fluency: Inherently positive or moderated by motivational context?Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 636–44.
Gallace, A., and Spence, C. (2011). To what extent do Gestalt grouping principles influence tactile perception?Psychological Bulletin, 137, 538–61.
Gallese, V., and Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 455–79.
Gaver, W. W., and Mandler, G. (1989). Play it again Sam: On liking music. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 259–82.
Gibbs, R. (2003). Embodied experience and linguistic meaning. Brain and Language, 84, 1–15.
Goldman, A. (2001). The aesthetic. In B. Gaut and D. McIver Lopes (Eds.), The Routledge companion to aesthetics (pp. 181–92). London: Routledge.
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hardy, G. H. (1940). A mathematician’s apology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hassenzahl, M. (2004). The interplay of beauty, goodness, and usability in interactive products. Human–Computer Interaction, 15, 1–30.
Hassenzahl, M., and Monk, A. (2010). The inference of perceived usability from beauty. Human#8211;Computer Interaction, 25, 235–60.
Hassenzahl, M., Schöbel, M., and Trautmann, T. (2008). How motivational orientation influences the evaluation and choice of hedonic and pragmatic interactive products: The role of regulatory focus. Interacting with Computers, 20, 473–79.
Hauser, M. D., and McDermott, J. (2003). The evolution of the music faculty: A comparative perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 663–68.
Hekkert, P. (2006). Design aesthetics: Principles of pleasure in design. Psychology Science, 48, 157–72.
Hekkert, P., and van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Complexity and prototypicality as determinants of the appraisal of Cubist paintings. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 483–95.
Hekkert, P., Snelders, D., and van Wieringen, P. C. W. (2003). “Most advanced, yet acceptable”: Typicality and novelty as joint predictors of aesthetic preference in industrial design. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 111–24.
Hekkert, P. and Leder, H. (2008). Product aesthetics. In H. N. J. Schifferstein and P. Hekkert (Eds.), Product experience (pp. 259–85). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Hekkert, P., and van Dijk, M. B. (2011). Vision in design: A guidebook for innovators. Amsterdam: BIS Publishers.
Hekkert, P., Thurgood, C., and Whitfield, T.W.A. (2013). The mere exposure effect for consumer products as a consequence of existing familiarity and controlled exposure. Acta Psychologica, 144, 411–17.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1,280–1,300.
Hunter, M. A., Ames, E. W., and Koopman, R. (1983). Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarization time on infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 19, 338–52.
Isen, A. M. (2002). Some perspectives on positive affect and self-regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 184–87.
Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body: Aesthetics of human understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Johnston, V. S. (2003). The origin and function of pleasure. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 167–79.
Kanazawa, S. (2004). General intelligence as a domain-specific adaptation. Psychological Review, 111, 512–23.
Kant, I. (1790/1952). The critique of judgement. (Trans. J. C. Meredith). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Kaplan, L. B., Szybillo, G. J., and Jacoby, J. (1974). Components of perceived risk in product purchase. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 287–91.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., and Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder. Science, 322, 1,681–85.
Kleine, S. S., Kleine III, R. E., and Allen, C. T. (1995). How is a possession “me” or “not me”? Characterizing types and an antecedent of material possession attachment. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 327–42.
Lawless, H. T. (2000). Sensory combinations in the meal. In H. L. Meiselman (Ed.), Dimensions of the meal: The science, culture, business and art of eating (pp. 92–106). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
Liberman, N., Idson, L. C., Camacho, C. J., and Higgins, E. T. (1999). Promotion and prevention choices between stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1,135–45.
Lindgaard, G., Fernandes, G., Dudek, C., and Brown, J. (2006). Attention web designers: You have 50 milliseconds to make a good first impression. Behaviour and Information Technology, 25, 115–26.
Locher, P., Overbeeke, K., and Wensveen, S. (2010). Aesthetic interaction: A framework. Design Issues, 26, 70–79.
Margulies, S. (1977). Principles of beauty. Psychological Reports, 41, 3–11.
Markus, H., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.
Martindale, C. (1984). The pleasures of thought: A theory of cognitive hedonics. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 5, 49–80.
Martindale, C. (1990). The clockwork muse: The predictability of artistic change. New York: Basic Books.
Martindale, C., and Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 661–70.
McWhinnie, H. J. (1968). A review of research on aesthetic measure. Acta Psychologica, 28, 363–75.
Moshagen, M., Musch, J., and Göritz, A. S. (2009). A blessing, not a curse: Experimental evidence for beneficial effects of visual aesthetics on performance. Ergonomics, 52, 1,311–20.
Moshagen, M., and Thielsch, M. T. (2010). Facets of visual aesthetics. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 68, 689–709.
Murphy, S. T., and Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–39.
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently . . . and why. New York: The Free Press.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norezayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.
Orians, G. H., and Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 555–79). New York: Oxford University Press.
Overbeeke, K., Djajadiningrat, T., Hummels, C., Wensveen, S., and Frens, J. (2003). Let’s make things engaging. In M. A. Blythe, K. Overbeeke, A. F. Monk and P. C. Wright (Eds.), Funology: From usability to enjoyment (pp. 7–17). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
Park, D. C., and Huang, C. M. (2010). Culture wires the brain: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 391–400.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.
Ramachandran, V. S., and Hirstein, W. (1999). The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 15–51.
Ratner, R. K., and Kahn, B. E. (2002). The impact of private versus public consumption on variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 246–57.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., and Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience?Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–82.
Reed, A. (2004). Activating the self-importance of consumer selves: Exploring identity salience effects on judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 286–95.
Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., and Anderson, A. K. (2006). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 383–88.
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., and Johnstone, T. (Eds.) (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schifferstein, H. N. J., and Hekkert, P. (2011). Sensory aesthetics in product design. In F. Bacci and D. Melcher (Eds.), Art and the senses (pp. 529–55). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Scruton, R. (1983). The aesthetic understanding. Essays in the philosophy of art and culture. South Bend, IN: St. Augustine’s Press.
Sekuler, R., and Blake, R. (1994). Perception (3rd edition). New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Shah, J. Y., Higgins, E. T., and Friedman, R. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285–93.
Shusterman, R. (2008). Body consciousness: A philosophy of mindfulness and somaesthetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sonderegger, A., and Sauer, J. (2009). The influence of design aesthetics in usability testing: Effects on user performance and perceived usability. Applied Ergonomics, 41, 403–10.
Symons, D. (1995). Beauty is in the adaptations of the beholder: The evolutionary psychology of human female sexual attractiveness. In P. R. Abramson and S. D. Pinkerton (Eds.), Sexual nature/sexual culture (pp. 80–118). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Takahashi, S. (1995). Aesthetic properties of pictorial perception. Psychological Review, 102, 671–83.
Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics informs traditional aesthetics. In E. Voland and K. Grammar (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 9–35). Berlin: Springer.
Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Human facial beauty: Averageness, symmetry and parasite resistance. Human Nature, 4, 237–69.
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5–67). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Tractinsky, N. (2012). Visual aesthetics in human–computer interaction and interaction design. In M. Soegaard and R. F. Dam (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human–Computer Interaction. Aarhus, Denmark: The Interaction Design Foundation. Available from: www.interactiondesign.org/encyclopedia/visual_aesthetics.html
Tractinsky, N., Katz, A. S., and Ikar, D. (2000). What is beautiful is usable. Interacting with Computers, 13, 127–45.
Tractinsky, N., Abdu, R., Forlizzi, J., and Seder, T. (2011). Towards personalisation of the driver environment: Investigating responses to instrument cluster design. International Journal of Vehicle Design, 55, 208–36.
Uehara, I. (2000). Transition from novelty to familiarity preference depending on recognition performance by 4-yr.-olds. Psychological Reports, 87, 837–48.
van Schaik, P., Hassenzahl, M., and Ling, J. (2012). User-experience from an inference perspective. ACM Transactions on Computer–Human Interaction, 19, Article No. 11.
Veryzer, R. W., Jr., and Hutchinson, J. W. (1998). The influence of unity and prototypicality on aesthetic responses to new product designs. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 374–94.
Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A., Singh, M., and von der Heydt, R. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1,172–1,217.
Whitfield, T. W. A. (1983). Predicting preference for familiar, everyday objects: An experimental confrontation between two theories of aesthetic behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 221–37.
Whitfield, T. W. A. (2005). Aesthetics as pre-linguistic knowledge: A psychological perspective. Design Issues, 21, 3–17.
Whitfield, T. W. A., and Slatter, P. E. (1979). The effects of categorization and prototypicality on aesthetic choice in a furniture selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 65–75.
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement, 9, 1–27.
Zhang, Y., Feick, L., and Price, L. J. (2006). The impact of self-construal on aesthetic preference for angular versus rounded shapes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 794–805.

References

Alperson, P. (1994). What is music? An introduction to the philosophy of music. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Armstrong, T., and Detweiler-Bedell, B. (2008). Beauty as an emotion: The exhilarating prospect of mastering a challenging world. Review of General Psychology, 12(4), 305–29.
Besson, M., and Macar, F. (1986). Visual and auditory event-related potentials elicited by linguistic and non-linguistic incongruities. Neuroscience letters, 63(2), 109–14.
Besson, M., and Faita, F. (1995). An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(6), 1,278–96.
Besson, M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A. M., and Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the brain: Independence of lyrics and tunes. Psychological Science, 9(6), 494–98.
Besson, M., and Schön, D. (2001). Comparison between language and music. In R. J. Zatorre and I. Peretz (Eds.), The biological foundations of music, Vol. 930 (pp. 232–58). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.
Bharucha, J. J., and Krumhansl, C. L. (1983). The representation of harmonic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of context. Cognition, 13, 63–102.
Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46–52.
Blood, A., and Zatorre, R. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(20), 11,818.
Bregman, A. (1994). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Budd, M. (1996). Values of art. London: Penguin Books.
Cacioppo, J., and Hawkley, L. (2003). Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 46(3), S39–S52.
Callan, D., Tsytsarev, V., Hanakawa, T., Callan, A., Katsuhara, M., Fukuyama, H., and Turner, R. (2006). Song and speech: Brain regions involved with perception and covert production. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1,327–42.
Crapse, T., and Sommer, M. (2008). Corollary discharge circuits in the primate brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(6), 552–57.
Cross, I. (2009). The evolutionary nature of musical meaning. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2), 179–200.
Cross, I., and Morley, I. (2008). The evolution of music: Theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence. In S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship (pp. 61–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Daltrozzo, J., and Schön, D. (2009a). Conceptual processing in music as revealed by N400 effects on words and musical targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1,882–92.
Daltrozzo, J., and Schön, D. (2009b). Is conceptual processing in music automatic? An electrophysiological approach. Brain Research, 1270, 88–94.
Darwin, C. (1997). Auditory grouping. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(9), 327–33.
Darwin, C. (2008). Listening to speech in the presence of other sounds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1,493), 1,011–21.
Davies, S. (1994). Musical meaning and expression. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Di Pietro, M., Laganaro, M., Leemann, B., and Schnider, A. (2004). Receptive amusia: Temporal auditory processing deficit in a professional musician following a left temporo-parietal lesion. Neuropsychologia, 42(7), 868–77.
Drost, U., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T. C., and Prinz, W. (2005a). Action-effect coupling in pianists. Psychological Research, 69(4), 233–41.
Drost, U., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T. C., and Prinz, W. (2005b). When hearing turns into playing: Movement induction by auditory stimuli in pianists. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(8), 1,376–89.
Fazio, P., Cantagallo, A., Craighero, L., D’Ausilio, A., Roy, A., Pozzo, T., . . . (2009). Encoding of human action in Broca’s area. Brain, 132(7), 1,980–88.
Fedorenko, E., Patel, A., Casasanto, D., Winawer, J., and Gibson, E. (2009). Structural integration in language and music: Evidence for a shared system. Memory and Cognition, 37(1), 1–19.
Fitch, W., and Hauser, M. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5,656), 377–80.
Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., and Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(7), 2,458–63.
Friedrich, R., and Friederici, A. D. (2009). Mathematical logic in the human brain: Syntax. PLoS ONE, 4(5), e5599.
Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammler, D., Peretz, I., Turner, R., . . . (2009). Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Current Biology, 19(7), 573–76.
Grahn, J., and Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 893–906.
Grewe, O., Nagel, F., Kopiez, R., and Altenmüller, E. (2007a). Emotions over time: Synchronicity and development of subjective, physiological, and facial affective reactions of music. Emotion, 7(4), 774–88.
Grewe, O., Nagel, F., Kopiez, R., and Altenmüller, E. (2007b). Listening to music as a recreative process: Physiological, psychological, and psychoacoustical correlates of chills and strong emotions. Music Perception, 24(3), 297–314.
Grieser-Painter, J., and Koelsch, S. (2011). Can out-of-context musical sounds convey meaning? An ERP study on the processing of meaning in music. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 645–55.
Griffiths, T., and Warren, J. (2002). The planum temporale as a computational hub. Trends in Neurosciences, 25(7), 348–53.
Groussard, M., Viader, F., Hubert, V., Landeau, B., Abbas, A., Desgranges, B., . . . (2010). Musical and verbal semantic memory: Two distinct neural networks?Neuro-Image, 49(3), 2,764–73.
Hackett, T. A., and Kaas, J. (2004). Auditory cortex in primates: Functional subdivisions and processing streams. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 215–32). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., and Rapson, R. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2(3), 96–100.
Hatsopoulos, N., Xu, Q., and Amit, Y. (2007). Encoding of movement fragments in the motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(19), 5,105–14.
Haueisen, J., and Knösche, T. (2001). Involuntary motor activity in pianists evoked by music perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(6), 786–92.
Herrojo-Ruiz, M., Jabusch, H., and Altenmüller, E. (2009). Detecting wrong notes in advance: Neuronal correlates of error monitoring in pianists. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2,625–39.
Herrojo-Ruiz, M., Strübing, F., Jabusch, H. C., and Altenmüller, E. (2010). EEG oscillatory patterns are associated with error prediction during music performance and are altered in musician’s dystonia. NeuroImage, 55, 1,791–1,803.
Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Istók, E., Brattico, E., Jacobsen, T., Krohn, K., Müller, M., and Tervaniemi, M. (2009). Aesthetic responses to music: A questionnaire study. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2), 183–206.
Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Höfel, L., and Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage, 29(1), 276–85.
Jakobson, R. (1960). Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics. Style in Language, 350, 377.
Janata, P. (2009). The neural architecture of music-evoked autobiographical memories. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2,579–94.
Janata, P., Birk, J., van Horn, J., Leman, M., Tillmann, B., and Bharucha, J. (2002). The cortical topography of tonal structures underlying Western music. Science, 298(5,601), 2,167–70.
Janata, P., Tillmann, B., and Bharucha, J. (2002). Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 121–40.
Juslin, P., and Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code?Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 770–814.
Juslin, P., and Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(05), 559–75.
Kaas, J., and Hackett, T. (2000). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11,793.
Karbusický, V. (1986). Grundriß der musikalischen Semantik. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
Katahira, K., Abla, D., Masuda, S., and Okanoya, K. (2008). Feedback-based error monitoring processes during musical performance: An ERP study. Neuroscience Research, 61(1), 120–28.
Khalfa, S., Isabelle, P., Jean-Pierre, B., and Manon, R. (2002). Event-related skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328(2), 145–49.
Kivy, P. (1999). Feeling the musical emotions. British Journal of Aesthetics, 39(1), 1–13.
Knösche, T., Neuhaus, C., Haueisen, J., Alter, K., Maess, B., Witte, O., and Friederici, A. D. (2005). Perception of phrase structure in music. Human Brain Mapping, 24(4), 259–73.
Koechlin, E., and Jubault, T. (2006). Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron, 50(6), 963–74.
Koelsch, S. (2000). Brain and music – A contribution to the investigation of central auditory processing with a new electrophysiological approach. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Koelsch, S. (2004). Spatio-temporal aspects of processing syntax and semantics in music. Habilitation thesis, University of Leipzig.
Koelsch, S. (2009). Music-syntactic processing and auditory memory: Similarities and differences between ERAN and MMN. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 179–90.
Koelsch, S. (2011). Towards a neural basis of processing musical semantics. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(2), 89–105.
Koelsch, S. (2012). Brain and music. Oxford: Wiley.
Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., von Cramon, D. Y., Zysset, S., Lohmann, G., and Friederici, A. D. (2002). Bach speaks: A cortical “language-network” serves the processing of music. NeuroImage, 17, 956–66.
Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T. C., and Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language, and meaning: Brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 302–7.
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., Schulze, K., Alsop, D., and Schlaug, G. (2005). Adults and children processing music: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 25(4), 1,068–76.
Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., Wittfoth, M., and Sammler, D. (2005). Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1,565–77.
Koelsch, S., and Siebel, W. (2005). Towards a neural basis of music perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 578–84.
Koelsch, S., Fuermetz, J., Sack, U., Bauer, K., Hohenadel, M., Wiegel, M., . . . (2011). Effects of music listening on cortisol levels and propofol consumption during spinal anesthesia. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(58), 1–9.
Koelsch, S., and Stegemann, T. (2012). The brain and positive biological effects in healthy and clinical populations. In R. MacDonald, D. Kreutz and L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, health and well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Koopman, C., and Davies, S. (2001). Musical meaning in a broader perspective. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 59(3), 261–73.
Kraus, N., and Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 599–605.
Krumhansl, C. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., and Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: Audio-motor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(2), 308–14.
Lau, E., Phillips, C., and Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (De) constructing the n400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920–33.
LeDoux, J. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review Neuroscience, 23, 155–84.
Levitt, P., and Moore, R. (1979). Origin and organization of brainstem catecholamine innervation in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 186(4), 505–28.
Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Peretz, I., Babaie, M., Laguitton, V., and Chauvel, P. (1998). Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music processing. Brain, 121(10), 1,853–67.
Lundqvist, L., Carlsson, F., Hilmersson, P., and Juslin, P. (2009). Emotional responses to music: Experience, expression, and physiology. Psychology of Music, 37(1), 61–90.
Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., and Friederici, A. D. (2001). Musical syntax is processed in the area of Broca: An MEG-study. Nature Neuroscience, 4(5), 540–45.
Maidhof, C., Rieger, M., Prinz, W., and Koelsch, S. (2009). Nobody is perfect: ERP effects prior to performance errors in musicians indicate fast monitoring processes. PLoS One, 4(4), e5032.
Maidhof, C., Vavatzanidis, N., Prinz, W., Rieger, M., and Koelsch, S. (2010). Processing expectancy violations during music performance and perception: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2,401–13.
Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A., and Friederici, A. D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20), 8,362.
Meyer, L. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Meyer, M., Alter, K., Friederici, A. D., Lohmann, G., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). FMRI reveals brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Human Brain Mapping, 17(2), 73–88.
Meyer, M., Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., Friederici, A. D., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Brain activity varies with modulation of dynamic pitch variances in sentence melody. Brain and Language, 89, 277–89.
Miranda, R., and Ullman, M. (2007). Double dissociation between rules and memory in music: An event-related potential study. NeuroImage, 38(2), 331–45.
Müller, M., Höfel, L., Brattico, E., and Jacobsen, T. (2010). Aesthetic judgments of music in experts and laypersons – An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76(1), 40–51.
Näätänen, R., Tervaniemi, M., Sussman, E., Paavilainen, P., and Winkler, I. (2001). Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(5), 283–88.
Nan, Y., Knösche, T., and Friederici, A. D. (2006). The perception of musical phrase structure: A cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Research, 1,094(1), 179–91.
Neuhaus, C., Knösche, T., and Friederici, A. D. (2006). Effects of musical expertise and boundary markers on phrase perception in music. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 472–93.
Opitz, B., and Kotz, S. (2012). Ventral premotor cortex lesions disrupt learning of sequential grammatical structures. Cortex, 48(2), 664–73.
Orini, M., Bailón, R., Enk, R., Koelsch, S., Mainardi, L., and Laguna, P. (2010). A method for continuously assessing the autonomic response to music-induced emotions through HRV analysis. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 48(5), 423–33.
Paller, K. A., McCarthy, G., and Wood, C. C. (1992). Event-related potentials elicited by deviant endings to melodies. Psychophysiology, 29(2), 202–6.
Panksepp, J., and Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: The neuroaffective foundations of musical appreciation. Behavioural Processes, 60(2), 133–55.
Pantev, C., Roberts, L. E., Schulz, M., Engelien, A., and Ross, B. (2001). Timbre-specific enhancement of auditory cortical representation in musicians. NeuroReport, 12(1), 169–74.
Patel, A. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 674–81.
Patel, A., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., and Holcomb, P. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(6), 717–33.
Patel, A., Iversen, J., Wassenaar, M., and Hagoort, P. (2008). Musical syntactic processing in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Aphasiology, 22(7), 776–89.
Patterson, R., Uppenkamp, S., Johnsrude, I., and Griffiths, T. (2002). The processing of temporal pitch and melody information in auditory cortex. Neuron, 36(4), 767–76.
Peretz, I., and Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity of music processing. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 688–91.
Peretz, I., and Zatorre, R. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annual Reviews in Psychology, 56, 89–114.
Peretz, I., Brattico, E., Järvenpää, M., and Tervaniemi, M. (2009). The amusic brain: In tune, out of key, and unaware. Brain, 132(5), 1,277–86.
Perlovsky, L. (2007). Neural dynamic logic of consciousness: The knowledge instinct. Neurodynamics of Cognition and Consciousness, 73–108.
Platel, H., Baron, J., Desgranges, B., Bernard, F., and Eustache, F. (2003). Semantic and episodic memory of music are subserved by distinct neural networks. Neuro-Image, 20, 244–56.
Poulet, J., and Hedwig, B. (2007). New insights into corollary discharges mediated by identified neural pathways. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(1), 14–21.
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92.
Rohrmeier, M., and Cross, I. (2008). Statistical properties of tonal harmony in Bach’s chorales. InProc 10th intl conf on music perception and cognition.
Scherer, K. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr and T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 120–44). New York: Oxford University Press.
Scherer, K. (2004). Which emotions can be evoked by music? What are the underlying mechanisms? And how can we measure them?Journal of New Music Research, 33, 239–51.
Scherer, K., and Zentner, M. (2001). Emotional effects of music: Production rules. In P. Juslin and J. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 361–92). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Siebel, W. A., Winkler, T., and Seitz-Bernhard, B. (1990). Noosomatik i: Theoretische grundlegung. Langwedel: Glaser und Wohlschlegel.
Slevc, L., Rosenberg, J., and Patel, A. (2009). Making psycholinguistics musical: Self-paced reading time evidence for shared processing of linguistic and musical syntax. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 374–81.
Sloboda, J. A. (1991). Music structure and emotional response: Some empirical findings. Psychology of Music, 19, 110–20.
Steinbeis, N., and Koelsch, S. (2008a). Comparing the processing of music and language meaning using EEG and fMRI provides evidence for similar and distinct neural representations. PLoS One, 3(5), e2226.
Steinbeis, N., and Koelsch, S. (2008b). Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1,169–78.
Steinbeis, N., and Koelsch, S. (2008c). Understanding the intentions behind man-made products elicits neural activity in areas dedicated to mental state attribution. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 619–623.
Steinbeis, N., and Koelsch, S. (2011). Affective priming effects of musical sounds on the processing of word meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 604–21.
Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., and Friederici, A. D. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2(2), 191–96.
Sturt, P., Pickering, M., and Crocker, M. (1999). Structural change and reanalysis difficulty in language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 136–50.
Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J., and Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organized approach. Psychological Review, 107(4), 885–913.
Tillmann, B., Janata, P., and Bharucha, J. (2003). Activation of the inferior frontal cortex in musical priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 16(2), 145–61.
Tramo, M., Shah, G., and Braida, L. (2002). Functional role of auditory cortex in frequency processing and pitch perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87(1), 122–39.
Verleger, R. (1990). P3-evoking wrong notes: Unexpected, awaited, or arousing?International Journal of Neuroscience, 55(2–4), 171–79.
, M., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M., and Jacobs, A. (2009). The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 534–38.
Winkler, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3–4), 147–63.
Zatorre, R., Evans, A., and Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(4), 1,908–19.

References

Alter, J. B. (1984). Creativity profile of university and conservatory dance students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 153–58.
Ando, H. (2007). Expertise of actors: Three viewpoints in acting. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 50, 5–14.
Ando, H., and Koyasu, M. (2008). Differences between acting as if one is experiencing pain and acting as if one is pretending to have pain among actors at three expertise levels. In S. Itakura and K. Fujita (Eds.). Origins of the Social Mind (pp. 123–40). New York: Springer.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-Perception theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 1–62.
Benedetti, J. (2007). The art of the actor: The essential history of acting, from classical times to the present day. New York: Theatre Arts Book.
Bettle, N., Bettle, O., Neumarker, U., and Neumarker, K. J. (2001). Body image and self-esteem in adolescent ballet dancers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 297–309.
Biasi, V., Bonaiuto, P., Giannini, A. M., and Chiappero, E. (1999). Personological studies on dancers: Motivations, conflicts, and defense mechanisms. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 17, 171–86.
Blair, R. (2010). Acting, embodiment, and text: Hedda Gabbler and possible uses of cognitive science. Theatre Topics, 20, 11–21.
Boerner, S., Jobst, J., and Wiemann, M. (2010). Exploring the theatrical experience: Results from an empirical investigation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 173.
Brennan, M. A. (1982). Relationship between creative ability in dance and selected creative attributes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55(1), 47–56.
Brown, J. R. (2001). The Oxford illustrated history of theatre. New York: Oxford University Press.
Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., and Parsons, L. M. (2006). The neural basis of human dance. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1,157–67.
Burgoyne, S., Poulin, K., and Rearden, A. (1999). The impact of acting on student actors: Boundary blurring, growth, and emotional distress. Theatre Topics, 9, 157–79.
Caf, B., Kroflic, B., and Tancig, S. (1997). Activation of hypoactive children with creative movement and dance in primary school. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 24, 355–65.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1,243–49.
Calvo-Merino, B., Jola, C., Glaser, D. E., and Haggard, P. (2008). Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 911–22.
Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J., and Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21, 1,363–68.
Chandler, M. J. (1973). Egocentrism and antisocial behavior: The assessment and training of social perspective-taking skills. Developmental Psychology, 9, 326–32.
Chandler, M. J., Greenspan, S., and Barenboim, C. (1974). Assessment and training of role-taking and referential communication skills in institutionalized emotionally disturbed children. Developmental Psychology, 10, 546.
Çokadar, H., and Yilmaz, G. C. (2010). Teaching ecosystems and matter cycles with creative drama activities. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19, 80–89.
Compton, C. (2008). Thank you, Miss Katherine. Phi Delta Kappan, 90, 4.
Cook, A. (2007). Interplay: The method and potential of a cognitive scientific approach to theatre. Theatre Journal-Washington, 59, 579.
Cross, E. S., de Hamilton, A. F. C., and Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage, 31, 1,257–67.
Cross, E. S., Kirsch, L., Ticini, L. F., and Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 102.
Dabbs, J. M., de la Rue, D., and Williams, P. M. (1990). Testosterone and occupational choice: Actors, ministers, and other men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1,261–65.
Diderot, D. (1883). The paradox of acting. London: Chatto & Windus.
Dow, A. W., Leong, D., Anderson, A., Wenzel, R. P., and VCU Theater-Medicine Team. (2007). Using theater to teach clinical empathy: A pilot study. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22, 1,114–18.
Dumais, S. A. (2006). Elementary school students’ extracurricular activities: The effects of participation on achievement and teachers’ evaluations. Sociological Spectrum, 26, 117–47.
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Empathy and sympathy. Handbook of Emotions, 43, 677–91.
Etnier, J. L., Nowell, P. M., Landers, D. M., and Sibley, B. A. (2006). A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Research Reviews, 52, 119–30.
Eversmann, P. (2004). The experience of the theatrical event. In V. A. Cremona, P. Eversmann, H. van Maanen, W. Sauter and J. Tulloch/IFTR/FIRT Theatrical Event Working Group (Eds.), Theatrical Events: Borders Dynamics Frames (pp. 139–74). Amsterdam: Rodopi.
Fink, A., and Woschnjak, S. (2011). Creativity and personality in professional dancers. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 754–58.
Fink, A., Graif, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain correlates underlying creative thinking: EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers. NeuroImage, 46, 854–62.
Fisher, S., and Fisher, R. L. (1981). Pretend the world is funny and forever: A psychological analysis of comedians, clowns, and actors. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fleming, M., Merrell, C., and Tymms, P. (2004). The impact of drama on pupils’ language, mathematics, and attitude in two primary schools. Research in Drama Education, 9, 177–97.
Freeman, G. D., Sullivan, K., and Fulton, C. R. (2003). Effects of creative drama on self-concept, social skills, and problem behavior. The Journal of Educational Research, 96, 131–38.
Goldstein, T. R. (2011). Correlations among social-cognitive skills in adolescents involved in acting or arts classes. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5, 97–103.
Goldstein, T. R., and Winner, E. (2009). Living in alternative and inner worlds: Early signs of acting talent. Creativity Research Journal, 21, 117–24.
Goldstein, T. R., Wu, K., andWinner, E. (2009). Actors are skilled in theory of mind but not empathy. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29, 115–33.
Goldstein, T. R., and Tamir, M. (2010). When emotions are tools of the trade: Emotion regulation in actors. Poster presented at the Association for Psychological Science Convention, Boston, MA.
Goldstein, T. R., and Winner, E. (2010). Engagement in role play, pretense and acting classes predict advanced theory of mind skill in middle childhood. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 30, 249–58.
Goldstein, T. R., and Bloom, P. (2011). The mind on stage: Why cognitive scientists should study acting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 141–42.
Goldstein, T. R., and Winner, E. (2012a). Enhancing empathy and theory of mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 19–37.
Goldstein, T. R., and Winner, E. (2012b). Sympathy for a character’s plight: Sex differences in response to theatre. Empirical Studies in the Arts, 30, 129–41.
Goodill, S. (2005). An introduction to medical dance/movement therapy: Health care in motion. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Gordon, R. (2006). The purpose of playing: Modern acting theories in perspective. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Grady, S. (1999). Asking the audience: Talking to children about representation in children. Youth Theatre Journal, 13, 82–92.
Greer-Paglia, K. (2006). Examining the effects of creative dance on social competence in children with autism: A hierarchical linear growth modeling approach. Thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Gross, J. J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press.
Hammond, J., and Edelmann, R. J. (1991). The act of being: Personality characteristics of professional actors, amateur actors and non-actors. In G. Wilson (Ed.), Psychology and performing arts (pp. 123–131). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger Publishers.
Hanes, D. P., and Schall, J. D. (1996). Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science, 274, 427.
Hanna, J. L. (1987). To dance is human: A theory of nonverbal communication. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Hannah, M. T., Domino, G., Hanson, R., and Hannah, W. (1994). Acting and personality change: The measurement of change in self-perceived personality characteristics during the actor’s character development process. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 277–86.
Harris, P. L., Kavanaugh, R. D., Wellman, H. M., and Hickling, A. K. (1993). Young children’s understanding of pretense. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, v–92.
Harvey, M. L., and Miles, D. (2009). And then they came for me: The effectiveness of a theatrical performance and study guide on middle-school students’ holocaust knowledge and empathic concern. Youth Theatre Journal, 23, 91–102.
Heide, F. J., Porter, N., and Saito, P. K. (2012). Do you hear the people sing? Musical theatre and attitude change. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 224–30.
Hetland, L., Winner, E., Veenema, S., and Sheridan, K. M. (2007). Studio thinking: The real benefits of arts education. New York: Teachers College Press.
Hui, A., and Lau, S. (2006). Drama education: A touch of the creative mind and communicative-expressive ability of elementary school children in Hong Kong. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 1, 34–40.
Hull, S. L. (1985). Strasberg’s method as taught by Lorrie Hull: A practical guide for actors, teachers, and directors. Oakville, CT: Ox Bow Publishers.
Johnson, D. R., and Emunah, R. (2009). Current approaches in drama therapy. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher Ltd.
Jones, P. (2010). Drama as therapy: Clinical work and research into practice. New York: Routledge.
Kalliopuska, M. (1989). Empathy, self-esteem and creativity among junior ballet dancers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 1,227–34.
Kardash, C. A. M., and Wright, L. (1987). Does creative drama benefit elementary school students? A meta-analysis. Youth Theatre Journal, 1, 11–18.
Keinänen, M., Hetland, L., and Winner, E. (2000). Teaching cognitive skill through dance: Evidence for near but not far transfer. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 295–306.
Konijn, E. (1999). Spotlight on spectators: Emotions in the theater. Discourse Processes, 28, 169–94.
Konijn, E. (2000). Acting emotions: Shaping emotions on stage. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
Koshland, L., Wilson, J., and Wittaker, B. (2004). PEACE through dance/movement: Evaluating a violence prevention program. American Journal of Dance Therapy, 26(2), 69–90.
Landy, R. (2000). Role theory and the role method of drama therapy. In D. Johnson and R. Emunah (Eds.), Current Approaches in Drama Therapy, pp. 50–69. Spring-field, IL: Charles Thomas.
Larson, R. W., and Brown, J. R. (2007). Emotional development in adolescence: What can be learned from a high school theater program?Child Development, 78, 1,083–99.
Levy, J. (1997). Theatre and moral education. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 31, 65–75.
Lobo, Y. B., and Winsler, A. (2006). The effects of a creative dance and movement program on the social competence of head start preschoolers. Social Development, 15, 501–19.
Loman, S. (2005). Dance/movement therapy. Expressive Therapies, 68–89.
Marchant-Haycox, S. E., and Wilson, G. D. (1992). Personality and stress in performing artists. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1,061–68.
Marinovic, Z. M., and Carbonell, E. (2000). A psychological study of Chilean actors views of their art. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 27, 245–61.
McConachie, B. A., and Hart, F. E. (2006). Performance and cognition: Theatre studies and the cognitive turn. New York: Routledge.
McKenzie, J., and DaCosta, C. (1999). Openness-to-Experience as a descriptor of the student actress personality. Research in Drama Education, 4, 215–25.
Meekums, B. (2002). Dance movement therapy: A creative psychotherapeutic approach. CA: Sage Publications Ltd.
Metz-Lutz, M. N., Bressan, Y., Heider, N., and Otzenberger, H. (2010). What physiological changes and cerebral traces tell us about adhesion to fiction during theater-watching?Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 59.
Minton, S. (2003). Assessment of high school students’ creative thinking skills: A comparison of dance and nondance classes. Research in Dance Education, 4, 31–49.
Nettle, D. (2006). Psychological profiles of professional actors. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 375–83.
Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1,002–5.
Nieminen, P. (2000). Stereotypes and self-ratings among Finnish dancers. Research in Dance Education, 1, 133–54.
Noice, H. (1991). The role of explanations and plan recognition in the learning of theatrical scripts. Cognitive Science, 15, 425–60.
Noice, H. (1993). Effects of rote versus gist strategy on the verbatim retention of theatrical scripts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 75–84.
Noice, H., and Noice, T. (1999). Long-term retention of theatrical roles. Memory, 7, 357–82.
Noice, H., Noice, T., Perrig-Chiello, P., and Perrig, W. (1999). Improving memory in older adults by instructing them in professional actors’ learning strategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 315–28.
Noice, H., Noice, T., and Kennedy, C. (2000). Effects of enactment by professional actors at encoding and retrieval. Memory, 8, 353–63.
Noice, H., and Noice, T. (2001). Learning dialogue with and without movement. Memory and Cognition, 29, 820.
Noice, H., and Noice, T. (2006). What studies of actors and acting can tell us about memory and cognitive functioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 14–18.
Noice, T., and Noice, H. (1997). The nature of expertise in professional acting: A cognitive view. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Noice, T., and Noice, H. (2002). The expertise of professional actors: A review of recent research. High Ability Studies, 13(1), 7–19.
Noice, T., and Noice, H. (2011). Enhancing healthy cognitive aging through theater arts. In P. E. Hartman-Stein and A. LaRue (Eds.), Enhancing Cognitive Fitness in Adults (pp. 273–83). New York: Springer.
Olivier, L. (1986). On acting. London: Simon & Schuster.
Omasta, M. (2011). Artist intention and audience reception in theatre for young audiences. Youth Theatre Journal, 25, 32–50.
Pincus-Roth, Z. (2007). Ask Playbill.com Chorus Salary. Playbill.com. Retrieved on January 24, 2013, from www.playbill.com/features/article/110695-ASK-PLAYBILL-COM-Chorus-Salary
Podlozny, A. (2000). Strengthening verbal skills through the use of classroom drama: A clear link. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 239–75.
Reber, R., and Sherrill, C. (1981). Creative thinking and dance/movement skills of hearing-impaired youth: An experimental study. American Annals of the Deaf, 126, 1,004–9.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 511–14.
Solomon, R., Solomon, J., Micheli, L. J., Saunders, J. J., and Zurakowski, D. (2001). A personality profile of professional and conservatory student dancers. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 16, 85–93.
Stevens, C. J., Schubert, E., Morris, R. H., Frear, M., Chen, J., Healey, S., . . . Hansen, S. (2009). Cognition and the temporal arts: Investigating audience response to dance using pdas that record continuous data during live performance. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 67, 800–13.
Taft, R. (1955). The ability to judge people. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 1–23.
Taylor, L. D. (1997). MMPI-2 and ballet majors. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 521–26.
Taylor, M. (2001). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. New York: Oxford University Press.
Timmons, W. M. (1945). Personality changes from acting in a play. The Journal of Social Psychology, 21, 247–55.
Vaughn, K., and Winner, E. (2000). SAT scores of students who study the arts: What we can and cannot conclude about the association. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 77–89.
von Rossberg-Gempton, I. E., Dickinson, J., and Poole, G. (1999). Creative dance: Potentiality for enhancing social functioning in frail seniors and young children. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 26, 313–27.
Warger, C. L., and Kleman, D. (1986). Developing positive self-concepts in institutionalized children with severe behavior disorders. Child Welfare: Journal of Policy, Practice, and Program, 65, 165–76.
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–28.
Winner, E., Goldstein, T. R., and Vincent-Lancrin, S. (2012). The impact of arts education: What do we know?Paris, France: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.
Yeh, Y. C., and Li, M. L. (2008). Age, emotion regulation strategies, temperament, creative drama, and preschoolers’ creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 131–48.
Young, L. N., Winner, E., and Cordes, S. (2012, November 12). Heightened incidence of depressive symptoms in adolescents involved in the arts. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication.
Zentner, M., and Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academcy of Sciences, 107, 5,768–73.

References

Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., and Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 111–30.
Atterbury, B. W. (1985). Musical differences in learning-disabled and normal-achieving readers, aged seven, eight and nine. Psychology of Music, 13, 114–23.
Bardes, C. L., Gillers, D., and Herman, A. E. (2001). Learning to look: Developing clinical observational skills at an art museum. Medical Education, 35, 1,157–61.
Barwick, J., Valentine, E., West, R., and Wilding, J. (1989). Relations between reading and musical abilities. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 253–57.
Besson, M., Schon, D., Moreno, S., Santos, A., and Magne, C. (2007). Influence of musical expertise and musical training on pitch processing in music and language. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25, 399–410.
Bialystok, E., and DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 565–74.
Bilhartz, T. D., Bruhn, R. A., and Olson, J. E. (2000). The effect of early music training on child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, 615–36.
Brandler, S., and Rammsayer, T. H. (2003). Differences in mental abilities between musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 31, 123–38.
Brochard, R., Dufour, A., and Deprés, O. (2004). Effect of musical expertise on visuospatial abilities: Evidence from reaction times and mental imagery. Brain and Cognition, 54, 103–9.
Burger, K., and Winner, E. (2000). Instruction in visual art: Can it help reading skills?Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 277–93.
Butzlaff, R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading?Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 167–78.
Calabrese, L., and Marucci, F. (2006). The influence of expertise level on the visuo-spatial ability: Differences between experts and novices in imagery and drawing abilities. Cognitive Processing, 7, S118–S120.
Catterall, J., Chapleau, R., and Iwanaga, J. (1999). Involvement in the arts and human development: General involvement and intensive involvement in music and theatre arts. In E. Fiske (Ed.),Champions of change: The impact of the arts on learning (pp. 1–18). Available from The Kennedy Center ArtsEdge website: http://artsedge.kennedy-center.org/champions/pdfs/ChampsReport.pdf
Ceci, S. (2001, July/August). Intelligence: The surprising truth. Psychology Today, 48, 50, 52–53.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2003). Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 319–38.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and intellectual competence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., and Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory. Nature, 396, 128.
Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., and Gandour, J. T. (2009). Relative influence of musical and linguistic experience on early cortical processing of pitch contours. Brain and Language, 108, 1–9.
Cheek, J. M., and Smith, L. R. (1999). Music training and mathematics achievement. Adolescence, 34, 759–61.
Conard, F. (1992). The arts in education and a meta-analysis. Doctoral dissertation. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (Order no. 9229100).
Corenblum, B., and Marshall, E. (1998). The band played on: Predicting students’ intentions to continue studying music. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 128–40.
Corrigall, K. A., and Trainor, L. J. (2009). Effects of musical training on key and harmony perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1,169, 164–68.
Corrigall, K. A., and Trainor, L. J. (2011). Associations between length of music training and reading skills in children. Music Perception, 29, 147–55.
Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., and Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality,Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222.
Costa-Giomi, E. (1999). The effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s cognitive development. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 198–212.
Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychology of Music, 32, 139–52.
Dankovičová, J., House, J., Crooks, A., and Jones, K. (2007). The relationship between musical skills, music training, and intonation analysis skills. Language and Speech, 50, 177–225.
de Fruyt, F., and Mervielde, I. (1996). Personality and interests as predictors of educational streaming and achievement. European Journal of Personality, 10, 405–25.
de la Cruz, R. E., Lian, M.-C. J., and Morreau, L. E. (1998). The effects of creative drama on social and oral language skills of children with learning disabilities. Youth Theatre Journal, 12, 89–95.
Deary, I. J. (2001). Intelligence: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Degé, F., Kubicek, C., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). Music lessons and intelligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 195–201.
Degé, F., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 124.
Dolev, J. C., Friedlaender, L. K., and Braverman, I. M. (2001). Use of fine art to enhance visual diagnostic skills. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 1,020–21.
Dollinger, S. J., and Orff, L. A. (1991). Personality and performance in “personality”: Conscientiousness and openness. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 276–84.
Douglas, S. and Willatts, P. (1994). The relationship between musical ability and literacy skills. Journal of Research in Reading, 17, 99–107.
DuPont, S. (1992). The effectiveness of creative drama as an instructional strategy to enhance the reading comprehension skills of fifth-grade remedial readers. Reading Research and Instruction, 31, 41–52.
Fitzpatrick, K. R. (2006). The effect of instrumental music participation and socioeconomic status on Ohio fourth-, sixth-, and ninth-grade proficiency test performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54, 73–84.
Forgeard, M., Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Rosam, C., Iyengar, U., and Winner, E. (2008). The relation between music and phonological processing in normal-reading children and children with dyslexia. Music Perception, 25, 383–90.
Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Norton, A., and Schlaug, G. (2008). Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE, 3(10), e3566.
Frakes, L. (1985). Differences in music achievement, academic achievement, and attitude among participants, dropouts, and nonparticipants in secondary school music. Dissertation Abstracts International, 46, 370A. University Microfilms No. AAC8507938.
Franklin, M. S., Moore, K. S., Yip, C., Jonides, J., Rattray, K., and Moher, J. (2008). The effects of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology of Music, 36, 353–65.
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., and Pantev, C. (2004). Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1,010–21.
Gardiner, M., Fox, A., Knowles, F., and Jeffry, D. (1996). Learning improved by arts training. Nature, 381, 284.
Goswami, U., Huss, M., Mead, N., Fosker, T., and Verney, J. P. (in press). Perception of patterns of musical beat distribution in phonological developmental dyslexia: Significant longitudinal relations with word reading and reading comprehension. Cortex.
Gouzouasis, P., Guhn, M., and Kishor, N. (2007) The predictive relationship between achievement and participation in music and achievement in core Grade 12 academic subjects. Music Education Research, 9, 81–92.
Graziano, A. B., Peterson, M., and Shaw, G. L. (1999). Enhanced learning of proportional math through music training and spatial-temporal training. Neurological Research, 21, 139–52.
Gromko, J. E. (2005). The effect of music instruction on phonemic awareness in beginning readers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 53, 199–209.
Gromko, J. E., and Poorman, A. S. (1998). Developmental trends and relationships in children’s aural perception and symbol use. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 16–23.
Gruhn, W., Galley, N., and Kluth, C. (2003). Do mental speed and musical abilities interact?Annals of the New York Academy of the Sciences, 999, 485–96.
Haanstra, F. (1996). Effects of art education on visual-spatial ability and aesthetic perception: A quantitative review. Studies in Art Education, 37, 197–209.
Haanstra, F. (2000). Dutch studies of the effects of arts education programs on school success. Studies in Art Education, 42, 20–35.
Hannon, E. E., and Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 466–72.
Hansen, M., Wallentin, M., and Vuust, P. (2012, August 1). Working memory and musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music. Advance online publication.
Hassler, M., Birbaumer, N., and Feil, A. (1985). Musical talent and visuo-spatial abilities: Longitudinal study. Psychology of Music, 13, 99–113.
Helmbold, N., Rammsayer, T., and Altenmüller, E. (2005). Differences in primary mental abilities between musicians and nonmusicians. Journal of Individual Differences, 26, 74–85.
Hetland, L. (2000). Learning to make music enhances spatial reasoning. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 179–238.
Ho, Y., Cheung, M., and Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology, 17, 439–50.
Hurwitz, I., Wolff, P. H., Bortnick, B. D., and Kokas, K. (1975). Nonmusical effects of the Kodály music curriculum in primary grade children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 8, 167–74.
Huss, M., Verney, J. P., Fosker, T., Mead, N., and Goswami, U. (2011). Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: Perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex, 47, 674–89.
Jakobson, L. S., Lewycky, S. T., Kilgour, A. R., and Stoesz, B. M. (2008). Memory for verbal and visual material in highly trained musicians. Music Perception, 26, 41–55.
Jentschke, S., and Koelsch, S. (2009). Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children. NeuroImage, 47, 735–44.
Kardash, C., and Wright, L. (1987). Does creative drama benefit elementary school students? A meta-analysis. Youth Theatre Journal, 1, 11–18.
Keinänen, M., Hetland, L., and Winner, E. (2000). Teaching cognitive skill through dance: Evidence for near but not far transfer. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 295–306.
Kilgour, A. R., Jakobson, L. S., and Cuddy, L. L. (2000). Music training and rate of presentation as mediators of text and song recall. Memory and Cognition, 28, 700–10.
Kinney, D. W. (2008). Selected demographic variables, school music participation, and achievement test scores of urban middle school students,Journal of Research in Music Education, 56, 145–61.
Kinney, D. W. (2010). Selected nonmusic predictors of urban students’ decisions to enroll and persist in middle school band programs. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57, 334–50.
Klinedinst, R. E. (1991). Predicting performance achievement and retention of fifth-grade instrumental students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39, 225–38.
Koelsch, S., Schroeger, E., and Tervaniemi, M. (1999). Superior pre-attentive auditory processing in musicians. NeuroReport, 10, 1,309–13.
Kolinsky, R., Cuvelier, H., Goetry, V., Peretz, I., and Morais, J. (2009) Music training facilitates lexical stress processing. Music Perception, 26, 235–46.
Kozbelt, A. (2001). Artists as experts in visual cognition. Visual Cognition, 8, 705–23.
Lamb, S. J., and Gregory, A. H. (1993). The relationship between music and reading in beginning readers. Educational Psychology, 13, 19–27.
Loui, P., Kroog, K., Zuk, J., Winner, E., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Relating pitch awareness to phonemic awareness in children: Implications for tone-deafness and dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 111.
MacMahon, S. D., Rose, D. S., and Parks, M. (2003). Basic reading through dance program: The impact on first-grade students’ basic reading skills. Evaluation Review, 27, 104–25.
Mages, W. K. (2008). Does creative drama promote language development in early childhood? A review of the methods and measures employed in the empirical literature. Review of Educational Research, 78, 124–52.
Magne, C., Schon, D., and Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: Behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 199–211.
Marques, C., Moreno, S., Castro, S. L., and Besson, M. (2007). Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than non-musicians: Behavioral and electro-physiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 1,453–63.
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 525, 509–16.
Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., and Besson, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: More evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 712–23.
Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, G., Cepeda, N. and Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22, 1,425–33.
Moreno, S., Friesen, D., and Bialystok, E. (2011). Effect of music training on promoting preliteracy skills: Preliminary causal evidence. Music Perception, 29, 165–72.
O’Connor, M. C., and Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big Five personality predictors of postsecondary academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 971–90.
Orsmond, G. I., and Miller, L. K. (1999). Cognitive, musical and environmental correlates of early music instruction. Psychology of Music, 27, 18–37.
Overy, K. (2003). Dyslexia and music: From timing deficits to musical intervention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 497–505.
Overy, K., Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., and Clarke, E. F. (2003). Dyslexia and music: Measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia, 9, 18–36.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., and Kraus, N. (2009). Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 14,100–7.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., and Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech in noise,Ear and Hearing, 30, 653–61.
Patston, L. L. M., Hogg, S. L., and Tippett, L. J. (2007). Attention in musicians is more bilateral than in non-musicians. Laterality, 12, 262–72.
Patston, L. L. M., and Tippett, L. J. (2011). The effect of background music on cognitive performance in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 29, 173–83.
Pellegrini, A. D. (1980). The relationship between kindergartners’ play and achievement in prereading, language, and writing. Psychology in the Schools, 17, 530–35.
Pellegrini, A. D., and Galda, L. (1982). The effects of thematic-fantasy play training on the development of children’s story comprehension. American Educational Research Journal, 19, 443–52.
Pérez-Fabello, M. J., and Campos, A. (2007). Influence of training in artistic skills on mental imaging capacity. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 227–32.
Petitto, L. (2008). Arts education, the brain, and language. In B. Rich and C. Asbury (Eds.),Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana Consortium report on arts and cognition (pp. 93–104). New York/Washington, D.C.: The Dana Foundation.
Piro, J. M., and Oritz, C. (2009). The effect of piano lessons on the vocabulary and verbal sequencing skills of primary grade students. Psychology of Music, 37, 325–47.
Podlozny, A. (2000). Strengthening verbal skills through the use of classroom drama: A clear link. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 239–75.
Posedel, J., Emery, L., Souza, B., and Fountain, C. (2012). Pitch perception, working memory, and second-language phonological production. Psychology of Music, 40, 508–17.
Posner, M., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., and Kieras, J. (2008). How arts training influences cognition. In B. Rich and C. Asbury (Eds.), Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana Consortium report on arts and cognition (pp. 1–10). New York/ Washington, D.C.: The Dana Foundation.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., Wright, E. L., Dennis, W. R., and Newcomb, R. L. (1997). Music training causes long-term enhancement of preschool children’s spatial-temporal reasoning. Neurological Reasoning, 19, 1–8.
Rauscher, F. H., and Zupan, M. A. (1999). Classroom keyboard instruction improves kindergarten children’s spatial-temporal performance: A field experiment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 215–28.
Rauscher, F. H., and Hinton, S. C. (2011). Music instruction and its diverse extra-musical benefits. Music Perception, 29, 215–26.
Rose, D. S., Parks, M., Androes, K., and McMahon, S. D. (2000). Imagery-based learning: Improving elementary students’ reading comprehension with drama techniques. Journal of Educational Research, 94, 55–63.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccamanno, L., and Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 14,931–36.
Salthouse, T. A. (2005). Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 532–45.
Saltz, E., and Johnson, J. (1974). Training for thematic-fantasy play in culturally disadvantaged children: Preliminary results. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 623–30.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 511–14.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 317–20.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 457–68.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2011a). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 283–302.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2011b). Music lessons, emotional intelligence, and IQ. Music Perception, 29, 189–94.
Schellenberg, E. G., and Peretz, I. (2008). Music, language and cognition: Unresolved issues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 45–46.
Schellenberg, E. G., and Moreno, S. (2010). Music lessons, pitch processing and g. Psychology of Music, 38, 209–21.
Schellenberg, E. G., and Mankarious, M. (2012). Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood. Emotion, 12, 887–91.
Schon, D., Magne, C., and Besson, M. (2004). The music of speech: Music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology, 41, 341–49.
Shahin, A., Bosnyak, D. J., Trainor, L. J., and Roberts, L. E. (2003). Enhancement of neuroplastic P2 and N1c auditory evoked potentials in musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5,545–52.
Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–53.
Sluming, V., Barrick, T., Howard, M., Cezayirli, E., Mayes, A., and Roberts, N. (2002). Voxel-based morphometry reveals increased gray matter density in Broca’s area in male symphony orchestra musicians. Neuroimage, 17, 1,613–22.
Sluming, V., Brooks, J., Howard, M., Downes, J. J., and Roberts, N. (2007). Broca’s area supports enhanced visuospatial cognition in orchestral musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3,799–806.
Spelke, E. (2008). Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive systems at the foundation of mathematics and science. In B. Rich and C. Asbury (Eds.), Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana Consortium report on arts and cognition (pp.17–49). New York/Washington, D.C.: The Dana Foundation.
Standley, J. M. (2008). Does music instruction help children learn to read? Evidence of a meta-analysis. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 27, 17–32.
Stoesz, B., Jakobson, L., Kilgour, A., and Lewycky, S. (2007). Local processing advantage in musicians: Evidence from disembedding and constructional tasks. Music Perception, 25, 153–65.
Strait, D. L., Kraus, N., Parbery-Clark, A., and Ashley, R. (2010). Musical experience shapes top-down auditory mechanisms: Evidence from masking and auditory attention performance. Hearing Research, 261, 22–29.
Strait, D. L., and Kraus, N. (2011). Can you hear me now? Musical training shapes functional brain networks for selective auditory attention and hearing speech in noise. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 113.
Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E.G., and Husain, G. (2004). Decoding speech prosody: Do music lessons help?Emotion, 4, 46–64.
Tishman, S., MacGillivray, D., and Palmer, P. (1999). Investigating the educational impact and potential of the Museum of Modern Art’s Visual Thinking Curriculum: Final report (Unpublished manuscript). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Project Zero.
Trimmer, C. G., and Cuddy, L. L. (2008). Emotional intelligence, not music training, predicts recognition of emotional speech prosody. Emotion, 8, 838.
Vaughn, K. (2000). Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 149–66.
Vaughn, K., and Winner, E. (2000). SAT scores of students who study the arts: What we can and cannot conclude about the association. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 77–89.
Virtala, P., Huotilainen, M., Putkinen, V., Makkonen, T., and Tervaniemi, M. (2012). Musical training facilitates the neural discrimination of major versus minor chords in 13-year-old children. Psychophysiology, 49, 1,125–32.
Vitz, K. (1983). A review of empirical research in drama and language. Children’s Theatre Review, 32, 17–25.
Walker, C. M., Winner, E., Hetland, L., Simmons, S., and Goldsmith, L. (2010). Visual thinking: Art students have an advantage in geometric reasoning. Creative Education, 2, 22–26.
Wandell, B., Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., and Deutsch, G. K. (2008). Training in the arts, reading, and brain imaging. In B. Rich and C. Asbury (Eds.), Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana Consortium report on arts and cognition (pp. 51–60). New York/Washington, D.C.: The Dana Foundation.
Wetter, O. E., Koerner, F., and Schwaninger, A. (2009). Does musical training improve school performance?Journal of Instructional Science, 37, 365–74.
White, K. R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–81.
Winner, E., Casey, M. B., DaSilva, D., and Hayes, R. (1991). Spatial abilities and reading deficits in visual art students. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 9, 51–63.
Winner, E., and Cooper, M. (2000). Mute those claims: No evidence (yet) for a causal link between arts study and academic achievement. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 11–75.
Winner, E., Goldstein, T. R., and Vincent-Lancrin, S. (2013). The impact of arts education: What do we know?Paris: Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development.
Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., and Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 420–22.

References

Addy, C. L., Wilson, D. K., Kirtland, K. A., Ainsworth, B. E., Sharpe, P., and Kimsey, D. (2004). Associations of perceived social and physical environmental supports with physical activity and walking behavior. American Journal of Public Health, 94(3), 440–43.
Aiello, J. R., and Thomson, D. A. (1980). Personal space, crowding, and spatial behavior in a cultural context. In I. Altman, A. Rapoport and J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and environment: Advances in theory and research, Vol. IV:Environment and Culture. New York: Plenum Press.
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Altman, I., and Zube, E. H. (Eds.) (1989). Public places and spaces. Human behavior in environment: Advances in theory and research, Vol. X. New York: Plenum Press.
Anonymous. (1956, July 2). Art: The maturing modern. Time, pp. 50–57
Appleyard, D. (1981). Liveable streets. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Appleyard, D., LynchK., and Myer, J. (1964). The view from the road. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Badland, H., and Schofield, G. (2005). Transport, urban design, and physical activity: An evidence-based update. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, 10(3), 177–96.
Badoe, D. A., and Miller, E. J. (2000). Transportation–land-use interaction: Empirical findings in North America and their implications for modeling. Transportation Research, Part D: Transport and Environment, 5(4), 235–63.
Ball, K., Bauman, A., Leslie, E., and Owen, N. (2001). Perceived environmental aesthetics and convenience and company are associated with walking for exercise among Australian adults. Preventive Medicine, 33(5), 434–40.
Barthes, R. (1983). A Barthes reader. New York: Hill and Wang.
Bauman, A., Sallis, J., Dzewaltowski, D. A., and Owen, N. (2002). Toward a better understanding of the influences on physical activity: The role of determinants, correlates, causal variables, mediators, moderators, and confounders. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23(2S), 5–14.
Bingham, N., Carolin, C., Cook, P., and Wilson, R. (2004). Fantasy architecture 1500–2036. London: Hayward Gallery Publishing.
Brower, S. (1980). Territory in urban settings. In I. Altman, A. Rapoport and J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and environment: Advances in theory and research, Vol. IV: Environment and CultureNew York: Plenum Press.
Brüggen, E., Foubert, B., and Gremler, D. (2011). Extreme makeover: Short- and long-term effects of a remodeled servicescape. Journal of Marketing, 75, 71–87.
Callebat, L. (1998). “Architecte”: histoire d’un mot. In L. Callebat (Ed.), Histoire de l’architecte. Paris: Flammarion.
Camic, P. (2010). From trashed to treasured: A grounded theory analysis of the found object. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(2), 81–92.
Carr, S., Francis, M., Rivlin, L. G., and Stone, A. M. (1992). Public space. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Caspari, S., Eriksson, K., and Nåden, D. (2011). The importance of aesthetic surroundings: A study interviewing experts within different aesthetic fields. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 25(1), 134–42.
Coburn, T., and McCain, J. (2009). Out of gas: Congress raids the highway trust fund for pet projects while bridges and roads crumble. Washington, D.C.: United States Senate.
Crane, R. (2000). The influence of urban form on travel: An interpretive review. Journal of Planning Literature, 15(1), 3–24.
Cronon, W. (1983). Changes in the land: Indians, colonists, and the ecology of New England. New York: Hill and Wang.
Curtis, W. (1996). Modern architecture since 1900 (3rd edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Davis, B. (2011). Rooftop hospital gardens for physical therapy: A post-occupancy evaluation. Health Environments Research and Design Journal, 4(2), 14–43.
Davis, H. (1999). The culture of building. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Davis, M. (1998). Ecology of fear: Los Angeles and the imagination of disaster. New York: Vintage Books.
De Botton, A. (2006). The architecture of happiness. London: Penguin Books.
Dickinson, E. (2008, December 10). The art of energy. Metropolis Magazine.
Eco, U. (1986). Function and sign: Semiotics of architecture. In M. Gottdiener and A. Lagopulos (Eds.),The city and the sign. New York: Columbia University Press.
Ewing, R., Handy, S., Brownson, R. C., Clemente, O., and Winston, E. (2006). Identifying and measuring urban design qualities related to walkability. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 3(Suppl 1), S223–S240.
Faludi, A. (Ed.) (1973). A reader in planning theory. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Fleming, R. L. (2007). The art of placemaking: Interpreting community through public art and urban design. London: Merrell.
Flynn, J. E. (1988). Lighting-design decisions as interventions in human visual space. In J. L. Nasar (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Foderaro, L. W. (2011, October 18). Privately owned park, open to the public, may make its own rules. The New York Times.
Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (2010). Global forest resources assessment 2010. Rome: United Nations.
Frampton, K. (1992). Modern architecture: A critical history (3rd edition). London: Thames & Hudson.
Frank, L. D., Schmid, T. L., Sallis, J. F., Chapman, J., and Saelens, B. E. (2005). Linking objectively measured physical activity with objectively measured urban form. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28(2), 117–25.
Goldberger, P. (2009). Why architecture matters. New Haven, CN: Yale University Press.
Gordon, R. B., and Malone, P. M. (1994). The texture of industry: An archaeological view of the industrialization of North America. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Groat, L. N. (1988). Contextual compatibility in architecture: An issue of personal taste? In J. L. Nasar (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Haber, R.N., and Hershenson, M. (1973). The psychology of visual perception. New York: Holt, Reinhart and Winston, Inc.
Handy, S. L., Boarnet, M. G., Ewing, R., and Killingsworth, R. E. (2002). How the built environment affects physical activity: Views from urban planning. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23(2 Suppl), 64–73.
Heft, H., and Nasar, J. (2000). Evaluating environmental scenes using dynamic versus static displays. Environment and Behavior, 32(3), 301–22.
Herman, J. F., Miller, B. S., and Shiraki, J. H. (1995). The influence of affective associations on the development of cognitive maps of large environments. In T. G ärling (Ed.), Readings in environmental psychology: Urban cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hershberger, R. G. (1988). A study of meaning and architecture. In J. L. Nasar (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heynen, H. (1999). Architecture and modernity: A critique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hiss, T. (1990). The experience of place. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Hoehner, C., Brennan Ramirez, L., Elliott, M., Handy, S., and Brownson, R. (2005). Perceived and objective environmental measures and physical activity among urban adults. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28(2s2), 105–16.
Howard, E. (1902). Garden cities of to-morrow. London.
Humpel, N., Marshall, A. L., Leslie, E., Bauman, A., and Owen, N. (2004a). Changes in neighborhood walking are related to changes in perceptions of environmental attributes. Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 27(1), 60–67.
Humpel, N., Owen, N., Iverson, D., Leslie, E., and Bauman, A. (2004b). Perceived environment attributes, residential location, and walking for particular purposes. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 26(2), 119–25.
Jacobs, J. (1961). The death and life of great American cities. New York: Random House.
Jansson-Boyd, C. (2011). Designing aesthetic concepts: Can it be done?Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5(3), 279–90.
Kameron, J. (1973). Experimental studies of environmental perception. In W. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and cognition (pp. 157–67). New York: Seminar Press.
Kaplan, S., and Kaplan, R. (Eds.) (1978). Humanscape: Environments for people. N. Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.
Kitamura, R., Mokhtarian, P. L., and Laidet, L. (1997). A micro-analysis of land-use and travel in five neighbourhoods in the San Francisco Bay Area. Transportation, 24, 125–58.
Kostof, S. (1991). The city shaped: Urban patterns and meanings through history. New York: Bulfinch Press.
Kotzer, A.M., Kock Zacharakis, S., Raynolds, M., and Buenning, F. (2011). Evaluation of the built environment: Staff and family satisfaction pre- and post-occupancy of the children’s hospital. Health Environments Research and Design Journal, 4(4), 60–78.
Krupat, E. (1985). People in cities: The urban environment and its effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kunstler, J. H. (1993). The geography of nowhere: The rise and decline of America’s man-made landscape. New York: Touchtone.
Lang, J. (1988). Symbolic aesthetics in architecture: Towards a research agenda. In J. L. Nasar (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lang, J. (2005). Urban design. In A Typology of Procedures and Products. Amsterdam: Architectural Press.
Lee, C., and Moudon, A. (2004). Physical activity and environment research in the health field: Implications for urban and transportation planning practice and research. Journal of Planning Literature, 19(2), 147–81.
Lee, C., and Moudon, A. (2006). Correlates of walking for transportation or recreation purposes. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 3(1), S99–S117.
Leslie, E., and Cerin, E. (2008). Are perceptions of the local environment related to neighbourhood satisfaction and mental health in adults?Preventive Medicine, 47(3), 273–78.
Linden-Ward, B. (1992). Strange but genteel pleasure grounds: Tourist and leisure uses of nineteenth-century rural cemeteries. In R. E. Meyer (Ed.), Cemeteries and grave-markers: Voices of American culture. Logan: Utah State University Press.
Lindsay, P., and NormanD. A. (1977). Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. New York: Harcourt.
Lindsey, G., Han, Y., Wilson, J., and Yang, J. (2006). Neighborhood correlates of urban trail use. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 3(1), S139–S157.
Lio, H.-L., and Rody, R. (2009). The emotional impact of casino servicescape. UNLV Gaming Research and Review Journal, 13(2), 17–25.
Livi Smith, A. (2009). The contribution of perception in analysis of walking behavior. Transportation Research Record (2,140), 128–36.
Locher, P., Smith, J. K., and Smith, L. F. (2001). The influence of presentation format and viewer training in the visual arts on the perception of pictorial and aesthetic qualities of paintings. Perception (30), 449–65.
Longstreth, R. (2010). The American department store transformed 1920–1960. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Loyer, F. (1987). Paris XIXe siècle. L’immeuble et la rue. Paris: Hazan.
Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: Technology Press and Harvard University Press.
Macnaughton, J. (2007). Art in hospital spaces: The role of hospitals in an aestheticised society. International Journal of Cultural Policy, 13(1), 85–101.
Melnick, M. (1994). Manhole covers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mignot, C. (1983). Architecture of the 19th century. Fribourg: Evergreen.
Nasar, J. L. (Ed.) (1988). Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and application (1st edition; p. 556). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Nasar, J., and Hong, X. (1999). Visual preferences in urban signscapes. Environment and Behavior, 31(5), 671–91.
Nasar, J., and Devlin, A. S. (2000). Regional variation in preferences for vernacular houses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(1), 41–66.
Neumann, D. (1996). Before and after Metropolis: Film and architecture in search of the modern city. In D. Neumann (Ed.), Film architecture. Munich: Prestel.
Owen, N., Humpel, N., Leslie, E., Bauman, A., Sallis, J. (2004). Understanding environmental influences on walking: Review and research agenda. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 27(1), 67–76.
Painter, K. (1996). The influence of street lighting improvements on crime, fear and pedestrian street use, after dark. Landscape and Urban Planning, 35, 193–201.
Peterson, J. A. (1983). The city beautiful movement: Forgotten origins and lost meanings. In D. A. Krueckeberg (Ed.), Introduction to planning history in the United States. New Brunswick: Rutgers Center for Urban Policy Resarch.
Pikora, T., Bull, F. C., Jamrozik, K., Knuiman, M., Giles-Corti, B., and Donovan, R. (2002). Developing a reliable audit instrument to measure the physical environment for physical activity. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23(3), 187–94.
Porteous, J. D. (1977). Environment and behavior: Planning and everyday urban life (p. 446). Reading, MA: Addison Wesley Publishing Company.
Preston Blier, S. (1989). Moral architecture: Beauty and ethics in Batammaliba building design. In J.-P. Bourdier and N. Alsayyad (Eds.), Dwellings, settlements and tradition. Lanham: University Press of America.
Rapoport, A. (1969). House form and culture. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Reps, J. (1980). Town planning in frontier America. Columbia: University of Missouri Press.
Roth, L. (1993). Understanding architecture: Its elements, history, and meaning. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Ryu, K., and Shawn Jang, S. (2008). DINESCAPE: A scale for customers’ perception of dining environments. Journal of Foodservice Business Research, 11(1), 2–22.
Sloane, D. C. (1991). The last great necessity: Cemeteries in American history. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Stamps, A. E. III. (2005). Enclosure and safety in urbanscapes. Environment and Behavior, 37(1), 102–33.
Stein, H. F. (1987). Developmental time, cultural space: Studies in psychogeography. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press.
Stilgoe, J. R. (1998). Outside lies magic: Regaining history and awareness in everyday places. New York: Walker and Company.
Stokols, D., and Altman, I. (Eds.) (1987). Handbook of environmental psychology, Vol. I. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Summerson, J. (1963). The classical language of architecture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Trevisani, F., Casadio, R., Romagnoli, F., Zamagni, M. P., Francesconi, C., Tromellini, A., di Micoli, A., . . . (2010). Art in the hospital: Its impact on the feelings and emotional state of patients admitted to an internal medicine unit. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(8), 853–59.
Tuan, Y.-F. (1967). Attitudes toward environment: Themes and approaches. In D. Low-enthal (Ed.), Environmental perception and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Turley, L. W., and Milliman, R. E. (2000). Atmospheric effects on shopping behavior: A review of the experimental evidence. Journal of Business Research, 49, 193–211.
Turner, P. V. (1996). Joseph Ramée: International architect of the revolutionary era. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tyrnauer, M. (2010, August 13). Architecture in the age of Gehry. Vanity Fair.
Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science, 224, 420–21.
Venturi, R. (1966). Complexity and contradiction in architecture. New York: The Museum of Modern Art Press.
Venturi, R., Scott Brown, D., and Izenour, S. (1977). Learning from Las Vegas. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Viollet-le-Duc, E.-E. (1990). The architectural theory of Viollet-le-Duc (Ed. M. F. Hearn). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Webb, M. (1996). “Like today, only more so”: The credible dystopia of Blade Runner. In D. Neumann (Ed.), Film architecture. Munich: Prestel.
Whyte, W. H. (1980). The social life of small urban spaces. Washington, D.C.: The Conservation Foundation.
Wohlwill, J. (1980). The place of order and uncertainty in art and environmental aesthetics. Motivation and Emotion, 4(2), 133–42.
Yancey, W. L. (1972). Architecture, interaction, and social control: The case of a large-scale housing project. In J. F. Wohlwill and D. H. Carson (Eds.), Environment and the social sciences: Perspectives and applications (pp. 1–11). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

References

Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., and Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioural evidence. Neuron, 32, 537–51.
Augustin, M. D., Wagemans, J., and Carbon, C. C. (2012). All is beautiful? Generality vs. specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychologica, 139, 187–201.
Baker, M. J., and Churchill, G. A. (1977). Impact of physically attractive models on advertising evaluations. Journal of Marketing Research, 14, 538–555. doi: 10.2307/3151194.
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551–65.
Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., and Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: “liking,” “wanting,” and learning. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 9, 65–73.
Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., Hooge, I. T. C., Jenkins, R., and de Haan, E. H. F. (2005). Faces retain attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1,048–53.
Bornstein, R. F., and D’Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attributional account. Social Cognition, 12, 103–28.
Bower, A. B., and Landreth, S. (2001). Is beauty best? Highly versus normally attractive models in advertising. Journal of Advertising, 31, 112–17.
Bray, S., and O’Doherty, J. (2007). Neural coding of reward-prediction error signals during classical conditioning with attractive faces. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 3,036–45.
Bukach, C. M., Gauthier, I., and Tarr, M. J. (2006). Beyond faces and modularity: The power of an expertise framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 159–66.
Burgdorf, J., and Panksepp, J. (2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 173–87.
Buss, D. M., and Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory – An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–32.
Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Caspers, S., Kurth, F., Habel, U., Zilles, K., . . . Eickhoff, S. B. (2011). ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness. Brain Structure and Function, 215, 209–23.
Chatterjee, A. (2003). Prospects for a cognitive neuroscience of visual aesthetics. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 4, 55–60.
Chatterjee, A. (2011). Neuroaesthetics: A coming of age story. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 53–62.
Chatterjee, A., Thomas, A., Smith, S. E., and Aguirre, G. K. (2009). The neural response to facial attractiveness. Neuropsychology, 23, 135–43.
Cloutier, J., Heatherton, T. F., Whalen, P. J., and Kelley, W. M. (2008). Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 941–51.
Conway, C. A., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., and Little, A. C. (2008). Evidence for adaptive design in human gaze preference. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 275(1,630), 63–69.
Conway, C. A., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., and Little, A. C. (2010). Sexual dimorphism of male face shape, partnership status and the temporal context of relationship sought modulate women’s preferences for direct gaze. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 109–21.
Corneille, O., Monin, B., and Pleyers, G. (2005). Is positivity a cue or a response option? Warm glow vs evaluative matching in the familiarity for attractive and not-so-attractive faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 431–37.
Cunningham, M., Barbee, A., and Philhower, C. (2002). Dimensions of facial physical attractiveness: The intersection of biology and culture. InG. Rhodes and L. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives. Westport, CT: Ablex.
Deblieck, C., and Zaidel, D. W. (2003). Hemifield memory for attractiveness. International Journal of Neuroscience, 113, 931–41.
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., and Perrett, D. I. (2008). Social perception of facial resemblance in humans. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 64–77.
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Frederick, D. A., Haselton, M. G., Penton-Voak, I. S., and Perrett, D. I. (2010). Evidence for menstrual cycle shifts in women’s preferences for masculinity: A response to Harris (in press) “Menstrual cycle and facial preferences reconsidered.”Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 768–75.
Dewhurst, S. A., Hay, D. C., and Wickham, L. H. V. (2005). Distinctiveness, typicality, and recollective experience in face recognition: A principal components analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1,032–37.
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–90.
Duncan, L. A., Park, J. H., Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Neuberg, S. L., and Kenrick, D. T. (2007). Adaptive allocation of attention: Effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 359–64.
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., Ashmore, R. D., and Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good. A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109–28.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., and Ancoli, S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1,125–34.
Ekman, P., and Rosenberg, E. L. (1997). What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the facial action coding system (FACS). New York: Oxford University Press.
Enquist, M., and Arak, A. (1994). Symmetry, beauty and evolution. Nature, 372, 169–72.
Enquist, M., Ghirlanda, S., Lundquist, D., and Wachtmeister, C.-A. (2002). An ethological theory of attractiveness. In G. Rhoded and L. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractiveness. Westport, CT: Ablex.
Etcoff, N. (1999). Survival of prettiest. New York: Doubleday.
Ewing, L., Rhodes, G., and Pellicano, E. (2010). Have you got the look? Gaze direction affects judgements of facial attractiveness. Visual Cognition, 18, 321–30.
Finkbeiner, M., and Palermo, R. (2009). The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: A comparison of face and nonface stimuli. Psychological Science, 20, 42–51.
Gangestad, S. W., and Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–87.
Gerger, G., Leder, H., Tinio, P. P. L., and Schacht, A. (2011). Faces versus patterns: Exploring aesthetic reactions using facial EMG. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, 5, 241–50.
Ghirlanda, S., Jansson, L., and Enquist, M. (2002). Chickens prefer beautiful humans. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective, 13, 383–89.
Grammer, K., and Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection – the role of asymmetry and averageness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 233–42.
Haber, S. N., and Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 4–26.
Hajcak, G., Weinberg, A., MacNamara, A., and Foti, D. (2012). ERPs and the study of emotion. In S. J. Luck and E. S. Kappenmann (Eds.), Oxford handbook of ERP components (pp. 441–74). New York: Oxford University Press.
Halberstadt, J., and Rhodes, G. (2003). It’s not just average faces that are attractive: Computer-manipulated averageness makes birds, fish, and automobiles attractive. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 149–56.
Halit, H., de Haan, M., and Johnson, M. H. (2000). Modulation of event-related potentials by prototypical and atypical faces. Neuroreport, 11, 1,871–75.
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223–33.
Hayden, B. Y., Parikh, P. C., Deaner, R. O., and Platt, M. L. (2007). Economic principles motivating social attention in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 274(1,619), 1,751–56.
Höfel, L., and Jacobsen, T. (2007). Electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics – A result of judgment categorization or judgment report?Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 9–21.
Hönekopp, J. (2006). Once more: Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Relative contributions of private and shared taste to judgments of facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 199–209.
Iaria, G., Fox, C. J., Waite, C. T., Aharon, I., and Barton, J. J. S. (2008). The contribution of the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus in processing facial attractiveness: Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence. Neuroscience, 155, 409–22.
Ishai, A. (2007). Sex, beauty and the orbitofrontal cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 181–85.
Jansson, L., Forkman, B., and Enquist, M. (2002). Experimental evidence of receiver bias for symmetry. Animal Behaviour, 63, 617–21.
Johnston, V. S., and Oliver-Rodriguez, J. C. (1997). Facial beauty and the late positive component of event-related potentials. Journal of Sex Research, 34, 188–98.
Johnstone, R. A. (1994). Female preference for symmetrical males as a by-product of selection for mate recognition. Nature, 372, 172–75.
Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health – Support for a “good genes” explanation of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 417–29.
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Conway, C. A., and Feinberg, D. R. (2006). Integrating gaze direction and expression in preferences for attractive faces. Psychological Science, 17, 588–91.
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., and Smith, M. J. L. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 78–84.
Jones, D., and Hill, K. (1993). Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations. Human Nature, 4, 271–96.
Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, U. (2001). Reward value of attractiveness and gaze – Making eye contact enhances the appeal of a pleasing face, irrespective of gender. Nature, 413(6,856), 589–89.
Kanda, N., Tsuchida, T., and Tamaki, K. (1996). Testosterone inhibits immunoglobulin production by human peripheral blood mononuclear cells. Clinical and Experimental Immunology, 106, 410–15.
Kanwisher, N., and Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 361(1,476), 2,109–28.
Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact – A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1).
Korthase, K. M., and Trenholme, I. (1982). Perceived age and perceived physical attractiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 1,251–58.
Kranz, F., and Ishai, A. (2006). Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Current Biology, 16, 63–68.
LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 54–64.
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., Rieserdanner, L. A., and Jenkins, V. Y. (1987). Infants’ preference for attractive faces – Rudiments of a stereotype. Developmental Psychology, 23, 363–69.
Langlois, J. H., and Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science, 1, 115–21.
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., and Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces. Psychological Science, 5, 214–20.
Leder, H., and Bruce, V. (1998). Local and relational aspects of face distinctiveness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. Human Experimental Psychology, 51, 449–73.
Leder, H., Tinio, P. P. L., Fuchs, I. M., and Bohrn, I. (2010). When attractiveness demands longer looks: The effects of situation and gender. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1,858–71.
Leder, H., Forster, M., and Gerger, G. (2011). The glasses stereotype revisited: Effects of eyeglasses on perception, recognition, and impression of faces. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70, 211–22.
Levy, B., Ariely, D., Mazar, N., Chi, W., Lukas, S., and Elman, I. (2008). Gender differences in the motivational processing of facial beauty. Learning and Motivation, 39, 136–45.
Liang, X. Y., Zebrowitz, L. A., and Zhang, Y. (2010). Neural activation in the reward circuit shows a nonlinear response to facial attractiveness. Social Neuroscience, 5, 320–34.
Light, L. L., Hollander, S., and Kayra-Stuart, F. (1981). Why attractive people are harder to remember. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 269–76.
Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 268, 39–44.
Little, A. C., and Jones, B. C. (2006). Attraction independent of detection suggests special mechanisms for symmetry preferences in human face perception. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273, 3,093–99.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2008). Preferences for variation in masculinity in real male faces change across the menstrual cycle: Women prefer more masculine faces when they are more fertile. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 478–82.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1,638–59.
Loffler, G., Yourganov, G., Wilkinson, F., and Wilson, H. R. (2005). fMRI evidence for the neural representation of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1,386–90.
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C. J., and Neuberg, S. L. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: Beauty captures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1,107–20.
Marzi, T., and Viggiano, M. P. (2010). When memory meets beauty: Insights from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 84, 192–205.
Mason, M. F., Tatkow, E. P., and Macrae, C. N. (2005). The look of love – Gaze shifts and person perception. Psychological Science, 16, 236–39.
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Said, C. P., and Todorov, A. (2013). The social evaluation of faces: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.
Miesler, L., Leder, H., and Herrmann, A. (2011). Isn’t it cute: An evolutionary perspective of baby-schema effects in visual product designs. International Journal of Design, 5, 17–30.
Moller, A. P., and Swaddle, J. P. (1997). Asymmetry, developmental stability, and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1,035–48.
Moore, M. M. (2002). Courtship communication and perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 97–105.
Mueser, K. T., Grau, B. W., Sussman, S., and Rosen, A. J. (1984). You’re only as pretty as you feel – Facial expression as a determinant of physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 469–78.
Nadal, M., and Pearce, M. T. (2011). The Copenhagen Neuroaesthetics conference: Prospects and pitfalls for an emerging field. Brain and Cognition, 76, 172–83.
New, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2007). Category specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 104(42), 16,598–603.
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., and Hess, U. (2010). The simulation of smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–33.
O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147–55.
Öhman, A. (2007). Has evolution primed humans to “beware the beast”?Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 16,396–97.
Oliver-Rodriguez, J. C., Guan, Z. Q., and Johnston, V. S. (1999). Gender differences in late positive components evoked by human faces. Psychophysiology, 36, 176–85.
Olson, I. R., and Marshuetz, C. (2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion, 5, 498–502.
Osborn, D. R. (1996). Beauty is as beauty does? Make-up and posture effects on physical attractiveness judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 31–51.
Parkinson, B. (2005). Do facial movements express emotions or communicate motives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 278–311.
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., and Minamisawa, R. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741–42.
Penton-Voak, I. S., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Perceived personality and shifting female preferences for male traits across the menstrual cycle. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 30, 219–59.
Penton-Voak, I. S., and Chang, H. (2008). Attractiveness judgments of individuals vary across emotional expression and movement conditions. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 89–100.
Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oram, M. W., and Benson, P. J. (1992). Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 335, 23–30.
Perrett, D. I., May, K. A., and Yoshikawa, S. (1994). Facial shape and judgments of female attractiveness. Nature, 368, 239–42.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., . . . Akamatsu, S.(1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–87.
Peskin, M., and Newell, F. N. (2004). Familiarity breeds attraction: Effects of exposure on the attractiveness of typical and distinctive faces. Perception, 33, 147–57.
Pizzagalli, D. A., Lehmann, D., Hendrick, A. M., Regard, M., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2002). Affective judgments of faces modulate early activity (not similar to 160 ms) within the fusiform gyri. Neuroimage, 16, 663–77.
Potter, T., Corneille, O., Ruys, K. I., and Rhodes, G. (2007). “Just another pretty face”: A multidimensional scaling approach to face attractiveness and variability. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 368–72.
Principe, C. P., and Langlois, J. H. (2011). Faces differing in attractiveness elicit corresponding affective responses. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 140–48.
Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., and McCarthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2,188–99.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., and Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience?Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–82.
Reis, H. T., Wilson, I. M., Monestere, C., Bernstein, S., Clark, K., Seidl, E., . . . Radoane, K. (1990). What is smiling is beautiful and good. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 259–67.
Rellecke, J., Bakirtas, A. M., Sommer, W., and Schacht, A. (2011). Automaticity in attractive face processing: Brain potentials from a dual task. Neuroreport, 22, 706–10.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199–226.
Rhodes, G., and Tremewan, T. (1996). Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness. Psychological Science, 7, 105–10.
Rhodes, G., Proffitt, F., Grady, J. M., and Sumich, A. (1998). Facial symmetry and the perception of beauty. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 659–69.
Rhodes, G., Sumich, A., and Byatt, G. (1999). Are average facial configurations attractive only because of their symmetry?Psychological Science, 10, 52–58.
Rhodes, G., Hickford, C., and Jeffery, L. (2000). Sex-typicality and attractiveness: Are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 125–40.
Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L. A., Clark, A., Kalick, S. M., Hightower, A., and McKay, R. (2001). Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health?Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 31–46.
Ro, T., Russell, C., and Lavie, N. (2001). Changing faces: A detection advantage in the flicker paradigm. Psychological Science, 12, 94–99.
Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Gosling, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Carter, V., Jones, B. C., . . . Petrie, M. (2005). MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 213–26.
Roisson, B., and Jaques, C. (2012). The N170: Understanding the time course of face perception in the human brain. In S. J. Luck and E. S. Kappenman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of event related potential components (pp. 115–41). New York: Oxford University Press.
Roye, A., Höfel, L., and Jacobsen, T. (2008). Aesthetics of faces: Behavioural and electro-physiological indices of evaluative and descriptive judgment processes. Journal of Psychophysiology, 22, 41–57.
Rubenstein, A. J., Kalakanis, L., and Langlois, J. H. (1999). Infant preferences for attractive faces: A cognitive explanation. Developmental Psychology, 35, 848–55.
Said, C. P., and Todorov, A. (2011). A statistical model of facial attractiveness. Psychological Science, 22, 1,183–90.
Samson, N., Fink, B., and Matts, P. J. (2010). Visible skin condition and perception of human facial appearance. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 32, 167–84.
Sarno, J. A., and Alley, T. R. (1997). Attractiveness and the memorability of faces: Only a matter of distinctiveness?American Journal of Psychology, 110, 81–92.
Schacht, A., Werheid, K., and Sommer, W. (2008). The appraisal of facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 132–42.
Senior, C. (2003). Beauty in the brain of the beholder. Neuron, 38, 525–28.
Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., and Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1,317–22.
Slater, A., von der Schulenberg, C., Brown, E., Badenoch, M., Butterworth, G., Parson, S., and Samuels, C. (1998). Newborn infants prefer attractive faces. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 345–54.
Smith, D. V., Hayden, B. Y., Truong, T.-K., Song, A. W., Platt, M. L., and Huettel, S. A. (2010). Distinct value signals in anterior and posterior ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 2,490–95.
Sui, J., and Liu, C. H. (2009). Can beauty be ignored? Effects of facial attractiveness on covert attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 276–81.
Swaddle, J. P., and Reierson, G. W. (2002). Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 269, 2,285–89.
Tatarunaite, E., Playle, R., Hood, K., Shaw, W., and Richmond, S. (2005). Facial attractiveness: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics, 127, 676–82.
Thornhill, R., and Moller, A. P. (1997). Developmental stability, disease and medicine. Biological Reviews, 72, 497–548.
Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 452–60.
Tinio, P. P. L., and Leder, H. (2009). Just how stable are stable aesthetic features? Symmetry, complexity, and the jaws of massive familiarization. Acta Psychologica, 130, 241–50.
Tracy, J. L., and Beall, A. T. (2011). Happy guys finish last: The impact of emotion expressions on sexual attraction. Emotion, 111, 1,379–87.
Tsukiura, T., and Cabeza, R. (2011a). Remembering beauty: Roles of orbitofrontal and hippocampal regions in successful memory encoding of attractive faces. Neuro-image, 54, 653–60.
Tsukiura, T., and Cabeza, R. (2011b). Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral judgments: Implications for the Beauty-is-Good stereotype. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 138–48.
Valentine, T., and Bruce, V. (1986). The effects of distinctiveness in recognizing and classifying faces. Perception, 15, 525–35.
van Dongen, S., and Gangestad, S. W. (2011). Human fluctuating asymmetry in relation to health and quality: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 380–98.
van Hooff, J. C., Crawford, H., and van Vugt, M. (2011). The wandering mind of men: ERP evidence for gender differences in attention bias towards attractive opposite sex faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 477–85.
van Straaten, I., Holland, R. W., Finkenauer, C., Hollenstein, T., and Engels, R. (2010). Gazing behavior during mixed-sex interactions: Sex and attractiveness effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1,055–62.
van Valen, L. (1962). A study of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution, 16, 125–42.
Vuilleumier, P., and Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: Windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 837–55.
Weinberg, A., and Hajcak, G. (2010). Beyond good and evil: The time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. Emotion, 10, 767–82.
Werheid, K., Schacht, A., and Sommer, W. (2007). Facial attractiveness modulates early and late event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 76, 100–8.
Wickham, L. H. V., and Morris, P. E. (2003). Attractiveness, distinctiveness, and recognition of faces: Attractive faces can be typical or distinctive but are not better recognized. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 455–68.
Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., and Catty, S. (2006). Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological Science, 17, 799–806.
Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., and Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195–206.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – Selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–14.
Zhang, Y., Kong, F., Chen, H., Jackson, T., Han, L., Meng, J., . . . ul Hasan, A. N. (2011). Identifying cognitive preferences for attractive female faces: An event-related potential experiment using a study-test paradigm. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 89, 1,887–93.
Zietsch, B. P., Verweij, K. J. H., Heath, A. C., and Martin, N. G. (2011). Variation in human mate choice: Simultaneously investigating heritability, parental influence, sexual imprinting, and assortative mating. American Naturalist, 177, 605–16.

References

Alter, R. (1986/1996). The pleasures of reading in an ideological age. New York: Norton.
Anderson, R. C. (1985). Becoming a nation of readers: The report of the commission on reading. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Education.
Basbanes, N. A. (2003). A splendor of letters: The permanence of books in an impermanent world. New York: HarperCollins.
Basbanes, N. A. (2005). Every book its reader: The power of the printed word to stir the world. New York: HarperCollins.
Bateson, M. C. (1989). Composing a life. New York: Atlantic Monthly.
Begley, L. (2008). Franz Kafka: The tremendous world I have within my head. New York: Atlas and Co.
Benjamin, W. (1969). The storyteller. In H. Arendt (Ed.), Illuminations: Essays and reflections (pp. 83–109). New York: Schocken.
Birkerts, S. (1994). The Gutenberg elegies: The fate of reading in an electronic age. Boston, MA: Faber and Faber.
Birkerts, S. (1999). States of reading. In Readings (pp. 101–10). St. Paul, MN: Graywolf.
Birkerts, S. (2007). Reading life: Books for the ages. St. Paul, MN: Graywolf.
Booker, C. (2004). The seven basic plots. New York: Continuum.
Boxall, P. (Ed.) (2006). 1001 books you must read before you die. New York: Universe.
Brown, K. (Ed.) (1993). The true subject: Writers on life and craft. St. Paul, MN: Graywolf.
Bruner, J. S. (2002). Making stories: Law, literature, life. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.